Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-pfhbr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-08T18:07:51.323Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Developmental Dyslexia in Czech and Slovak

from Part I - Developmental Dyslexia across Languages and Writing Systems

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 September 2019

Ludo Verhoeven
Affiliation:
Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen
Charles Perfetti
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Kenneth Pugh
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Two small, closely related countries in Central Europe, the Czech Republic and Slovakia, have enjoyed a recent surge in research activities, at the national and international levels, in the areas of typical and atypical literacy development. Many new and exciting findings are informing developments in diagnosis, assessment, and educational policy regarding the prevention and remediation of literacy problems due to dyslexia. In the present chapter, we review much of this recent work. Despite their historical, geopolitical, cultural, and linguistic connections – having existed as a single country for seventy-five years (1918–1993) – the Czech and Slovak countries and their languages are nevertheless distinct. We therefore take a comparative approach throughout the chapter, contrasting the findings in these languages with each other as well as with findings from English and other languages.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Borkowski, J., & Muthukrishna, N. (1992). Moving metacognition into the classroom: “Working models” and effective strategy teaching. In Pressley, M., Harris, K. R., & Guthrie, J. T. (Eds.), Promoting academic competence and literacy in school (pp. 477501). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bosse, M.-L., Tainturier, M.-J., & Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104, 198230.Google Scholar
Bruck, M., & Waters, G. (1988). An analysis of the spelling errors of children who differ in their reading and spelling skills. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 7792.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M. (2017). Learning to read Czech and Slovak. In Verhoeven, L. & Perfetti, C. (Eds.), Learning to read across languages and writing systems (pp. 371392). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Defior, S., Seidlová Málková, G., & Hulme, C. (2013). Different patterns, but equivalent predictors, of growth in reading in consistent and inconsistent orthographies. Psychological Science, 24, 13981407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., & Mousikou, P. et al. (2012). Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies. Psychological Science, 23, 678686.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., & Mikulajová, M. (2008). Effects of letter-sound consistency, letter-form complexity, and frequency in learning canonical and contextually conditioned letter spellings in Slovak. Phonetica Pragensia, 11, 2130.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., Mikulajová, M., & Vencelová, L. (2008a). Spelling of Slovak children with dyslexia. Paper presented at the International Conference of the British Dyslexia Association, Harrogate, UK.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., Mikulajová, M., & Vencelová, L. (2008b). Súbor testov na hodnotenie pravopisných schopností pre škoslkú a klinickú prax [Battery of tests for the assessment of spelling skills for school and clinical practice]. Bratislava, Slovakia: Slovenská asociácia logopédov.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., & Samara, A., (2015). Learning to read and spell words in different writing systems. In Pollatsek, A. & Treiman, R. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of reading. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199324576.013.21.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., & Volín, J. (2001). Phonological spelling errors among dyslexic children learning a transparent orthography: The case of Czech. Dyslexia, 7, 229245.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., & Volín, J. (2005). Baterie diagnostických testů gramotnostních dovedností pro žáky 2. až 5. ročníků ZŠ [Battery of diagnostic tests of literacy skills for pupils in 2nd to 5th grade]. Prague: IPPP ČR.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., Volín, J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 107139.Google Scholar
Cimlerová, P., Pokorná, D., & Chalupová, E. (2007). Diagnostika specifických poruch učení u adolescentů a dospělých osob [Diagnosis of specific reading disorders in adolescents and adults].Prague: IPPP ČR.Google Scholar
Edfeldt, W. A. (1968). Reverzní test [Reversals test]. Bratislava: Psychodiagnostika.Google Scholar
Frith, U. (1980). Unexpected spelling problems. In Frith, U. (Ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Frostig, M. (1972). Vývojový test zrakového vnímání [Developmental test of visual perception]. Bratislava: Psychodiagnostika.Google Scholar
Froyen, D., Willems, G., & Blomert, L. (2010). Evidence for a specific cross-modal association deficit in dyslexia: An electrophysiological study of letter-speech sound processing. Developmental Science, 14, 635648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2011). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming predicting early development in reading and spelling: Results from a cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 8595.Google Scholar
Gebhardtová, D. (1994). Výsledky výzkumu dětí se specifickými vývojovými poruchami v oblasti čtení a psaní [Results of research on children with specific developmental disorders in reading and spelling]. In Kucharská, A., Slavíková, I., & Lepová, R. (Eds.), Specifické poruchy učení a chování [Specific disorders of learning and behavior]. Prague: IPPP ČR.Google Scholar
Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Liao, C. H. (2008). Rapid naming speed and reading across languages that vary in orthographic consistency. Reading and Writing, 21, 885903.Google Scholar
Goulandris, N. (2003). Dyslexia in different languages: Cross-linguistic comparisons. London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Hulme, C., Caravolas, M., Málková, G., & Brigstocke, S. (2005). Phoneme isolation ability is not simply a consequence of letter-sound knowledge. Cognition, 97, B1B11.Google Scholar
International Dyslexia Association. (2002). Definition of dyslexia. http://eida.org/definition-of-dyslexia/.Google Scholar
Kessler, B., & Caravolas, M. (2011). Weslalex: West Slavic lexicon of child-directed printed words. http://spell.psychology.wustl.edu/weslalex.Google Scholar
Kirby, J. R., Georgiou, G. K., Martinussen, R., & Parrila, R. (2010). Naming speed and reading: From prediction to instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 341362.Google Scholar
Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. K., & Pfeiffer, S. L. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 453464.Google Scholar
Košč, L. (1987). Patopsychológia učenia a jej neuropsychologické základy [Pathology of learning and its neuropsychological foundations]. Bratislava: SPN.Google Scholar
Kučera, H., & Monroe, G. K. (1968). A comparative, quantitative phonology of Russian, Czech, and German. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Kucharská, A. (2014). Rizika vzniku dyslexie [Risks of developing dyslexia]. Habilitační práce. Prague: PedF UK v Praze.Google Scholar
Kucharská, A., & Barešová, P. (2012). Vývojová dynamika čtení v analyticko-syntetické metodě čtení a metodě genetické v 1. a 2. třídě a její uplatnění v poradenské diagnostice [The dynamics of reading development in the analytic-synthetic versus the genetic methods in 1st and 2nd grade, and their validity for clinical diagnosis]. Pedagogika, 62(1–2), 6580.Google Scholar
Kucharská, A., Seidlová, M., Sotáková, H. et al. (2014). Porozumění čtenému I. Typický vývoj porozumění čtenému – východiska, témata, zdroje (kritická analýza) [Reading Comprehension I. Typical development of reading comprehension – perspectives, themes, tools (critical analysis)]. Prague, Czech Republic: Pedagogical Faculty – Charles University.Google Scholar
Landerl, K., Ramus, F., & Moll, K. et al. (2013). Predictors of developmental dyslexia in European orthographies with varying complexity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 686694.Google Scholar
Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315334.Google Scholar
Lervåg, A., & Hulme, C. (2009). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) taps a mechanism that places constraints on the development of early reading fluency. Psychological Science, 20, 10401048.Google Scholar
Matějček, Z. (1959). Klinický obraz dyslektického dieťaťa [Clinical profile of the dyslexic child]. Psychológia a patopsychológia dieťaťa, 1(2), 8386.Google Scholar
Matějček, Z. (1961). Školní selhání některých dětí normálního nadání [Scholastic failure of some children with normal abilities]. Československá pediatrie, 16(10), 877882.Google Scholar
Matějček, Z. (1978). Vývojové poruchy čtení [Developmental disorders of learning]. Prague: SPN.Google Scholar
Matějček, Z. (1995). Dyslexie: specifické poruchy čtení [Dyslexias: specific disorder of reading]. Jinočany, Czech Republic: H&H.Google Scholar
Mertin, V. (2008). Reakce na intervenci – nové kritérium při diagnostice specifických poruch učení [Response to intervention – New criteria in diagnosing specific disorders of learning]. Pražské sociálně vědní studie, Psychologická řada PSY-22. Prague: FSV UK a FF UK.Google Scholar
Míka, J. (1982). Orientační test dynamické praxe [Orientational test of dynamic practice]. Bratislava: Psychodiagnostika.Google Scholar
Mikulajová, M. (2010). Metódy diagnostiky dyslexie [Methods of diagnosing dyslexia]. CD nosič. Bratislava: Mabag.Google Scholar
Mikulajová, M., Nováková Schöffelová, M., Tokárová, O., & Dostálová, A. (2016). Trénink jazykových schopností podle D. B. Elkonina [Training of language abilities according to D. B. Elkonin]. 2nd ed. Prague: Centrum Rozum, V.O.S.Google Scholar
Mikulajová, M., Tokárová, O., & Sümegiová, Z. (2014). Tréning fonematického uvedomovania podľa D. B. Eľkonina [Training of phoneme awareness according to D.B. Elkonin]. 2nd ed. Bratislava: Dialóg, spol. s r.o.Google Scholar
Mikulajová, M., Váryová, B., Vencelová, L., Caravolas, M., & Škrabáková, G. (2012). Čítanie, písanie a dyslexia (s testami a normami) [Reading, writing and dyslexia (with tests and norms]. Bratislava: Slovenská asociácia logopédov.Google Scholar
Mikulajová, M. & Velecká, A. (2012). Profil slovenských dětí s „dyslexií” z pohledu jejich čtenářských schopností. Pedagogika, 62(12), 137149.Google Scholar
Mladá, P. (1996). Zbývá jen málo – chtít pochopit, aneb dyslexie na 2. stupni ZŠ. [All that remains is to want to understand, or, dyslexia in the second grade of elementary school]. In Kucharská, A. (Ed.), Specifické poruchy učení a chování 1996 (pp. 6164.). Prague: Portál.Google Scholar
Moll, K., & Landerl, K. (2009). Double dissociation between reading and spelling deficits. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 359382.Google Scholar
Moll, K., Loff, A., & Snowling, M. (2013). Cognitive endophenotypes of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 385397. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2012.736439.Google Scholar
Moll, K., Ramus, F., & Bartling, et al. (2014). Cognitive mechanisms underlying reading and spelling development in five European orthographies. Learning and Instruction, 29, 6577. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.09.003.Google Scholar
Moll, K., Thompson, P. A., & Mikulajová, M. et al. (2016). Precursors of reading difficulties in Czech and Slovak children at-risk of dyslexia. Dyslexia, 22, 120136. doi: http://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1002/dys.1526.Google Scholar
Pardel, T. (1966). Písaná reč, jej vývin a poruchy u detí [Written language, its development and disorders]. Bratislava: SPN.Google Scholar
Pennington, B. F. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition, 101, 385413.Google Scholar
Pennington, B. F., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Development, 72, 816833. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00317 .Google Scholar
Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 265282.Google Scholar
Schöffelová, M., & Mikulajová, M. (2012). Vývoj různých aspektů čtení ve slovenštině [The development of various aspects of reading in Slovak]. Pedagogika, 1–2, 111125.Google Scholar
Seidlová Málková, G., & Caravolas, M. (2013). Baterie testů fonologických schopností, BTFS [Battery of tests of phonological abilities]. Prague: Národní ústav pro vzdělávání.Google Scholar
Share, D. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584615.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74, 358373. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.7402003.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2012). Annual research review: The nature and classification of reading disorders – A commentary on proposals for DSM-5. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 593607.Google Scholar
Sokolová, M., Musilová, K., & Slančová, D. (2005). Slovenčina a čeština (Synchrónne porovnanie s cvičeniami) [Slovak and Czech (Synchronic comparison with exercises)]. Bratislava: FF UK.Google Scholar
Švancarová, D., & Kucharská, A. (2001). Test rizika poruch čtení a psaní pro rané školáky [Test of risk of disorders in reading and writing for young school children].Prague: Scientia.Google Scholar
Tiefenbacherová, B. (2014). Možnosti stimulácie morfo-syntaktických schopností v predškolskom veku [Approaches to the stimulation of morphosyntactic abilities in the preschool years]. Master’s thesis. Bratislava: Pedagogical Faculty, Comenius University.Google Scholar
Torppa, M., Lyytinen, P., Erskine, J., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2010). Language development, literacy skills, and predictive connections to reading in Finnish children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 308321. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219410369096.Google Scholar
Torppa, M., Poikkeus, A.-M., Laakso, M.-L., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2006). Predicting delayed letter knowledge development and its relation to grade 1 reading achievement among children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 42, 11281142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Váryová, B. (2012). Skúšky čítania pre mladší školský vek [Tests of reading for the younger school years]. In Mikulajová, M., Váryová, B., Vencelová, L., Caravolas, M., & Škrabáková, G. (Eds.). Čítanie, písanie a dyslexia (s testami a normami) [Reading, writing and dyslexia (with tests and norms)] (pp. 126159). Bratislava: Slovenská asociácia logopédov.Google Scholar
Vencelová, L., & Kapalková, S. (2014). Východiská tvorby novej inovatívnej metodiky výučby slovenského jazyka v 1. ročníku základnej školy [The scope of the new innovative method in Slovak language arts curriculum for grade 1]. Logopaedica XVII, 105117.Google Scholar
Vencelová, L., & Mikulajová, M. (2010). Čítanie a pravopis po slovensky hovoriacich dyslektických detí – porovnávacia štúdia [Reading and spelling of Slovak-speaking children with dyslexia – Comparative study]. Psychológia a patopsychológia dieťaťa, 45 (3), 215228.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 133.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (2000). The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 668680.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×