Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-2lccl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T09:42:28.329Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

21 - The cognitive neuroscience of agency in schizophrenia

from Part III - Disturbances of the self: the case of schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2009

Henrik Walter
Affiliation:
Universitätsklinik für Psychiatrie, ULM, Germany
Manfred Spitzer
Affiliation:
Universitätsklinik für Psychiatrie, ULM, Germany
Tilo Kircher
Affiliation:
Eberhard-Karls-Universität Tübingen, Germany
Anthony David
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, London
Get access

Summary

Abstract

Agency is the sense of ownership, i.e. the personal experience of being the originator of one's thoughts and actions (Walter, 2001). Disorders of agency are prominent in schizophrenia and may also be part of several other neuropsychiatric disorders and syndromes, e.g. alien-hand syndrome, drug-induced psychoses or anosognosia. In recent years, cognitive neuroscience has made considerable progress in understanding the neural basis of agency. In this chapter, current neurocognitive theories of agency are reviewed which are based on the assumption that an internal monitoring deficit lies at the core of disorders of agency in schizophrenia. It is demonstrated how they fail to explain several features of disturbed agency. A complementary theory is proposed which takes into account the experiences of reference and insertion of personal relatedness as well as the acknowledged role of dopamine for schizophrenia and its role as a neuromodulator regulating signal-to-noise ratio.

The psychopathology of agency in schizophrenia

Schizophrenic patients often report the immediate experience of someone else controlling their thoughts and actions. In addition, sometimes they feel they are in control of external events or are convinced they know what other people think. They think that things or events are related to themselves in a special way, have a personal significance or are made especially for them. The German psychiatrist Kurt Schneider emphasized these types of ideation as criteria for the diagnosis of schizophrenia, and because of their relative homogeneity and recognizability, they are frequently referred to as Schneiderian first-rank symptoms.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Angyal, A. (1936). The experience of the body-self in schizophrenia. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 35, 1029–53Google Scholar
Bechara, A. (1999). Emotionen und Entscheidungsfindung nach Frontalhirnläsionen: Ansätze zum Verständnis bestimmter psychiatrischer Erkrankungen. Nervenheilkunde, 18, 54–9Google Scholar
Blakemore, S. J., Frith, C. D. & Wolpert, D. M. (1999). The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action. Neuroimage, 9, S445Google Scholar
Brown, H. D. & Kosslyn, S. M. (1993). Cerebral lateralization. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 3, 183–6Google Scholar
Cutting, J. (1989). Body image disorders: comparision between unilateral hemisphere damage and schizophrenia. Behavioral Neurology, 2, 201–10Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes's Error. New York: Putnam's
Daprati, E., Franck, N., Georgieff, N. et al. (1997). Looking for the agent: an investigation into consciousness of action and self-consciousness in schizophrenic patients. Cognition, 65, 71–86Google Scholar
Decety, J., Grezes, J. & Costes, N. (1997). Brain activity during observation of action. Influence of action content and subject's strategy. Brain, 120, 1763–77Google Scholar
Feinberg, T. E. & Roane, D. M. (1997). Misidentification syndromes. In Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology, ed. T. E. Feinberg & M. J. Farah, pp. 391–7. New York: McGraw Hill
Fink, G. R., Markowitsch, H. J., Reinkemeier, M. et al. (1996). Cerebral representation of one's own past: neural networks involved in autobiographical memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 4275–82Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. & Done, D. J. (1988). Towards a neuropsychology of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 437–43Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. & Done, D. J. (1989). Experiences of alien control in schizophrenia reflect a disorder in the central monitoring of action. Psychological Medicine, 19, 359–63Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (1992). The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Frith, C. D. (1996). The role of the prefrontal cortex in self-consciousness: the case of auditory hallucinations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 351, 1505–12Google Scholar
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. et al. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609Google Scholar
Gasquoine, P. G. Alien hand sign. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 663–7
Georgieff, N. & Jeannerod, M. (1998). Beyond consciousness of external reality: a ‘who’ system for consciousness of action and self-consciousness. Conscious Cognition, 7, 465–77Google Scholar
Goel, V., Grafman, J., Sadato, N. et al. (1995). Modeling other minds. NeuroReport, 6, 1741–6Google Scholar
Greig, T. C., Bell, M. D. & Bryson, G. J. (1999). ‘Theory of mind’ impairment in schizophrenia. Cognitive and symptom correlates. Schizophrenia Research, 36, 168Google Scholar
Henik, A., Priel, B. & Umansky, R. (1992). Attention and automaticity in semantic processing of schizophrenic patients. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurology, 5, 161–9Google Scholar
Keenan, J. P., Wheeler, M. A., Gallup, G. G. Jr. et al. (2000). Self-recognition and the right prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 338–44Google Scholar
Kiefer, M., Weisbrod, M., Kern, I. et al. (1998). Right hemisphere activation during indirect semantic priming: evidence from event-related potentials. Brain and Language, 64, 377–408Google Scholar
Kischka, U., Kammer, T., Maier, S. et al. (1996). Dopaminergic modulation of semantic network activation. Neuropsychologia, 34, 1107–13Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M., Chabris, C. F., Marsolek, C. J. et al. (1992). Categorical versus coordinate spatial relations: computation analyses and computer simulations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 562–77Google Scholar
Mlakar, J., Jensterle, J. & Frith, C. D. (1994). Central monitoring deficiency and schizophrenic symptoms. Psychological Medicine, 24, 557–64Google Scholar
Moselhy, H., & Oyebode, F. (1997). Delusional misidentification syndromes: a review of the anglophone literature. Neurology and Psychiatry and Brain Research, 5, 21–6Google Scholar
Nasrallah, H. A. (1985). The unintegrated right cerebral hemispheric consciousness as alien intruder: a possible mechanism for Schneiderian delusions in schizophrenia. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 26, 273–82Google Scholar
Nowak, G. (1989). Lateralization of neocortical dopamine receptors and dopamine level in normal Wistar rats. Polish Journal of Pharmacology and Pharmacy, 41, 133–7Google Scholar
Oepen, G., Harrington, A., Spitzer, M. et al. (1988). ‘Feelings’ of conviction. On the relation of affect and thought disorder. In Psychopathology and Philosophy, ed. M. Spitzer., F. A. Uehlein & G. Oepen, pp. 43–55. Berlin: Spinger
Ramachandran, V. S. & Blakeslee, S. (1998). Phantoms in the Brain. New York: Morrow
Spence, S. A., Brooks, D. J., Hirsch, S. R. et al. (1997). A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). Brain, 120, 1997–2011Google Scholar
Spitzer, M. (1990). On defining delusions. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 31, 377–97Google Scholar
Spitzer, M. (1995). A neurocomputational approach to delusions. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 36, 83–105Google Scholar
Spitzer, M. (1997). A cognitive neuroscience of thought disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23, 29–50Google Scholar
Spitzer, M. & Casas, B. (1997). Project for a scientific psychopathology. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 10, 395–401Google Scholar
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 640–56Google Scholar
Walter, H. (1998). Emergence and the cognitive neuroscience of psychiatry. Zeitschrift für Naturforschung, 53c, 723–37Google Scholar
Walter, H. (2001). Neurophilosophy of Free Will. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×