Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-m9kch Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T17:25:07.727Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - The neural nature of the core SELF: implications for understanding schizophrenia

from Part II - Cognitive and neurosciences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2009

Jaak Panksepp
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Bowling Green State University, Bowling Green, OH, USA
Tilo Kircher
Affiliation:
Eberhard-Karls-Universität Tübingen, Germany
Anthony David
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, London
Get access

Summary

Abstract

Schizophrenia is largely an organic disease that impacts many brain systems, especially those mediating cognition–emotion interactions. The present analysis is premised on the existence of a variety of basic emotional operating systems of the brain – birthrights that allow all newborn mammals to begin navigating the complexities of the world and to learn about the values and reward-related contingencies of the environment. Some of the basic emotional systems have now been provisionally characterized, and they help coordinate behavioural, physiological and psychological aspects of emotionality, including the valenced affective feeling states that provide internally experienced values for the guidance of behaviour. Converging lines of evidence suggest that emotional feelings emerge from the interaction of these systems with longitudinally organized brain process for self-representation that is concentrated in the medial strata of the brain. These include anterior cingulate, insular and frontal cortices, which are richly connected to various medial diencephalic and mesencephalic structures, especially the periaqueductal grey (PAG). This basic neural substrate for self-representation appears to be grounded in stable motor coordinates that generate emotion-specific intentions in action, yielding a variety of feeling states that help construct mood-congruent cognitive structures. These systems generate a sense of causality from correlated environmental events and hence promote the emergence of both adaptive and delusional cognitive states. Certain symptoms of schizophrenia may reflect the uncoupling of the higher cognitive and the lower affective processes, disrupting normal modes of emotion regulation and reality testing.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andreasen, N., Nasrallah, H., Dunn, V. et al. (1986). Structural abnormalities in the frontal system in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 136–44Google Scholar
Bailey, P. & Davis, E. W. (1942). Effects of lesions of the periaqueductal gray matter in the cat. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 351, 305–6Google Scholar
Bailey, P. & Davis, E. W. (1943). Effects of lesions of the periaqueductal gray matter on the Macaca mulatta. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, 3, 69–72Google Scholar
Bekoff, M. (ed.) (2000). The Smile of a Dolphin. New York: Discovery Books
Benes, F. M. (2000). Emerging principles of altered neuronal circuitry in schizophrenia. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 252–69Google Scholar
Benes, F. M., McSparren, J., Bird, E. D., SanGiovanni, J. P. & Vincent, S. L. (1991). Deficits in small interneurons in prefrontal and cingulate cortices of schizophrenic and schizoaffective patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 996–1001Google Scholar
Bogerts, B., Meertz, E. & Schonfeldt-Bausch, R. (1985). Basal ganglia and limbic system pathology in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 784–91Google Scholar
Borod, J. C. (2000). The Neuropsychology of Emotion. New York: Oxford University Press
Butler, A. B. & Hodos, W. (1996). Comparative Vertebrate Neuroanatomy: Evolution and Adaptation New York: Wiley-Liss
Cartwright, J. (2000). Evolution and Human Behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Chugani, H. T. (1996). Neuroimaging of developmental nonlinearity and developmental pathologies. In Developmental Neuroimaging: Mapping the Development of Brain and Behavior, ed. R. W. Thatcher, G. Reid Lyon, J. Rumsey & N. Krasnegor, pp. 187–95. San Diego: Academic Press
Crick, F. & Koch, C. (2000). The unconscious homunculus. Neuro-Psychoanalysis, 2, 3–10Google Scholar
Crow, T. J. (2000). Schizophrenia as the price that Homo sapiens pays for language: a resolution of the central paradox in the origin of the species. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 118–29Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace
Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A. et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1049–56Google Scholar
Douglass, A. B. (1996). Sleep abnormalities in major psychiatric illnesses: polysomnographic and clinical features. In Advances in Biological Psychiatry, vol. 2, ed. J. Panksepp, pp. 153–77. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press
Freeman, W. J. (1999). How the Brain Makes up Its Mind. London: Blackwell
Gallagher, S. & Shear, J. (eds) (1999). Models of the Self, Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic
Godfrey-Smith, P. (1996). Complexity and the Function of Mind in Nature. New York: Cambridge University Press
Gordon, N., Panksepp, J., Secor, A. et al. (1998). Peripherally administered kainic acid induced brain damage effects on social and non-social behaviors. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 24, 691Google Scholar
Grace, A. A. (2000). Gating of information flow within the limbic system and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 330–41Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D. (2000). Wild Minds: What Animals Really Think. New York: Henry Holt
Holstege, G., Bandler, R. & Saper, C. B. (eds) (1996) The Emotional Motor System. Progress in Brain Research, vol. 107. Amsterdam: Elsevier
Ikemoto, S. & Panksepp, J. (1999). The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: a unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 6–41Google Scholar
Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Bradley, M. M. et al. (1997). Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion. Neuropsychologia, 15, 1437–44Google Scholar
Libet, B. (1999). Do we have free will? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 47–57Google Scholar
Lipska, B. K. & Weinberger, D. R. (2000). To model a psychiatric disorder in animals: schizophrenia as a reality test. Neuropsychopharmacology, 23, 223–39Google Scholar
Maquet, P., Peters, J.-M., Aerts, J. et al. (1996). Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and dreaming. Nature, 383, 163–6Google Scholar
Mesulam, M.-M. (ed.) (2000). Principles of Behavior and Cognitive Neurology, 2nd edn. New York: Oxford University Press
Newman, J. (1997). Putting the puzzle together: towards a general theory of the neural correlates of consciousness, Journal of Consciousness Studies, 4, 47–66, 101–21Google Scholar
Nyberg, L., Habib, R., McIntosh, A. R. & Tulving, E. (2000). Reactivation of encoding-related brain activity during memory retrieval. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 97, 1120–24Google Scholar
Olney, J. W. & Farber, N. B. (1995). NMDA antagonists as neurotherapeutic drugs, psychotogens, neurotoxins, and research tools for studying schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 13, 335–45Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1982). Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 407–67Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998a). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotion. New York: Oxford University Press
Panksepp, J. (1998b). The periconscious substrates of consciousness: affective states and the evolutionary origins of the self. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5, 566–82Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2000a). Emotions as natural kinds within the mammalian brain. In: The Handbook of Emotions, 2nd edn, ed. M. Lewis and J. Haviland, pp. 137–56. New York: Guilford
Panksepp, J. (2000b). The neurodynamics of emotions: an evolutionary-neurodevelopmental view. In Emotion, Self-Organization, and Development, ed. M. D. Lewis & I. Granic, pp. 236–64. New York: Cambridge University Press
Panksepp, J. (2000c). Affective consciousness and the instinctual motor system: The neural sources of sadness and joy. In The Caldron of Consciousness: Motivation, Affect and Self-Organization. Advances in Consciousness Research, ed. R. Ellis & N. Newton, pp. 27–54. Amsterdam: John Benjamins
Panksepp, J. (2000d). The neuro-evolutionary cusp between emotions and cognitions: implications for understand consciousness and the emergence of a unified mind science. Consciousness and Emotion, 1, 17–56Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2000e). Emotional circuits of the mammalian brain: implications for biological psychiatry. In Biological Psychiatry, ed. E. E. Bittar, pp. 27–58. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press
Panksepp, J. (2000f). The cradle of consciousness: a periconscious emotional homunclus? Neuro-Psychoanalysis, 2, 24–32Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. & Panksepp, J. B. (2000). The seven sins of evolutionary psychology. Evolution and Cognition, 6, 108–31Google Scholar
Parvizi, J., Hoesen, G. W. & Damasio, A. (2000). Selective pathological changes of the periaqueductal gray in Alzheimer's disease. Annals of Neurology, 48, 344–53Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (1999). The Brain and Emotion. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press
Rossi, A., Stratta, P., Mancini, F. et al. (1994). Magnetic resonance imaging findings of amygdala-anterior hippocampus shrinkage in male patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 52, 43–53Google Scholar
Schiff, N. & Plum, F. (1999). The neurology of impaired consciousness: global disorders and implied models. Target article for Association for the Scientific Study of Consciousness Electronic Seminar. http://athena.english.vt.edu/cgi.bin/netforum/nic/a/1
Schiff, N. D. & Plum, F. (2000). The role of arousal and ‘gating’ systems in the neurology of impaired consciousness. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 438–452Google Scholar
Sedvall, G. & Terenius, L. (eds) (2000). Schizophrenia: pathophysiological mechanisms. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 106–404Google Scholar
Sewards, T. V. & Sewards, M. A. (2000). Visual awareness due to neuronal activities in subcortical structures: a proposal. Consciousness and Cognition, 9, 86–116Google Scholar
Stevens, A. & Price, J. (1996). Evolutionary Psychology: A New Beginning. London: Routledge
Strehler, B. L. (1991). Where is the self? A neuroanatomical theory of consciousness. Synapse, 7, 44–91Google Scholar
Swerdlow, N. R. (1996). Cortico-striate substrates of cognitive, motor, and sensory gating: speculation and implications for psychological function and dysfunction. In Advances in Biological Psychiatry, vol. 2, ed. J. Panksepp, pp. 179–207. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press
Thune, J. J. & Pakkenberg, B. (2000). Stereological studies of the schizophrenic brain. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 200–4Google Scholar
Toga, A. W. & Mazziotta, J. C. (eds.) (2000) Brain Mapping: The Systems. San Diego, CA: Academic Press
Tononi, G. & Edelman, G. M. (1998). Consciousness and complexity. Science, 282, 1846–51Google Scholar
Tononi, G. & Edelman, G. M. (2000). Schizophrenia and the mechanisms of conscious integration. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 391–400Google Scholar
Watt, D. G. (2000). The centrencephalon and thalamocortical integration: neglected contributions of periaqueductal gray. Consciousness and Emotion, 1: 91–114Google Scholar
Wheeler, M. E., Petersen, S. E. & Buckner, R. L. (2000). Memory's echo: vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 97, 11125-9Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×