Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T16:52:10.181Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory and mood induction studies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2011

Rapson Gomez
Affiliation:
Professor, Department of Psychology University of Ballarat, Australia
Andrew Cooper
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Wales Swansea, UK
Philip J. Corr
Affiliation:
University of Wales, Swansea
Get access

Summary

In relation to affect, Gray's (1970) original version of the Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST) suggests that positive emotion and negative emotion reflect independent activities of the Behavioural Approach System (BAS) and the Behavioural Inhibition System (BIS), respectively (Gray 1990). A recent revision of the RST (Corr 2001, 2004; Gray and McNaughton 2000) implies that the BAS will facilitate positive emotion and antagonize negative emotion, while the BIS will facilitate negative emotion and antagonize positive emotion. This chapter reviews the extent to which mood induction studies support the original and revised RST. Also covered are the limitations of these studies, and the implications of existing data for psychopathology. Suggestions for future research in this area are proposed.

Reinforcement sensitivity theory and mood states

In the initial RST (Gray 1970; see Corr, chapter 1), the BIS is hypothesized to be sensitive to signals of conditioned punishment, frustrative non-reward and novelty. It is involved in regulating aversive motivation, in that its activation will cause individuals to inhibit further responses to such cues. The BAS, by contrast, is sensitive to signals of conditioned reward and non-punishment. It is involved in regulating appetitive motivation as its activation increases goal directed responses. A third system, the Flight-Fight System, is hypothesized to regulate responses to unconditioned punishment and non-reward cues.

In the revised RST (Gray and McNaughton 2000), the BAS is linked to conditioned and unconditioned appetitive stimuli. Aversive stimuli that do not involve approach responses are mediated by a Fight-Flight-Freeze System.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (1994), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV (Washington, DC: APA)Google Scholar
Amin, Z., Constable, T. R. and Canli, T. (2004), Attentional bias for valenced stimuli as a function of personality in the dot–probe task, Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 15–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, A. T. (1976), Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders (Madison, CT: International Universities Press)Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Emery, G. and Greenberg, R. C. (1985), Anxiety Disorders and Phobias: A Cognitive Perspective (New York: Guilford Press)Google Scholar
Bendig, A. W. (1956), The development of a short form of the manifest anxiety scale, Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20, 384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, G. H. (1981), Mood and memory, American Psychologist, 36, 129–148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, B., Mogg, K., Galbraith, M. and Perrett, A. (1993), Negative recall bias and neuroticism: state vs trait effects, Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 125–127CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, M. M. and Lang, P. J. (1999a), Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Technical Manual and Affective Ratings (Gainsville, FL: Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida)Google Scholar
Bradley, M. M. and Lang, P. J.(1999b), International Affective Digitized Sounds: Technical Manual and Affective Ratings (Gainsville, FL: Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida)Google Scholar
Bradley, M. M. and Lang, P. J. (2000), Measuring emotion: behaviour, feeling and physiology in Lane, R. D. and Nadel, L. (eds), Cognitive Neuroscience of Emotion (New York: Oxford University Press), pp. 242–276Google Scholar
Brenner, E. (2000), Mood induction in children: methodological issues and clinical implications, Review of General Psychology, 4, 264–283CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caccioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L. and Berntson, G. G. (1999), The affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: form follows function, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 839–855CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canli, T., Amin, Z. and Haas, B. (2004), A double dissociation between mood states and personality traits in the anterior cingulate, Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 897–904CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J. and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001), An functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli, Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 33–42CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S. (2004), Negative affects deriving from the behavioral approach system, Emotion, 4, 3–22CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S. and White, T. L. (1994), Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the Behavioural Inhibition System/Behavioural Approach System scales, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention (1999), The International Affective Picture System (Gainsville, FL: University of Florida)Google Scholar
Clark, D. M. (1983), On the induction of depressed mood in the laboratory: evaluation and comparison of the Velten and musical procedures, Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 5, 27–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corr, P. J. (2001), Testing problems in J. A. Gray's personality theory: a commentary on Matthews and Gilliland (1999), Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 333–352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corr, P. J.(2002a), J. A. Gray's reinforcement sensitivity theory and frustrative nonreward: a theoretical note on expectancies in reactions to rewarding stimuli, Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1247–1253CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corr, P. J.(2002b), J. A. Gray's reinforcement sensitivity theory: tests of the joint subsystems hypothesis of anxiety and impulsivity, Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 511–532CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corr, P. J.(2004), Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality, Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Review, 28, 317–333CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, P. T. and McCrae, R. R. (1980), Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: happy and unhappy people, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668–678CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, P. T. and McCrae, R. R.(1992), Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI–R) and NEO Five–Factor Inventory (NEO–FFI): Professional Manual (Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources)Google Scholar
Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W. and Taylor, S. (2001), The effect of rumination as a mediator of elevated anxiety sensitivity in major depression, Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 525–535CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascalis, V., Arwari, B., Matteucci, M. and Mazzocco, A. (2005), Effects of emotional visual stimuli on auditory information processing: a test of J. A. Gray's reinforcement sensitivity theory, Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 163–176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Depue, R. A., Krauss, S. P. and Spoont, M. R. (1987), A two-dimensional threshold model of seasonal bipolar affective disorder in Magnusson, D. and Ohman, A. (eds.), Psychopathology: An Interactional Perspective (San Diego, CA: Academic Press Inc.), pp. 95–123Google Scholar
Derryberry, D. and Reed, M. A. (1994), Temperament and attention: orienting toward and away from positive and negative signals, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 1128–1139CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, E., Smith, H. and Fujita, F. (1995), The personality structure of affect, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 130–141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E. and Smith, H. L. (1999), Subjective well-being: three decades of research, Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276–302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1967), The Biological Basis of Personality (Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas)Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. and Eysenck, M. W. (1985), Personality and Individual Differences: A Natural Science Approach (New York: Plenum Press)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. and Eysenck, S. B. G. (1964), Manual of the Eysenck Personality Inventory (London: University of London Press)Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. and Eysenck, S. B. G.(1975), Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (London: Hodder and Stoughton)Google Scholar
Eysenck, S. B. G. and Eysenck, H. J. (1977), The place of impulsiveness in a dimensional system of personality description, British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 16, 57–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, S. B. G., Pearson, P. R., Easting, G. and Allsop, J. F. (1985), Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults, Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 613–619CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, L. S., Knight, B. G. and Zelinski, E. M. (1998), Mood induction with older adults: a tool for investigating effects of depressed mood, Psychology and Aging, 13, 519–523CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T. and Elliot, A. J. (2000), Behavioural activation and inhibition in everyday life, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1135–1149CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerrards-Hesse, A. and Spies, K. (1994), Experimental inductions of emotional states and their effectiveness: a review, British Journal of Psychology, 85, 55–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1999), A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five–factor models in Mervielde, I., Deary, I., Fruyt, F. and Ostendorf, F. (eds), Personality Psychology in Europe (Tilburg: Tilburg University Press), vol. 7, pp. 7–28Google Scholar
Gomez, R. (2003), Underlying processes in the poor response inhibition of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, Journal of Attention Disorders, 6, 111–122CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomez, R., Cooper, A. and Gomez, A. (2000), Susceptibility to positive and negative mood states: test of Eysenck's, Gray's and Newman's models, Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 351–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomez, R., Cooper, A., McOrmond, R. and Tatlow, S. (2004), Gray's reinforcement sensitivity theory: comparing the separable and joint subsystems hypotheses in the predictions of pleasant and unpleasant emotional information processing, Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 289–305CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomez, R. and Francis, L. (2003), Generalised anxiety disorder: relationships with Eysenck's, Gray's and Newman's theories, Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 3–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomez, R. and Gomez, A. (2002), Personality traits of the behavioural approach and inhibition systems: associations with processing of emotional stimuli, Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1299–1316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomez, R. and Gomez, A.(2005), Convergent, discriminant and concurrent validities of measures of the behavioural approach and behavioural inhibition systems: exploratory and confirmatory factor analytic approaches, Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 87–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomez, R., Gomez, A. and Cooper, A. (2002), Neuroticism and extraversion as predictors of negative and positive emotional information processing: comparing Eysenck's, Gray's and Newman's theories, European Journal of Personality, 16, 333–350CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomez, R. and McLaren, S. (1997), The effects of reward and punishment on response disinhibition, moods, heart rate and skin conductance levels during instrumental learning, Journal of Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 305–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, A. M. and Williams, M. J. (1982), Mood–induction research: its implications for clinical depression, Behaviour Research and Therapy, 20, 373–382CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, J. A. (1970), The psychophysiological basis of intraversion–extraversion, Behavior Research and Therapy, 8, 249–266CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A.(1990), Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition, Cognition and Emotion, 4, 269–288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1994), Framework for a taxonomy of psychiatric disorder in Goozen, S. H. M., Poll, N. E. and Sergeant, J. A. (eds), Emotions: Essays on Emotion Theory (New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum), pp. 29–59Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. and McNaughton, N. (2000), The Neuropsychology of Anxiety (2nd edn, Oxford: Oxford University Press)Google Scholar
Gray, J. R. and Braver, T. S. (2002), Personality predicts working–memory–related activation in the caudal anterior cingulate cortex, Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 64–75CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J. J., Sutton, S. K. and Ketelaar, T. (1998), Relations between affect and personality: support for the affect–level and affective reactivity views, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 279–288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmon-Jones, E. (2003), Anger and the behavioral approach system, Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 995–1005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmon-Jones, E. and Allen, J. J. B. (1997), Behavioral activation sensitivity and resting frontal EEG asymmetry: covariation of putative indicators related to risk for mood disorders, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 159–163CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmon-Jones, E. and Sigelman, J. (2001), State anger and prefrontal brain activity: evidence that insult–related relative left–prefrontal activation is associated with experienced anger and aggression, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 797–803CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helmers, K. F., Young, S. N. and Pihl, R. O. (1997), Extraversion and behavioral impulsivity, Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 441–452CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henriques, J. B., Glowacki, J. M. and Davidson, R. J. (1994), Reward fails to alter response bias in depression, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 460–466CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heponiemi, T., Keltikangas-Järvinen, L., Puttonen, S. and Ravaja, N. (2003), Behavioural Inhibition System/Behavioural Approach System sensitivity and self–rated affects during experimentally induced stress, Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 943–957CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S. L., Turner, J. R. and Iwata, N. (2003), Behavioural Inhibition System/Behavioural Approach System levels and psychiatric disorder: an epidemiological study, Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 25, 25–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Bradley, M. M., Lang, P. J., Ahern, G. L., Davidson, R. J. and Schwartz, G. E. (1997), Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion, Neuropsychologica, 35, 1437–1444CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P. J. (1980), Behavioural treatment and bio-behavioural assessment: computer applications in Sidowski, J. B., Johnson, J. H. and Williams, T. A. (eds), Technology in Mental Health Care Delivery Systems (Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing), pp. 119–137Google Scholar
Larsen, R. J. and Diener, E. (1992), Promises and problems with the circumplex model of emotion in Clark, M. S., Emotion (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications), pp. 25–59Google Scholar
Larsen, R. J. and Ketelaar, T. (1989), Extraversion, neuroticism, and susceptibility to positive and negative mood induction procedures, Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 1221–1228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larsen, R. J. and Ketelaar, T.(1991), Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132–140CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsen, R. J. and Sinnett, L. M. (1991), Meta-analysis of experimental manipulations: some factors affecting the Velten mood induction procedure, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 323–334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leen-Feldner, E. W., Zvolensky, M. J., Feldner, M. T. and Lejuez, C. W. (2004), Behavioral inhibition: relation to negative emotion regulation and reactivity, Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1235–1247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lischetzke, T. and Eid, M. (2006), Why extraverts are happier than introverts: the role of mood regulation, Journal of Personality, 74, 1127–1161CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucas, R. E. and Baird, B. M. (2004), Extraversion and emotional reactivity, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 473–485CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucas, R. E. and Fujita, F. (2000), Factors influencing the relation between extraversion and pleasant affect, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1039–1056CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, M. (1990), On the induction of mood, Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 669–697CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, B., Johnson, S. L. and Winters, R. (2001), Responsiveness to threat and incentive in bipolar disorder: relations of the Behavioural Inhibition System/Behavioural Approach System scales with symptoms, Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23, 133–143CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, W. M. and Reilly, N. P. (1987), Toward the self regulation of mood: theory and research, Motivation and Emotion, 11, 215–249CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrone-Strupinsky, J. V. and Depue, R. A. (2004), Differential relation of two distinct, film–induced positive emotional states to affiliative and agentic extraversion, Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1109–1126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrone, J. V., Depue, R. A., Scherer, A. J. and White, T. L. (2000), Film–induced incentive motivation and positive activation in relation to agentic and affiliative components of extraversion, Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 199–216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. P., Wallace, J. F., Schmitt, W. A. and Arnett, P. A. (1997), Behavioral inhibition system functioning in anxious, impulsive and psychopathic individuals, Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 583–592CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S. and Barratt, E. S. (1995), Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale, Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 768–7753.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ravaja, N. (2004), Effects of a small talking facial image on autonomic activity: the moderating influence of dispositional Behavioural Inhibition System and Behavioural Approach System sensitivities and emotions, Biological Psychology, 65, 163–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ravaja, N. and Kallinen, K. (2004), Emotional effects of startling background music during reading news reports: the moderating influence of dispositional Behavioural Inhibition System and Behavioural Approach System sensitivities, Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 45, 231–239CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reuter, M., Stark, R. and Hennig, J. (2004), Personality and emotion: test of Gray's personality theory by means of an functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging study, Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 462–469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revelle, W. and Anderson, K. J. (1994), Personality, Motivation and Cognition (Paper presented at the Conference on Human Cognitive Abilities, University of Virginia, Charlottesville)Google Scholar
Rogers, G. M. and Revelle, W. (1998), Personality, mood, and the evaluation of affective and neutral word pairs, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1592–1605CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1980), A circumplex model of affect, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rusting, C. L. (1998), Personality, mood and cognitive processing of emotional information: three conceptual frameworks, Psychological Bulletin, 124, 165–196CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rusting, C. L.(1999), Interactive effects of personality and mood on emotion–congruent memory and judgment, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1073–1086CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rusting, C. L. (2001), Personality as a moderator of affective influences on cognition in Forgas, J. P. (ed.), Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition (New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum), pp. 371–391Google Scholar
Rusting, C. L. and DeHart, T. (2000), Retrieving positive memories to regulate negative mood: consequences for mood–congruent memory, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 737–752CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rusting, C. L. and Larsen, R. J. (1997), Extraversion, neuroticism, and susceptibility to positive and negative affect: a test of two theoretical models, Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 607–612CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rusting, C. L. and Larsen, R. J.(1998), Personality and cognitive processing of affective information, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 200–213CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rusting, C. L. and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998), Regulating responses to anger: effects of rumination and distraction on angry mood, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 790–803CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M. and Petty, R. E. (1995), Personality moderators of mood congruency effects on cognition: the role of self-esteem and negative mood regulation, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1092–1107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smits, D. J. M. and Kuppens, P. (2005), The relations between anger, coping with anger, and aggression, and the Behavioural Inhibition System/Behavioural Approach System system, Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 783–793CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R. and Jacobs, G. A. (1983), Manual for the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y) (Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press)Google Scholar
Suls, J., Green, P. and Hillis, S. (1998), Emotional reactivity to everyday problems, affective inertia, and neuroticism, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 127–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Surakka, V., Tenhunen-Eskelinen, M., Hietanen, J. K., Jari, K. and Sams, M. (1998), Modulation of human auditory information processing by emotional visual stimuli, Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 159–163CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamir, M., Robinson, M. D. and Clore, G. L. (2002), The epistemic benefits of trait–consistent mood states: an analysis of extraversion and mood, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 663–677CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamir, M., Robinson, M. D. and Solberg, E. C. (2006), You may worry, but can you recognize threats when you see them? Neuroticism, threat identifications and negative affect, Journal of Personality, 74, 1481–1506CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teasdale, J. D. and Fogarty, S. J. (1979), Differential effects of induced mood on retrieval of pleasant and unpleasant events from episodic memory, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 248–257CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tellegen, A. (1985), Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety with an emphasis on self–report in Tuma, A. H. and Maser, J. D. (eds), Anxiety and the Anxiety Disorders (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), pp. 681–706Google Scholar
Tellegen, A. and Waller, N. G. (in press), Exploring Personality through Test Construction: Development of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press)
Torrubia, R., Ávila, C., Moltó, J. and Caseras, X. (2001), The sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray's anxiety and impulsivity dimensions, Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 837–862CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Updegraaf, J. A., Gable, S. L. and Taylor, S. E. (2004), What makes experiences satisfying? The interaction of approach–avoidance motivations and emotions in well-being, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 496–504CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wacker, J., Heldmann, M. and Stemmler, G. (2003), Separating emotion and motivational direction in fear and anger: effects on frontal asymmetry, Emotion, 3, 167–193CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., Clark, L. A. and Tellegen, A. (1988), Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J. and Tellegen, A. (1999), The two general activation systems of affect: structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 820–838CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G. and Hesse, F. W. (1996), Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: a meta-analysis, European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 557–5813.0.CO;2-4>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelenski, J. M. and Larsen, R. J. (1999), Susceptibility to affect: a comparison of three personality taxonomies, Journal of Personality, 67, 761–791CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×