Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-vpsfw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T05:51:55.450Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Animal cognition and human personality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2011

Neil McNaughton
Affiliation:
Professor, University of Otago
Philip J. Corr
Affiliation:
Professor, University of Wales Swansea
Philip J. Corr
Affiliation:
University of Wales, Swansea
Get access

Summary

Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST) is based, both in terms of data and concepts, on the experimental analysis of (non-human) animal behaviour. Some workers in emotion and human personality research question its validity on this count alone. But, since Galileo and Copernicus, our world has clearly not been the centre of the wider universe. Likewise, since Darwin, biology has accepted the essential continuity between our own and other species (with chimpanzees, genetically speaking, being 98 per cent human), where the human form and characters have not been the goal of evolution. In this chapter we evaluate the claim that emotion and personality, nonetheless, remain distinct from the rest of biology; that with them it is still the case that ‘the only proper study of mankind is man’. This will clarify the foundations on which RST rests.

Emotion and personality can be approached from more ‘cognitive’ (e.g., Matthews, chapter 17) or ‘biological’ (McNaughton and Corr, chapter 1) perspectives. But these are really different sides of the same coin. There are cases where cognitive or biological factors may seem relatively more important. But both kinds of case exist. This requires a true theoretical and empirical integration to take a combined ‘biocognitive’ perspective. Each approach fills the gaps left by the other. Their combination leads to a richer picture and a deeper understanding.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H. and Damasio, A. R. (1994), Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala, Nature, 372, 669–672CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H. and Damasio, A. R.(1995), Fear and the human amygdala, Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 5879–5891CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aggleton, J. P. (1993), The contribution of the amygdala to normal and abnormal emotional states, Trends in Neuroscience, 16, 328–333CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. and Tomasello, M. (1998), Chimpanzee and human cultures, Current Anthropology, 39, 591–604CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brady, J. V. (1975a), Toward a behavioural biology of emotion, in Levi, L. (ed.), Emotions: their Parameters and Measurement (New York: Raven Press), pp. 17–46Google Scholar
Brady, J. V. (1975b), Conditioning and emotion in Levi, L. (ed.), Emotions: their Parameters and Measurement (New York: Raven Press), pp. 309–340Google Scholar
Breiter, H. C., Etcoff, N. L., Whalen, P. J., Kennedy, W. A., Rauch, S. L., Buckner, R. L., Strauss, M. M., Hyman, S. E. and Rosen, B. R. (1996), Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression, Neuron, 17, 875–887CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, J. D., Randall, P., Scott, T. M., Bronen, R. A., Seibyl, J. P., Southwick, S. M., Delaney, R. C., McCarthy, G., Charney, D. S. and Innis, R. B. (1995), MRI-based measurement of hippocampal volume in patients with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder, American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 973–981Google ScholarPubMed
Broks, P., Young, A. W., Maratos, E. J., Coffey, P. J., Calder, A. J., Isaac, C. L., Mayes, A. R., Hodges, J. R., Montaldi, D., Cezayirli, E., Roberts, N. and Hadley, D. (1998), Face processing impairments after encephalitis:amygdala damage and recognition of fear, Neuropsychologia, 36, 59–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cahill, L., McGaugh, J. L. and Weinberger, N. M. (2001), The neurobiology of learning and memory: some reminders to remember, Trends in Neurosciences, 24, 578–581CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catania, K. C. (2002), The nose takes a starring role, Scientific American, 287, 38–43CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chagnon, N. A. (1977), Yanomamo: The Fierce People (London: Holt, Rinehart and Winston)Google Scholar
Corr, P. J. (2001), Testing problems in J. A. Gray's personality theory: a commentary on Matthews and Gilliland (1999), Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 333–352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corr, P. J.(2004), Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 317–332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872, reprinted in 1965), The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (Chicago, Illinois: University of Chicago Press)Google Scholar
Davis, M. (1992), The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety, Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 353–375CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, A. (1980), Contemporary Animal Learning Theory (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press)Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. (1992), Why gossip is good for you, New Scientist, 28–31Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M (in press) Co-evolution of neocortex size, group size and language in humans, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 681–735CrossRef
Eysenck, M. W. (1992), The nature of anxiety in Gale, A. and Eysenck, M. W. (eds.), Handbook of Individual Differences: Biological Perspectives (John Wiley and Sons), pp. 157–178Google Scholar
Eysenck, M. W. and Calvo, M. G. (1992), Anxiety and performance: the processing efficiency theory, Cognition and emotion, 6, 409–434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gewirtz, J. C. and Davis, M. (1997), Beyond attention: the role of amygdala NMDA receptors in fear conditioning, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 618CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A. (2004), Consciousness: Creeping Up on the Hard Problem (Oxford: Oxford University Press)Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. and McNaughton, N. (2000), The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: an Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo-hippocamoal System (2nd edn, Oxford: Oxford University Press)Google Scholar
Gray, J. R. and Braver, T. S. (2002), Personality predicts working-memory-related activation in the caudal anterior cingulate cortex, Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 64–75CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinde, R. A. (1966), Animal Behaviour (New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company)Google Scholar
James, W. (1884), What is an emotion?, Mind, 9, 188–205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimble, G. A. (1961), Hilgard and Marquis' Conditioning and Learning (New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts)Google Scholar
Kimura, D. (1992), Sex differences in the brain, Scientific American, 267, 118–125CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. E. (1993), Emotional memory: in search of systems and synapses, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 702, 149–157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. E.(1994), Emotion, memory and the brain, Scientific American, 270, 50–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeDoux, J.(1998), Fear and the brain: where have we been, and where are we going?, Biological Psychiatry, 44, 1229–1238CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, H. and Kim, J. J. (1998), Amygdalar NMDA receptors are critical for new fear learning in previously fear-conditioned rats, Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 8444–8454CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libet, B. (1985), Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 529–566CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luu, P., Tucker, D. M. and Derryberry, D. (1998), Anxiety and the motivational basis of working memory, Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22, 577–594CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKay, D. G. (1987), The Organization of Perception and Action: a Theory for Language and Other Cognitive Skills (New York: Springer-Verlag)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackintosh, N. J. (1974), The Psychology of Animal Learning (New York: Academic Press)Google Scholar
Macphail, E. M. (1996), Cognitive function in mammals: the evolutionary perspective, Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 279–290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macphail, E. M. and Bolhuis, J. J. (2001), The evolution of intelligence: adaptive specializations versus general process, Biological Reviews, 76, 341–364CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maguire, E. A., Frackowiak, R. S. J. and Frith, C. D. (1997), Recalling routes around London: activation of the right hippocampus in taxi drivers, Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 7103–7110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J. and Frith, C. D. (2000), Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97, 4398–4403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathews, A. (1993), Biases in processing emotional information, The Psychologist, 6, 493–499Google Scholar
Mathews, A. and MacLeod, C. (1994), Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders, Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 25–50CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, G. (1997), An introduction to the cognitive science of personality and emotion in Matthews, G. (ed.), Cognitive Science Perspectives on Personality and Emotion (Amsterdam: Elsevier), pp. 3–30Google Scholar
Matthews, G.(2000), A cognitive science critique of biological theories of personality traits, History and Philosophy of Psychology, 2, 1–17Google Scholar
Matthews, G., Derryberry, D. and Siegle, G. J. (2000), Personality and emotion: cognitive science perspectives in Hampson, S. E. (ed.), Advances in Personality Psychology (London: Routledge), pp. 199–237Google Scholar
Matthews, G. and Gilliland, K. (1999), The personality theories of H. J. Eysenck and J. A. Gray: a comparative review, Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 583–626CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNaughton, N. (1989), Biology and Emotion (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNaughton, N.(1997), Cognitive dysfunction resulting from hippocampal hyperactivity: a possible cause of anxiety disorder, Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 56, 603–611CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNaughton, N. (2002), Aminergic transmitter systems in D'haenen, H., Boer, J. A., Westenberg, H. and Willner, P. (eds.), Textbook of Biological Psychiatry (John Wiley and Sons), pp. 895–914CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, J. S., Friston, K. J., Büchel, C., Frith, C. D., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J. and Dolan, R. J. (1998), A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions, Brain, 121, 47–57CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., Rowland, D., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J. and Dolan, R. J. (1996), A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions, Nature, 383, 812–815CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, R. G. M., Garrud, P., Rawlins, J. N. P. and O'Keefe, J. (1982), Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions, Nature, 297, 681–683CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadel, L. and Jacobs, W. J. (1996), The role of the hippocampus in post-traumatic stress disorder, panic and phobia in Kato, N. (ed.), The Hippocampus: Functions and Clinical Relevance (Elsevier Science B. V.), pp. 455–463Google Scholar
Nauta, W. J. H. and Feirtag, M. (1986), Fundamental Neuroanatomy (New York: W. H. Freeman and Co.)Google Scholar
Nudo, R. J., Milliken, G. W., Jenkins, W. M. and Merzenich, M. M. (1996), Use-dependent alterations of movement representations in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys, Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 785–807CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogden, J. (1988), Language and memory functions after long recovery periods in left-hemispherectomised subjects, Neuropsychologia, 26, 645–659CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogden, J.(1989), Visuospatial and other ‘right-hemispheric’ functions after long recovery periods in left-hemispherectomised subjects, Neuropsychologia, 27, 765–776CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pandya, D. N. and Barnes, C. L. (1987), Architecture and connections of the frontal lobe in Perceman, E. (ed.), The Frontal Lobe Revisited (New York: IRBN Press), pp. 41–72Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1927), Conditioned Reflexes (London: Oxford University Press, G. V. Anrep (trans.))Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. and Lynn, S. K. (2000), Possible levels of animal consciousness with reference to grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus), American Zoologist, 40, 893–901Google Scholar
Rendell, L. and Whitehead, H. (2001), Culture in whales and dolphins, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 309–324CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, J. D. (2002), The neurobehavioral nature of fishes and the question of awareness and pain, Reviews in Fisheries Science, 10, 1–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smolensky, P. (1988), On the proper treatment of connectionism, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 1–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smuts, B. (2004), Orangutan technology, Scientific American, 291, 112–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tebbich, S., Taborsky, M., Fessl, B. and Blomqvist, D. (2001), Do woodpecker finches acquire tool-use by social learning?, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London.B:Biological Sciences, 268, 2189–2193CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Towe, A. L. and Luschei, E. S. (1981), Preface in Towe, A. L. and Luschei, E. S. (eds.), Motor Coordination (New York: Plenum Press), pp. vii–viiiCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velmans, M. (1991), Is human information processing conscious?, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 651–726CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A. (2001), Imitation and cultural transmission in apes and cetaceans, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 359–360CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W.C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G., Wrangham, R. W. and Boesch, C. (1999), Cultures in chimpanzees, Nature, 399, 682–685CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×