Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 11
  • Print publication year: 2006
  • Online publication date: September 2009

5 - Genetic and Environmental Influences on Continuity and Change in Reading Achievement in the Colorado Adoption Project

    • By Sally J. Wadsworth, Senior Research Associate at the Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado at Boulder, Robin Corley, Senior Research Associate at the Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado at Boulder, Robert Plomin, Professor and Deputy Director in Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry, Research Centre Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, John K. Hewitt, Professor and Director of the Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado at Boulder, John C. DeFries, Professor at the Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado at Boulder
  • Edited by Aletha C. Huston, University of Texas, Austin, Marika N. Ripke, University of Hawaii, Manoa
  • Publisher: Cambridge University Press
  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511499760.006
  • pp 87-106

Summary

Mastery of reading skills in childhood has far-reaching implications for development. Serving as a foundation for all academic learning, reading performance affects not only educational attainment, but also employment options, interpersonal relationships, and emotional well-being. Although this essential skill continues to develop throughout adolescence and adulthood, measures of reading achievement are highly stable, with correlations ranging from .23 to .96 over intervals of 1 to 8 years (e.g., Bast & Reitsma, 1998; DeFries, 1988; DeFries & Baker, 1983; Shaywitz, Escobar, Shaywitz, Fletcher, & Makuch, 1992; Wadsworth, Corley, Hewitt, & DeFries, 2001; Wagner et al., 1997; Williams & McGee, 1996). Results obtained from both twin and adoption studies have provided compelling evidence for substantial genetic influences on individual differences in reading performance from age 7 to adulthood, with heritability estimates ranging from .18 to .81 (e.g., Alarcón & DeFries, 1995; Cardon, DiLalla, Plomin, DeFries, & Fulker, 1990; Knopik & DeFries, 1999; Stevenson, Graham, Fredman, & McLoughlin, 1987; Wadsworth et al., 2001). Despite the striking evidence for stability and heritability, reading achievement also shows some change as a function of environmental influences. All of these factors – genes and environment, continuity and change – are needed for a complete understanding of the development of reading achievement.

The evidence that individual differences in reading achievement are both stable and heritable throughout development suggests that genetic influences contribute to continuity. Genetic influences that are important at an early age may also be important at later ages.

References
Alarcón, M., & DeFries, J. C. (1995). Quantitative trait locus for reading disability: An alternative test [Abstract]. Behavior Genetics, 25, 253.
Bast, J., & Reitsma, P. (1998). Analyzing the development of individual differences in terms of Matthew effects of reading: Results from a Dutch longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1373–1399.
Cardon, L. R., DiLalla, L. F., Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1990). Genetic correlations between reading performance and IQ in the Colorado Adoption Project. Intelligence, 14, 245–257.
Cherny, S. S., Fulker, D. W., & Hewitt, J. K. (1997). Cognitive development from infancy to middle childhood. In Sternberg, R. & Grigorenko, E. (Eds.) Intelligence, heredity, and environment (pp. 463–482). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
DeFries, J. C. (1988). Colorado Reading Project: Longitudinal analyses. Annals of Dyslexia, 38, 120–130.
DeFries, J. C., & Baker, L. A. (1983). Parental contributions to longitudinal stability of cognitive measures in the Colorado Family Reading Study. Child Development, 54, 388–395.
DeFries, J. C., Plomin, R., & Fulker, D. W. (1994). Nature and nurture during middle childhood. Oxford: Blackwell.
DeFries, J. C., Plomin, R., & LaBuda, M. (1987). Genetic stability of cognitive development from childhood to adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 23, 4–12.
Dibble, E., & Cohen, D. J. (1974). Companion instruments for measuring children's competence and parental style. Archives of General Psychiatry, 30, 805–815.
Dunn, L. M., & Markwardt, F. C. (1970). Examiner's Manual: Peabody Individual Achievement Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Fisher, S. E., & DeFries, J. C. (2002). Developmental dyslexia: Genetic dissection of a complex cognitive trait. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 3, 767–782.
Fulker, D. W., & Cherny, S. S. (1995). Genetic and environmental influences on cognition during childhood. Population Research and Policy Review, 14, 283–300.
Fulker, D. W., Cherny, S. S., & Cardon, L. R. (1993). Continuity and change in cognitive development. In Plomin, R. & McClearn, G. E. (Eds.), Nature, nurture, and psychology (pp. 99–120). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New York: The Free Press.
Jimerson, S., Egeland, B., & Teo, A. (1999). A longitudinal study of achievement trajectories: Factors associated with change. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 116–126.
Knopik, V. S., & DeFries, J. C. (1999). Etiology of covariation between reading and mathematics performance: A twin study. Twin Research, 2, 226–234.
Koutsoulis, M. K., & Campbell, J. R. (2001). Family processes affect students' motivation, and science and math achievement in Cypriot high schools. Structural Equation Modeling, 8, 108–127.
McGue, M., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Iacono, W. G., & Lykken, D. T. (1993). Behavioral genetics of cognitive ability: A life-span perspective. In Plomin, R. & McClearn, G. E. (Eds.) Nature, nurture, and psychology (pp. 59–76). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1981). The Family Environmental Scale manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Neale, M. C., & Cardon, L. R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. NATO ASI series. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Press.
Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H. H. (2002). Mx: Statistical modeling. (6th ed.) [Computer software], Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University.
Petrill, S. A., Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. C. (2003). Nature, nurture, and the transition to early adolescence. New York: Oxford University Press.
Plomin, R. (1994). Genetics and experience: The interplay between nature and nurture. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Plomin, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1979). Multivariate behavioral as genetic analysis of twin data on scholastic abilities. Behavior Genetics, 9, 505–517.
Plomin, R. & DeFries, J. C. (1985). Origins of individual differences in infancy: The Colorado Adoption Project. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1988). Nature and nurture during infancy and early childhood. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Plomin, R., Fulker, D. W., Corley, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1997). Nature, nurture, and cognitive development from 1 to 16 years: A parent-offspring adoption study. Psychological Science, 8, 442–447.
Rhea, S. A., & Corley, R. (1994). Applied Issues. In DeFries, J. C., Plomin, R., & Fulker, D. W. (Eds.) Nature and nurture during middle childhood (pp. 295–309). Oxford: Blackwell.
Robinson, N. M., Lanzi, R. G., Weinberg, R. A., Ramey, S. L., & Ramey, C. T. (2002). Family factors associated with high academic competence in former head start children at third grade. Gifted Child Quarterly, 46, 278–290.
Shaywitz, S. E., Escobar, M. D., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Makuch, R. (1992). Evidence that dyslexia may represent the lower tail of a normal distribution of reading ability. New England Journal of Medicine, 326, 144–150.
Stevenson, J., Graham, P., Fredman, G., & McLoughlin, V. (1987). A twin study of genetic influences on reading and spelling ability and disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28, 229–247.
Stoolmiller, M. (1998). Correcting estimates of shared environmental variance for range restriction in adoption studies using a truncated multivariate normal model. Behavior Genetics, 28, 429–441.
Stoolmiller, M. (1999). Implications of the restricted range of family environments for estimates of heritability and nonshared environment in behavior-genetic adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 392–405.
Wadsworth, S. J., Corley, R., Hewitt, J. K., & DeFries, J. C. (2001). Stability of genetic and environmental influences on reading performance at 7, 12, and 16 years of age in the Colorado Adoption Project. Behavior Genetics, 31, 353–359.
Wadsworth, S. J., & DeFries, J. C. (2003). Etiology of the stability of reading performance from 7 to 12 years of age and its possible mediation by IQ. In Petrill, S. A., Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (Eds.) Nature, nurture, and the transition to early adolescence (pp. 49–61). New York: Oxford University Press.
Wadsworth, S. J., Fulker, D. W., & DeFries, J. C. (1999). Stability of genetic and environmental influences on reading performance at 7 and 12 years of age in the Colorado Adoption Project. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 319–332.
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, et al . (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468–479.
Walker, H. M., & McConnell, S. W. (1988). Walker-McConnell Scale of Social Competence and School Adjustment. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Welsh, M., Parke, R. D., Widaman, K., & O'Neil, R. (2001). Linkages between children's social and academic competence: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 39, 463–482.
Wechsler, D. (1974). Examiner's manual: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.
Williams, S., & McGee, R. (1996). Reading in childhood and mental health in early adulthood. In Beitchman, J. H., Cohen, N.J., Konstantareas, M. M., & Tannock, R. (Eds.), Language, learning, and behavior disorders: Developmental, biological, and clinical perspectives (pp. 530–554). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.