Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-7qhmt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T09:02:20.632Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive Underpinnings of Creativity

from Part II - Underpinnings of Creativity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 April 2019

James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context: Update to “The social psychology of creativity.” Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1983). Social psychology of creativity: A componential conceptualization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 357377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ashcraft, M. H. (1978). Property norms for typical and atypical items from 17 categories: A description and discussion. Memory and Cognition, 6(3), 227232. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197450CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlowe, W. D. & Summers, I. (1979). Barlowe’s guide to extraterrestrials. New York: Workman Publishing.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory and Cognition, 11, 211227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (1991). Deriving categories to achieve goals. In Bower, G. H. (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 27, pp. 164). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Basadur, M. (1994). Managing the creative process in organizations. In Runco, M.A. (ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity (pp. 237268). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Basalla, G. (1988). The evolution of technology. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 8795. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Becker, A. H. & Ward, T. B. (1991). Children’s use of shape in extending novel labels to animate objects: Identity versus postural change. Cognitive Development, 6, 316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bredart, S., Ward, T. B., & Marczewski, P. (1998). Structured imagination of novel creatures’ faces. American Journal of Psychology, 111, 607725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browne, B. A. & Cruse, D. F. (1988). The incubation effect: Illusion or illumination? Human Performance, 1(3), 177185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cacciari, C., Levorato, M. C., & Cicogna, P. (1997). Imagination at work: Conceptual and linguistic creativity in children. In Ward, T. B., Smith, S. M., & Vaid, J. (eds.), Creative thought: An investigation of conceptual structures and processes (pp. 145177). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cardoso, C. & Badke-Schaub, P. (2011). The influence of different pictorial representations during idea generation. Journal of Creative Behavior, 45(2), 130146. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.2011.tb01092.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, J., Fu, K., Schunn, C., Cagan, J., Wood, K., & Kotovsky, K. (2011). On the benefits and pitfalls of analogies for innovative design: Ideation performance based on analogical distance, commonness, and modality of examples. Journal of Mechanical Design, 133, 081004. https://doi.org/10.1115/1.4004396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, J. & Schunn, C. (2015a). The impact of analogies on creative concept generation: Lessons from an in vivo study in engineering design. Cognitive Science, 39, 126155. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, J. & Schunn, C. (2015b). The importance of iteration in creative conceptual combination. Cognition, 145, 104115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.08.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, P., Mugge, R., & Schoormans, J. P. (2014). A new strategy to reduce design fixation: Presenting partial photographs to designers. Design Studies, 35(4), 374391. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2014.02.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, B.T. & Schunn, C.D. (2005). Spontaneous access and analogical incubation effects. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 207220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, B.T. & Schunn, C.D. (2007). The relationship of analogical distance to analogical function and pre-inventive structure: The case of engineering design. Memory and Cognition, 35, 2938.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chrysikou, E. G. & Weisberg, R. W. (2005). Following the wrong footsteps: Fixation effects of pictorial examples in a design problem-solving task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(5), 11341148.Google Scholar
Chua, R. Y. J., Yannig, , & Lemoine, J. F. (2014). The impact of culture on creativity: How cultural tightness and cultural distance affect global innovation crowdsourcing work. Administrative Science Quarterly, 60, 189227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cockbain, J., Vertolli, M. O., & Davies, J. (2014). Creative imagination is stable across technological media: The spore creature creator versus pencil and paper. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 48(1), 1324. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cropley, A. J. (1999). Creativity and cognition: Producing effective novelty. Roeper Review, 21(4), 253265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cropley, D. & Cropley, A. (2010). Functional creativity: “Products” and the generation of effective novelty. In Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.). The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 301317), New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crouch, T. D. (1992). Why Wilbur and Orville? Some thoughts on the Wright brothers and the process of invention. In Weber, R. J. & Perkins, D. N. (eds.), Inventive minds (pp. 8092), London: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Implications of a systems perspective for the study of creativity. In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 313335). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Getzels, J. W. (1971). Discovery-oriented behavior and the originality of creative products: A study with artists. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 19, 4752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, D. & Moreau, P. (2002). The influence and value of analogical thinking during new product ideation. Journal of Marketing Research, 39, 4760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Cruz, H. (2013). Religious concepts as structured imagination. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 23(1), 6374. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2013.735495CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Cruz, H. & De Smedt, J. (2010). Science as structured imagination. Journal of Creative Behavior, 44, 3752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodds, R. A., Smith, S. M., & Ward, T. B. (2002). The use of environmental clues during incubation. Creativity Research Journal, 14, 287304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodds, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (2003). A review of the experimental literature on incubation in problem solving and creativity. In Runco, M. A. (ed.), Creativity research handbook (Vol. 3). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.Google Scholar
Donaldson, S. R. (1992). The real story (Afterword). New York: Bantam Books.Google Scholar
Dunbar, K. (1997). How scientists think: On-line creativity and conceptual change in science. In Ward, T. B., Smith, S. M., & Vaid, J. (eds.), Creative thought: An investigation of conceptual structures and processes (pp. 461494). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estes, Z. & Ward, T. B. (2002). The emergence of novel attributes in concept modification. Creativity Research Journal, 14, 149156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(4), 290309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldhusen, J. F. (1995). Creativity: A knowledge base, metacognitive skills, and personality factors. Journal of Creative Behavior, 29(4), 255268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldhusen, J. F. (2002). Creativity: The knowledge base and children. High Ability Studies, 13(2), 179183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finke, R. A. (1990). Creative imagery: Discoveries and inventions in visualization. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative cognition: Theory, research, and applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Frensch, P. A. & Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Expertise and intelligent thinking: When is it worse to know better? In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 5, pp. 157188). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Friedel, R. D., Israel, P., & Finn, B. S. (1986). Edison’s electric light: Biography of an invention. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. (1983). Structure mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7, 155170.Google Scholar
Gentner, D. (1989). The mechanisms of analogical learning. In Vosniadou, S. & Ortony, A. (eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., Brem, S., Ferguson, R., Wolff, P., Markman, A. B., & Forbus, K. (1997). Analogy and creativity in the works of Johannes Kepler. In Ward, T. B., Smith, S. M., & Vaid, J. (eds.), Creative thought: An investigation of conceptual structures and processes (pp. 403460). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D., Holyoak, K., & Kokinov, B. (2001). The analogical mind: Perspectives from cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. & Toupin, C. (1986). Systematicity and surface similarity in the development of analogy. Cognitive Science, 10, 277300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, T., Wiley, J., Koppel, R. H., & Storm, B. C. (2017). Constraining or constructive? The effects of examples on idea novelty. The Journal of Creative Behavior, https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Getzels, J. W. & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The creative vision: A longitudinal study of problem finding in art. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Gick, M. L. & Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 306355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gick, M. L. & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glaveanu, V. P. (2010). Principles for a cultural psychology of creativity. Culture and Psychology, 16, 147163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, G. & Kaufman, J. C. (2014). Gremlins in my head: Predicting stage fright in elite actors. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 32, 133148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, W. (1961). Synectics: The development of creative capacity. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Graf, P., Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1985). Priming across modalities and priming across category levels: Extending the domain of preserved function in amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 386396.Google ScholarPubMed
Hampton, J. A. (1979). Polymorphous concepts in semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18(4), 441461. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(79)90246-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampton, J. A. (1997). Emergent attributes in combined concepts. In Ward, T. B., Smith, S. M., & Vaid, J. (eds.), Creative thought: An investigation of conceptual structures and processes (pp. 83110). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hastie, R., Schroeder, C., & Weber, R. (1990). Creating complex social conjunction categories from simple categories. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28, 242247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holyoak, K. & Thagard, P. (1995). Mental leaps: Analogy in creative thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Howard-Jones, P. A., Blakemore, S., Samuel, E. A., Summers, I. R., & Claxton, G. (2005). Semantic divergence and creative story generation: An fMRI investigation. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 240250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.05.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jansson, D. G. & Smith, S. M. (1991). Design fixation. Design Studies, 12, 311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1990). Constraints on representational change: Evidence from children’s drawing. Cognition, 34, 5783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, A. B. & Kaufman, J. C. (2015). Animal creativity and innovation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. & Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Beyond big and little: The Four C model of creativity. Review of General Psychology, 13, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., Haider, H., & Rhenius, D. (1999). Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 15341555. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.25.6.1534Google Scholar
Kohn, N. W., Paulus, P. B., & Korde, R. M. (2011). Conceptual combinations and subsequent creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 203210. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2011.595659CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohn, N. & Smith, S. M. (2009). Partly versus completely out of your mind: Effects of incubation and distraction on resolving fixation. Journal of Creative Behavior, 43, 102118. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.2009.tb01309.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunda, Z., Miller, D. T., & Claire, T. (1990). Combining social concepts: The role of causal reasoning. Cognitive Science, 14, 551577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3(3), 299321. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(88)90014-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landau, J. D. & Lehr, D. P. (2004). Conformity to experimenter-provided examples: Will people use an unusual feature? Journal of Creative Behavior, 38, 180191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landau, J. D. & Leynes, P.A. (2004). Manipulations that disrupt the generative stage decrease conformity to examples: Evidence from two paradigms. Memory, 12, 90103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubart, T. (2010). Cross-cultural perspectives on creativity. In Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.). The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 265278), New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubart, T. I. & Sternberg, R. J. (1995). An investment approach to creativity. In Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Finke, R. A. (eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 269302). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marsh, R. L., Landau, J. D., & Hicks, J. L. (1996). How examples may (and may not) constrain creativity. Memory and Cognition, 24, 669680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, R. L., Ward, T. B., & Landau, J. D. (1999). The inadvertent use of prior knowledge in a generative cognitive task. Memory and Cognition, 27, 94105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 12581265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mobley, M. I., Doares, L. M., & Mumford, M. D. (1992). Process analytic models of creative capacities: Evidence for the combination and reorganization process. Creativity Research Journal, 5, 125155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreau, C. P. & Dahl, D. W. (2005). Designing the solution: The impact of constraints on consumers’ creativity, Journal of Consumer Research, 32(1), 1322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrison, R. G., McCarthy, S. W., & Molony, J. M. (2017). The experience of insight follows incubation in the compound remote associates task. Journal of Creative Behavior, 51, 180187. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mumford, M. D. & Gustafson, S. B. (1988). Creativity Syndrome: Integration, application, and innovation. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 2743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mumford, M. D., Mobley, M. I., Uhlman, C. E., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Doares, L. M. (1991). Process analytic models of creative thought. Creativity Research Journal, 4, 91122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mumford, M. D., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Redmond, M. R. (1994). Problem construction and cognition: Applying problem representations in ill-defined problems. In Runco, M. A. (ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity (pp. 339). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Niu, W. & Sternberg, R. J. (2001). Cultural influences on artistic creativity and its evaluation. International Journal of Psychology, 36, 225241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okada, T. & Ishibashi, K. (2016). Imitation, inspiration, and creation: Cognitive process of creative drawing by copying others’ artworks. Cognitive Science, https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perkins, D. N. (1997). Creativity’s camel: The role of analogy in invention. In Ward, T. B., Smith, S. M., & Vaid, J. (eds.), Creative thought: An investigation of conceptual structures and processes (pp. 523538). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10227-019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purcell, A. T. & Gero, J. S. (1996). Design and other types of fixation. Design Studies, 17, 363383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., Intons-Peterson, M. J., & Anderson, R. A. (eds.). (1993). Imagery, creativity, and discovery: A cognitive perspective, Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers (North-Holland).Google Scholar
Rothenberg, A. (1979). The emerging goddess. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rothenberg, A. & Sobel, R. S. (1980). Creation of literary metaphors as stimulated by superimposed versus separated visual images. Journal of Mental Imagery, 4, 7991.Google Scholar
Rubin, D. C. & Kontis, T. C. (1983). A schema for common cents. Memory and Cognition, 11, 335341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runco, M. A. & Chand, I. (1994). Conclusions concerning problem finding, problem solving, and creativity. In Runco, M. A. (ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity (pp. 217290). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. & Chand, I. (1995). Cognition and creativity. Educational Psychology Review, 7, 243267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seifert, C. M., Meyer, D. E., Davidson, N., Patalano, A. L., & Yaniv, I. (1994). Demystification of cognitive insight: Opportunistic assimilation and the prepared-mind perspective. In Sternberg, R. J. & Davidson, J. E. (eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 65124). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seifert, C. M. & Patalano, A. L. (2001). Opportunism in memory: Preparing for chance encounters. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10(6), 198201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Kaufman, J. C., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Wigert, B. (2011). Cantankerous creativity: Honesty–humility, agreeableness, and the HEXACO structure of creative achievement. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(5), 687689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1997). Creativity in personality, developmental, and social psychology: Any links with cognitive psychology. In Ward, T. B., Smith, S. M., & Vaid, J. (eds.), Creative thought: An investigation of conceptual structures and processes (pp. 309324). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, S. M. (1995a). Fixation, incubation, and insight in memory and creative thinking. In Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Finke, R. A. (eds.), The creative cognition approach (135156). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smith, S. M. (1995b). Getting into and out of mental ruts: A theory of fixation, incubation, and insight. In Sternberg, R. J. & Davidson, J. E. (eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 229250). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smith, S. M. & Blankenship, S. E. (1989). Incubation effects. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 311314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, S. M. & Blankenship, S. E. (1991). Incubation and the persistence of fixation in problem solving. American Journal of Psychology, 104, 6187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., Gerkens, D. R., & Angello, G. (2017). Alternating incubation effects in the generation of category exemplars. Journal of Creative Behavior, 51, 95106. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Finke, R. A. (1995). The creative cognition approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Schumacher, J. S. (1993). Constraining effects of examples in a creative generation task. Memory and Cognition, 21, 837845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sobel, R. S. & Rothenberg, A. (1980). Artistic creation as stimulated by superimposed versus separated visual images. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 953961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, R. J. (1988). A three-facet model of creativity. In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.), The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives (pp. 125147). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. & Lubart, T. (1995). Defying the crowd. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. & Lubart, T. (1996). Investing in creativity. American Psychologist, 51, 677688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 24052415. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0052CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thagard, P. (1984). Conceptual combination and scientific discovery. In Asquith, P. & Kitcher, P. (eds.), PSA, 1, East Lansing, MI: Philosophy of Science Association.Google Scholar
Tourangeau, R. & Rips, L. (1991). Interpreting and evaluating metaphors. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 452472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tourangeau, R. & Sternberg, R. J. (1981). Aptness in metaphor. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 2755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tourangeau, R. & Sternberg, R. J. (1982). Understanding and appreciating metaphors. Cognition, 11, 203244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treffinger, D. J., Isaksen, S. G., & Dorval, K. B. (1994). Creative problem solving: An overview. In Runco, M.A. (ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity (pp. 223236). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Tversky, B. & Hemenway, K. (1984). Objects, parts, and categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(2), 169193. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.113.2.169CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York: Harcourt Brace and Company.Google Scholar
Ward, T. B. (1994). Structured imagination: The role of conceptual structure in exemplar generation. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, T. B. (1995). What’s old about new ideas? In Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Finke, R. A. (eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 157178). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ward, T. B. (2001). Creative cognition, conceptual combination and the creative writing of Stephen R. Donaldson. American Psychologist, 56, 350354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, T. B. (2008). The role of domain knowledge in creative generation. Learning and Individual Differences, 18(4), 363366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, T. B. (2015). Content, collaboration and creativity in virtual worlds. In Green, G. & Kaufman, J. (eds.). Video Games and Creativity. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ward, T. B. (2018). Creativity as a continuum. In Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.), The nature of human creativity (pp. 335350). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, T. B., Dodds, R. A., Saunders, K. N., & Sifonis, C. M. (2000). Attribute centrality and imaginative thought. Memory and Cognition, 28, 13871397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ward, T. B., Finke, R. A., & Smith, S. M. (1995). Creativity and the mind: Discovering the genius within. New York: Plenum Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-3330-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, T. B., Patterson, M. J. & Sifonis, C. (2004). The role of specificity and abstraction in creative idea generation. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, T. B., Patterson, M. J., Sifonis, C. M., Dodds, R. A., & Saunders, K. N. (2002). The role of graded category structure in imaginative thought. Memory and Cognition, 30, 199216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ward, T. B., Saunders, K. N., & Dodds, R. A. (1999). Creative cognition in gifted adolescents. Roeper Review, 21, 260265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, T. B. & Sifonis, S. M. (1997). Task demands and generative thinking: What changes and what remains the same? Journal of Creative Behavior, 31, 245259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, T. B. & Sonneborn, M. S. (2009). Creative expression in virtual environments: Imitation, imagination and individualized collaboration. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 3, 211221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, T. B. & Wickes, K. N. S. (2009). Stable and dynamic properties of graded category structure in imaginative thought. Creativity Research Journal, 21, 1523. https://doi-org.libdata.lib.ua.edu/10.1080/10400410802633376CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisberg, R. W. (1995). Case studies of creative thinking: Reproduction versus restructuring in the real world. In Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Finke, R. A. (eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 5372). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Weisberg, R. W. (2006 ). Creativity: Understanding innovation in problem solving, science, invention, and the arts. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
White, J. H. (1978). The American railroad passenger car. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Wilkenfeld, M. J. & Ward, T. B. (2001). Similarity and emergence in conceptual combination. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 2138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yi, X., Hu, W., Scheithauer, H., & Niu, W. (2013). Cultural and bilingual influences on artistic creativity performances: Comparison of German and Chinese students. Creativity Research Journal, 25(1), 97108. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2013.752260CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yi, X., Plucker, J. A., & Guo, J. (2015). Modeling influences on divergent thinking and artistic creativity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 16, 16621668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2015.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Youmans, R. J. (2011). The effects of physical prototyping and group work on the reduction of design fixation. Design Studies, 32(2), 115138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2010.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeng, L., Proctor, R. W., & Salvendy, G. (2011). Fostering creativity in product and service development: Validation in the domain of information technology. Human Factors, 53(3), 245270. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720811409219CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Abraham, A. (2016). The imaginative mind. Human Brain Mapping, 37, 41974211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abraham, A, Beudt, S, Otta, DVM, & von Cramon, DY. (2012). Creative cognition and the brain: Dissociations between frontal, parietal–temporal and basal ganglia groups. Brain Research 1482, 5570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amer, T., Campbell, K. L., & Hasher, L. (2016). Cognitive control as a double-edged sword. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 905915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J., & Spreng, R. N. (2014). The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316, 2952.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avitia, M. J., & Kaufman, J. C. (2014). Beyond g and c: The relationship of rated creativity to long-term storage and retrieval (Glr). Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(3), 293302. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036772CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W. Y., Franklin, M. S., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Inspired by distraction: Mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychological Science, 23, 11171122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F., Tugade, M. M., & Engle, R. W. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 553573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batey, M. & Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence, and personality: A critical review of the scattered literature. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 132, 355429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Kaufman, S. B., & Silvia, P. J. (2015). Default and executive network coupling supports creative idea production. Scientific Reports, 5, 10964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 8795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaty, R. E., Nusbaum, E. C., & Silvia, P. J. (2014). Does insight problem solving predict real-world creativity?. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 287292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaty, R. E. & Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 309319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaty, R. E. & Silvia, P. J. (2013). Metaphorically speaking: Cognitive abilities and the production of figurative language. Memory and Cognition, 41, 255267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaty, R. E., Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Jauk, E., & Benedek, M. (2014). The role of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Memory and Cognition, 42, 11861195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M. (2018a). Internally directed attention in creative cognition. In Jung, R. E. & Vartanian, O. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of the neuroscience of creativity (pp. 180194). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M. (2018b). The neuroscience of creative idea generation. In Kapoula, Z., Renoult, J., Volle, E., & Andreatta, M. (eds.), Exploring transdisciplinarity in art and science (pp. 3148). Cham: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-76054-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., Bergner, S., Könen, T., Fink, A., & Neubauer, A. C. (2011). EEG alpha synchronization is related to top-down control in convergent and divergent thinking. Neuropsychologia, 49, 35053511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 480485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M. & Jauk, E. (2018). Spontaneous and controlled processes in creative cognition. In Fox, K. C. R. & Christoff, K. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of spontaneous thought: mind-wandering, creativity, dreaming, and clinical conditions (pp. 285298). New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.22Google Scholar
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Kerschenbauer, K., Anderwald, R., & Grond, L. (2017). Creating art: An experience sampling study in the domain of moving image art. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Sommer, M., Arendasy, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: The common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence, 46, 7383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M., Kenett, Y. N., Umdasch, K., Anaki, D., Faust, M., & Neubauer, A. (2017). How semantic memory structure and intelligence contribute to creative thought: A network science approach. Thinking and Reasoning, 23, 158183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., Nordtvedt, N., Jauk, E., Koschmieder, C., Pretsch, J., Krammer, G., & Neubauer, A. C. (2016). Assessment of creativity evaluation skills: A psychometric investigation in prospective teachers. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 21, 7584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., Panzierer, L., Jauk, E., & Neubauer, A. C. (2017). Creativity on tap? Effects of alcohol intoxication on creative cognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 56, 128134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.06.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M., Schickel, R.J., Jauk, E., Fink, A., & Neubauer, A.C. (2014). Alpha power increases in right parietal cortex reflects focused internal attention. Neuropsychologia, 56, 393400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M., Stoiser, R., Walcher, S., & Körner, C. (2017). Eye behavior associated with internally versus externally directed cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1092. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01092CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burch, G. St. J., Hemsley, D. R., Corr, P. J., & Pavelis, C. (2006). Personality, creativity, and latent inhibition. European Journal of Personality, 20, 107122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cai, D. J., Mednick, S. A., Harrison, E. M., Kanady, J. C., & Mednick, S. C. (2009). REM, not incubation, improves creativity by priming associative networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 1013010134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 499506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cerruti, C. & Schlaug, G. (2009). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex enhances complex verbal associative thought. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 19801987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, L., Hu, W., Jia, X., & Runco, M. A. (2016). The different role of cognitive inhibition in early versus late creative problem finding. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10, 3241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cho, S. H., Nijenhuis, J. T., van Vianen, A. E. M., Kim, H.-B., & Lee, K.-H. (2010). The relationship between diverse components of intelligence and creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 44, 125137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chrysikou, E. G., Hamilton, R. H., Coslett, H. B., Datta, A., Bikson, M., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2013). Noninvasive transcranial direct current stimulation over the left prefrontal cortex facilitates cognitive flexibility in tool use. Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 8189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chrysikou, E. G., Weber, M. J., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2014). A matched filter hypothesis for cognitive control. Neuropsychologia, 62, 341355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, A. R. A., Getz, S. J., Macnamara, B., & Engel de Abreu, P. M. J. (2010). Working memory and intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. & Kaufman, S. B. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of intelligence (pp. 394418). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cooper, N. R., Croft, R. J., Dominey, S. J. J., Burgess, A. P., & Gruzelier, J. H. (2003). Paradox lost? Exploring the role of alpha oscillations during externally vs. internally directed attention and the implications for idling and inhibition hypotheses. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 47(1), 6574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corgnet, B., Espín, A. M., & Hernán-González, R. (2016). Creativity and cognitive skills among millennials: Thinking too much and creating too little. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 18, 391404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Currey, M. (2013). Daily rituals: How artists work. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
De Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A., Bass, M., Wolsink, I., & Roskes, M. (2012). Working memory benefits creative insight, musical improvisation, and original ideation through maintained task-focused attention. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 656669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dietrich, A. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness: The transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 231256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dietrich, A. & Haider, H. (2016). A neurocognitive framework for human creative thought. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 2078.Google ScholarPubMed
Dixon, M. L., Fox, K. C. R., & Christoff, K. (2014). A framework for understanding the relationship between externally and internally directed cognition. Neuropsychologia, 62, 321330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dorfman, L., Martindale, C., Gassimova, V., & Vartanian, O. (2008). Creativity and speed of information processing: A double dissociation involving elementary versus inhibitory cognitive tasks. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 13821390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edl, S., Benedek, M., Papousek, I., Weiss, E. M., & Fink, A. (2014). Creativity and the Stroop interference effect. Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 3842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellwood, S., Pallier, G., Snyder, A., & Gallate, J. (2009). The incubation effect: Hatching a solution? Creativity Research Journal, 21, 614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2008). Dual-process accounts of reasoning, judgement and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St. B. T. & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 223241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G. & Turner, M. (1998). Conceptual integration networks. Cognitive Science, 22, 133187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fink, A. & Benedek, M. (2013). The creative brain: Brain correlates underlying the generation of original ideas. In Vartanian, O., Bristol, A. S., & Kaufman, J. C. (eds.), Neuroscience of creativity (pp. 207232). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fink, A. & Benedek, M. (2014). EEG alpha power and creative ideation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 44, 111123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fink, A., Grabner, R. H., Benedek, M., Reishofer, G., Hauswirth, V., Fally, M., Neuper, C., Ebner, F., & Neubauer, A. C. (2009). The creative brain: investigation of brain activity during creative problem solving by means of EEG and fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 734748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative cognition: Theory, research, and applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Flaherty, AW. (2011). Brain illness and creativity: Mechanisms and treatment risks. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 132143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fleck, J. I. & Weisberg, R. W. (2013). Insight versus analysis: Evidence for diverse methods in problem solving. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 436463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forthmann, B., Gerwig, A., Holling, H., Çelik, P., Storme, M., & Lubart, T. (2016). The be-creative effect in divergent thinking: The interplay of instructions and object frequency. Intelligence, 57, 2532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forthmann, B., Wilken, A., Doebler, P., & Holling, H. (2016). Strategy induction enhances creativity in figural divergent thinking. Journal of Creative Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., Ellamil, M., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Christoff, K. (2015). The wandering brain: Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. NeuroImage, 111, 611621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., & Raichle, M.E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 102, 96739678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N. P. & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 101135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galang, A. J. R. (2010). The prosocial psychopath: Explaining the paradoxes of the creative personality. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(8), 12411248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.03.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallate, J., Wong, C., Ellwood, S., Roring, R. W., & Snyder, A. (2012). Creative people use nonconscious processes to their advantage. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 146151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilhooly, K. J., & Fioratou, E. (2009). Executive functions in insight versus non-insight problem solving: An individual differences approach. Thinking and Reasoning, 15(4), 355376. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546780903178615CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., Anthony, S. H., & Wynn, V. (2007). Divergent thinking: Strategies and executive involvement in generating novel uses for familiar objects. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 611625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilhooly, K. J., Georgiou, G., & Devery, U. (2013). Incubation and creativity: Do something different. Thinking and Reasoning, 19, 137149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilhooly, K. J. & Murphy, P. (2005). Differentiating insight from non-insight problems. Thinking and Reasoning, 11, 279302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golden, C. J. (1975). The measurement of creativity by the Stroop Color and Word Test. Journal of Personality Assessment, 39, 502506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonen-Yaacovi, G., de Souza, L. C., Levy, R., Urbanski, M., Josse, G., & Volle, E. (2013). Rostral and caudal prefrontal contribution to creativity: A meta-analysis of functional imaging data. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, A. E., Spiegel, K. A., Giangrande, E. J., Weinberger, A. B., Gallagher, N. M., & Turkeltaub, P. E. (2017). Thinking cap plus thinking zap: tDCS of frontopolar cortex improves creative analogical reasoning and facilitates conscious augmentation of state creativity in verb generation. Cerebral Cortex, 27, 26282639.Google ScholarPubMed
Green, M. J. & Williams, L. M. (1999). Schizotypy and creativity as effects of reduced cognitive inhibition. Personality and Individuals Differences, 27, 263276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grim, T. (2008). A possible role of social activity to explain differences in publication output among ecologists. Oikos, 117, 484487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Groborz, M. & Necka, E. (2003). Creativity and cognitive control: Explorations of generation and evaluation skills. Creativity Research Journal, 15, 183197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Gustafson, R. (1991). Effect of alcohol on quantity of creative production using the Purdue tests. Psychological Reports, 69, 8390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hao, N., Wu, M., Runco, M. A., & Pina, J. (2015). More mind wandering, fewer original ideas: Be not distracted during creative idea generation. Acta Psychologica, 161, 110116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, Y. & Horne, J. (1998). Sleep loss impairs short and novel language tasks having a prefrontal focus. Journal of Sleep Research, 7, 95100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, Y. & Horne, J. (1999). One night of sleep loss impairs innovative thinking and flexible decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78, 128145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horne, J. (1998). Sleep loss and “divergent” thinking ability. Sleep, 11, 528536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarosz, A. F., Colflesh, G. J., & Wiley, J. (2012). Uncorking the muse: Alcohol intoxication facilitates creative problem solving. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 487493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Dunst, B., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). The relationship between intelligence and creativity: New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence, 41, 212221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jauk, E., Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014). The road to creative achievement: A latent variable model of ability and personality predictors. European Journal of Personality, 28, 95105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonides, J. & Smith, E. E. (1997). The architecture of working memory. In Rugg, M. D. (ed.), Cognitive neuroscience (pp. 243276). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Mead, B. S., Carrasco, J. & Flores, R. A. (2013). The structure of creative cognition in the human brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jung-Beeman, M. (2005). Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language. Trends in Cognitive. Sciences, 9, 512518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: An experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychological Science, 18, 614621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., & Conway, D. Z. (2005). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131, 6671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karwowski, M., Dul, J., Gralewski, J., Jauk, E., Jankowska, D. M., Gajda, A., Chruszczewski, M. H., & Benedek, M. (2016). Is creativity without intelligence possible? A Necessary Condition analysis. Intelligence, 57, 105117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2016.04.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karwowski, K., Kaufman, J.C., Lebuda, I., Szumskic, G., Firkowska-Mankiewicz, A. (2017). Intelligence in childhood and creative achievements in middle-age: The necessary condition approach. Intelligence, 64, 3644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kellner, R. & Benedek, M. (2017). The role of creative potential and intelligence for humor production. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11, 5258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, K. H. (2005). Can only intelligent people be creative? A metaanalysis. The Journal of Secondary Gifted Education, 16, 5766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, K. H. (2008). Can only intelligent people be creative? The Journal of Secondary Gifted Education, 2, 5766.Google Scholar
Klimesch, W. (2012). Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 606617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kounios, J. & Beeman, M. (2014). The cognitive neuroscience of insight. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 7193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwiatkowski, J., Vartanian, O., & Martindale, C. (1999). Creativity and speed of mental processing. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 17, 187196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, A. R., Verret, L. D., & Watt, C. (1984). Drinking and creativity: Objective and subjective effects. Addictive Behaviors, 9, 395399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, C. S., Huggins, A. C., & Therriault, D. J. (2014). A measure of creativity or intelligence? Examining internal and external structure validity evidence of the Remote Associates Test. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 446460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, C. S. & Therriault, D. J. (2013). The cognitive underpinnings of creative thought: A latent variable analysis exploring the roles of intelligence and working memory in three creative thinking processes. Intelligence, 41, 306320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, S., Erkkinen, M. G., Healey, M. L., Xu, Y., Swett, K. E., Chow, H. M., & Braun, A. R. (2015). Brain activity and connectivity during poetry composition: Toward a multidimensional model of the creative process. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 33513372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubart, T. I. (2001). Models of the creative process: Past, present and future. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 295308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E. (1997). Latent inhibition as a measure of learned inattention: Some problems and solutions. Behavioral Brain Research, 88, 7583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ludwig, A. M. (1990). Alcohol input and creative output. Addiction, 85, 953963.Google ScholarPubMed
Martindale, C. (1999). Biological Bases of Creativity. In: Sternberg, R. J. (ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 137152). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Martindale, C. & Hasenfus, N. (1978). EEG differences as a function of creativity, stage of the creative process, and effort to be original. Biological Psychology, 6, 157167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martindale, C. & Hines, D. (1975). Creativity and cortical activation during creative, intellectual and EEG feedback tasks. Biological Psychology, 3, 91100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez-Conde, S., Otero-Millan, J., & Macknik, S. L. (2013). The impact of microsaccades on vision: Towards a unified theory of saccadic function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 8396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayseless, N. & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2015). Enhancing verbal creativity: Modulating creativity by altering the balance between right and left inferior frontal gyrus with tDCS. Neuroscience, 291, 167176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrew, K. S. (2009). CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Intelligence, 37, 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMillan, R. L., Kaufman, S. B., & Singer, J. L. (2013). Ode to positive constructive daydreaming. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00626Google Scholar
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendelsohn, G.A. (1976). Associative and attentional processes in creative performance. Journal of Personality, 44, 341369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wagner, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trend in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 134139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mumford, M. D., Mobley, M. I., Reiter-Palmon, R., Uhlman, C., & Doares, L. M. (1991). Process analytic models of creative capacities. Creativity Research Journal, 4, 91122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nijstad, B. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., Rietzschel, E. F., & Bass, M. (2010). The dual pathway to creativity model: Creative ideation as a function of flexibility and persistence. European Review of Social Psychology, 21, 3477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norlander, T. & Gustafson, R. (1998). Effects of alcohol on a divergent figural fluency test during the illumination phase of the creative process. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 265274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nusbaum, E. C. & Silvia, P. J. (2011). Are intelligence and creativity really so different? Fluid intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use in divergent thinking. Intelligence, 39, 3645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nusbaum, E. C., Silvia, P. J., & Beaty, R. E. (2014). Ready, set, create: What instructing people to “be creative” reveals about the meaning and mechanisms of divergent thinking. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 423432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nusbaum, E. C., Silvia, P. J., & Beaty, R. E. (2017). Ha ha? Assessing individual differences in humor production ability. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11, 231241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oberauer, K., Süß, H.-M., Wilhelm, O., & Wittmann, W. W. (2008). Which working memory functions predict intelligence? Intelligence, 36, 641652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Hara, L. A. & Sternberg, R. J. (2001). It doesn’t hurt to ask: Effects of instructions to be creative, practical, or analytical on essay-writing performance and their interaction with students’ thinking styles. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 197210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmiero, M, Di Giacomo, D, & Passafiume, D. (2012) Creativity and dementia: A review. Cognitive Processing, 13, 193209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palva, S. & Palva, J. M. (2007). New vistas for α-frequency band oscillations. Trends in Neuroscience, 30(4), 150158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2007.02.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pan, X. & Yu, H. (2018). Different effects of cognitive shifting and intelligence on creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 52(3), 212225. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinho, A. L., Ullén, F., Castelo-Branco, M., Fransson, P., & de Manzano, Ö. (2015). Addressing a paradox: Dual strategies for creative performance in introspective and extrospective networks. Cerebral Cortex, 26(7), 30523063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plucker, J. A. (1999). Is the proof in the pudding? Reanalyses of Torrance’s (1958 to present) longitudinal data. Creativity Research Journal, 12(2), 103114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pringle, A. & Sowden, P. T. (2017a). The Mode Shifting Index (MSI): A new measure of the creative thinking skill of shifting between associative and analytic thinking. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 23, 1728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pringle, A. & Sowden, P. T. (2017b). Unearthing the creative thinking process: Fresh insights from a think aloud study of garden design. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11(3), 344358. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radel, R., Davranche, K., Fournier, M., Dietrich, A. (2015). The role of (dis)inhibition in creativity: Decreased inhibition improves idea generation. Cognition, 134, 110120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Randazzo, A. C., Muehlbach, M. J., Schweitzer, P. K., & Walsh, J. K. (1998). Cognitive function following sleep restriction in children ages 10–14. Sleep, 21, 861868.Google ScholarPubMed
Ray, W. J. & Cole, H. W. (1985). EEG alpha reflects attentional demands, and beta activity reflects emotional and cognitive processes. Science, 228, 750752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reverberi, C, Toraldo, A, D’Agostini, S, & Skrap, M. (2005). Better without (lateral) frontal cortex? Insight problems solved by frontal patients. Brain, 128, 28822890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritter, S. M. & Dijksterhuis, A. (2014). Creativity – the unconscious foundations of the incubation period. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rominger, C., Fink, A., Weiss, E. M., Bosch, J., & Papousek, I. (2017). Allusive thinking (remote associations) and auditory top-down inhibition skills differentially predict creativity and positive schizotypy. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 22, 108121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvi, C., Bricolo, E., Fronconeri, S.L., Kounios, J., Beeman, M. (2015). Sudden insight is associated with shutting out visual inputs. Psychonomic Bulletin and Reviews, 22, 18141819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santarnecchi, E., Brem, A. K., Levenbaum, E., Thompson, T., Kadosh, R. C., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2015). Enhancing cognition using transcranial electrical stimulation. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 4, 171178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D.R., Hassabis, D., Martin, V. C., Spreng, R .N., & Szpunar, K. K. (2012). The future of memory: Remembering, imagining, and the brain. Neuron, 76, 677694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seeley, W. W., Matthews, B. R., Crawford, R. K., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Foti, D., Mackenzie, I. R., et al. (2008). Unravelling Bolero: Progressive aphasia, transmodal creativity and the right posterior neocortex. Brain, 131, 3949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Adler, N., Aharon-Peretz, J., Perry, D., & Mayseless, N. (2011). The origins of originality: The neural bases of creative thinking and originality. Neuropsychologia, 49, 178185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silvia, P. J. (2008). Another look at creativity and intelligence: Exploring higher-order models and probable confounds. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 10121021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvia, P. J. (2015). Intelligence and creativity are pretty similar after all. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 599606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvia, P. J. & Beaty, R. E. (2012). Making creative metaphors: The importance of fluid intelligence for creative thought. Intelligence, 40, 343351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Beaty, R. E., & Nusbaum, E. (2013). Verbal fluency and creativity: General and specific contributions of broad retrieval ability (Gr) factors to divergent thinking. Intelligence, 41, 328340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2011). Creativity and discovery as blind variation: Campbell’s BVSR model after the half-century mark. Review of General Psychology, 15, 158174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, J. L. (1966). Daydreaming: An introduction to the experimental study of inner experience. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Sio, U. N. & Ormerod, T. C. (2009). Does incubation enhance problem solving? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 94120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smallwood, J. (2013). Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: A process–occurrence framework for self-generated mental activity. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 519535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smallwood, J. & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 66, 487518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeekens, B. A. & Kane, M. J. (2016). Working memory capacity, mind wandering, and creative cognition: An individual-differences investigation into the benefits of controlled versus spontaneous thought. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10, 389415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, K. A., Huber, D. A., & Vul, E. (2013). Multiply-constrained semantic search in the Remote Associates Test. Cognition, 128, 6475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M. & Blankenship, S. E. (1991). Incubation and the persistence of fixation in problem solving. American Journal of Psychology, 104, 6187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sowden, P. T., Pringle, A., & Gabora, L. (2014). The shifting sands of creative thinking: Connections to dual-process theory. Thinking and Reasoning, 21, 4060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., & Kim, A. S. (2009). The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 489510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, C., Malloch, S., McKechnie, S., & Steven, N. (2003). Choreographic cognition: The time-course and phenomenology of creating a dance. Pragmatics and Cognition, 11, 297326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2011). The Cognitive reflection test as a predictor of performance on heuristics-and-biases tasks. Memory and Cognition, 39, 1275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vartanian, O. (2009). Variable attention facilitates creative problem solving. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 5759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vartanian, O., Martindale, C., & Kwiatkowski, J. (2007). Creative potential, attention, and speed of information processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 14701480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Verleger, R., & Born, J. (2004). Sleep inspires insight. Nature, 427, 352355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walcher, S., Körner, C., & Benedek, M. (2017). Looking for ideas: Eye behavior during goal-directed internally-focused cognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 53, 165175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. London: Watts & Co.Google Scholar
Weinberger, A.B., Green, A.E., & Chrysikou, E.G. (2017). Using transcranial direct current stimulation to enhance creative cognition: Interactions between task, polarity, and stimulation site. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 246. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00246CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiggins, G. A., & Bhattacharya, J. (2014). Mind the gap: An attempt to bridge computational and neuroscientific approaches to study creativity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, e540. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00540CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wimmer, F., Hoffman, R. F., Bonato, R. A., & Moffitt, A. R. (1992). The effect of sleep deprivation on divergent thinking and attention processes. Journal of Sleep Research, 1, 223230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yanif, I. & Meyer, D. E. (1987). Activation and metacognition of inaccessible stored information: Potential bases for incubation effects in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 187205.Google Scholar
Zabelina, D. L. & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Dynamic network interactions supporting internally-oriented cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 40, 8693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zabelina, D. L., O’Leary, D., Pornpattananangkul, N., Nusslock, R., & Beeman, M. (2015). Creativity and sensory gating indexed by the P50: Selective versus leaky sensory gating in divergent thinkers and creative achievers. Neuropsychologia, 69, 7784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zabelina, D. L. & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Creativity as flexible cognitive control. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 136143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zabelina, D. L., Robinson, M. D., Council, J. R., & Bresin, K. (2012). Patterning and nonpatterning in creative cognition: Insights from performance in a random number generation task. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 137145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zabelina, D. L., Saporta, A., & Beeman, M. (2015). Flexible or leaky attention in creative people? Distinct patterns of attention for different types of creative thinking. Memory and Cognition, 44, 488498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Das Behalten erledigter und unerledigter Handlungen. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 185.Google Scholar
Zmigrod, S., Colzato, L. S., & Hommel, B. (2015). Stimulating creativity: Modulation of convergent and divergent thinking by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Creativity Research Journal, 27, 353360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Acar, S. & Runco, M. A. (2014). Assessing associative distance among ideas elicited by tests of divergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 26, 229238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Acar, S. & Runco, M. A. (2015). Thinking in multiple directions: Hyperspace categories in divergent thinking. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9, 4153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Acar, S. & Runco, M. A. (2017). Latency predicts category switch in divergent thinking. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11, 4351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Acar, S., Runco, M. A., & Park, H. (2018). A meta-analytic investigation of the explicit instructions in divergent thinking. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. (Manuscript under review.)Google Scholar
Albert, R. S. & Runco, M. A. (1987). The possible different personality dispositions of scientists and nonscientists. In Jackson, D. N. & Rushton, J. P. (eds.), Scientific excellence: Origins and assessment (pp. 6797). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
An, D., Song, Y., & Carr, M. (2016). A comparison of two models of creativity: Divergent thinking and creative expert performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 90, 7884.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barbot, B. (2018). The dynamics of creative ideation: Introducing a new assessment paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology. (Manuscript under review.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barron, F. (1963). Creativity and psychological health. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Barron, F. & Harrington, D. M. (1981). Creativity, intelligence, and personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 32, 439476. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.32.020181.002255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basadur, M. (1994). Managing the creative process in organizations. In Runco, M. A. (ed.)., Problem solving, problem finding, and creativity (pp. 237268). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Basadur, M. (1995). Optimal ideation-evaluation ratios. Creativity Research Journal, 8, 6375. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326934 crj0801_5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batey, M., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2009). Intelligence and personality as predictors of divergent thinking: The role of general, fluid and crystallised intelligence. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 4, 6069.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behrens, J., Ernst, H., & Shepherd, D. A. (2014). The decision to exploit an R&D project: Divergent thinking across middle and senior managers. Journal of Product Innovation Management, 31, 144158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beketayev, K. & Runco, M. A. (2016). Scoring divergent thinking tests with a semantics-based algorithm. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 12(2), 210220. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v12i2.1127CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M., Bergner, S., Könen, T., Fink, A., & Neubauer, A. C. (2011). EEG alpha synchronization is related to top-down processing in convergent and divergent thinking. Neuropsychologia, 49, 35053511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Besemer, S. P. & O’Quin, K. (1999). Confirming the three-factor creative product analysis matrix model in an American sample. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 287296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bijvoet-van den Berg, S., & Hoicka, E. (2014). Individual differences and age-related changes in divergent thinking in toddlers and preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 50, 16291639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonnardel, N., & Marmèche, E. (2005). Towards supporting evocation processes in creative design: A cognitive approach. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 63, 422435. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2005.04.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brophy, D. R. (1998). Understanding, measuring and enhancing individual creative problem-solving efforts. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 123150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1102_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2, 119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900000866CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380400. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0040373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cayirdag, N., & Runco, M. A. (2014). Creativity in adulthood. In Gullotta, T. & Bloom, M. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of primary prevention and health promotion (2nd ed., pp. 16111634). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Campion, M. & Levita, L. (2014). Enhancing positive affect and divergent thinking abilities: Play some music and dance. Journal of Positive Psychology, 9, 137145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, Y., Baas, M. & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2018). Creative responses to imminent threats: The role of threat direction and perceived effectiveness? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 74, 174186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chermahini, S. A. & Hommel, B. (2010). The (b) link between creativity and dopamine: Spontaneous eye blink rates predict and dissociate divergent and convergent thinking. Cognition, 115, 458465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chermahini, S. A. & Hommel, B. (2012). Creative mood swings: Divergent and convergent thinking affect mood in opposite ways. Psychological Research, 76, 634640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clapham, M. M. (2004). The convergent validity of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking and creativity interest inventories. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 64, 828841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. J., & Aiken, L. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd edn). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Colzato, L. S., Szapora, A., & Hommel, B. (2012). Meditate to create: The impact of focused-attention and open-monitoring training on convergent and divergent thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colzato, L. S., Szapora, A., Lippelt, D., & Hommel, B. (2017). Prior meditation practice modulates performance and strategy use in convergent-and divergent-thinking problems. Mindfulness, 8, 1016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cousijn, J., Koolschijn, P. C. M., Zanolie, K., Kleibeuker, S. W., & Crone, E. A. (2014). The relation between gray matter morphology and divergent thinking in adolescents and young adults. PloS One, 9(12), e114619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cramond, B. (1993). The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: From design through establishment of predictive validity. In Subotnik, R. F. & Arnold, K. D. (eds.), Beyond Terman: Contemporary longitudinal studies of giftedness and talent (pp. 229254). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Cramond, B., Matthews-Morgan, J., Bandalos, D., & Zuo, L. (2005). A report on the 40-year follow-up of the Torrance tests of creative thinking. Gifted Child Quarterly, 49, 283291. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 001698620504900402CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cronbach, L. J. (1970). Essentials of psychological testing. New York, NY: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 18, 391404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cropley, D. H., Kaufman, J. C., & Cropley, A. J. (2008). Malevolent creativity: A functional model of creativity in terrorism and crime. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 105115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York, NY: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Cundall, M. K. Jr. (2007). Humor and the limits of incongruity. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 203211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/104 00410701397263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damian, R. I. & Simonton, D. K. (2014). Diversifying experiences in the development of genius and their impact on creative cognition. In Simonton, D. K. (ed.), The Wiley handbook of genius (pp. 375394). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dietrich, A., & Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies of creativity and insight. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 822848. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019749CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dimkov, P. R. (2018). The genius of creativity and the creativity of genius: The neuro-dynamics of creativity in Karl Jaspers and Sigmund Freud. Journal of Genius and Eminence, 4, 8392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dudek, S. Z. & Verreault, R. (1989). The creative thinking and ego functioning of children. Creativity Research Journal, 2, 6486. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400418909534301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. (2003). Creativity, personality, and the convergent-divergent continuum. In Runco, M. A. (ed.), Critical creative processes (pp. 95114). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.Google Scholar
Fasko, D. (2001). Education and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 317328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fink, A., Grabner, R. H., Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2006). Divergent thinking training is related to frontal electroencephalogram alpha synchronization. European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 22412246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fink, A., Schwab, D., & Papousek, I. (2011). Sensitivity of EEG upper alpha activity to cognitive and affective creativity interventions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 82, 233239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative cognition: Theory, research, and applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (1990). Massive IQ gains on the Scottish WISC: Evidence against Brand et al.’s hypothesis. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 11, 4151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forthmann, B., Gerwig, A., Holling, H., Çelik, P., Storme, M., & Lubart, T. (2016). The be-creative effect in divergent thinking: The interplay of instruction and object frequency. Intelligence, 57, 2532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnham, A., Crump, J., Batey, M., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2009). Personality and ability predictors of the “consequences” test of divergent thinking in a large non-student sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 536540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Getzels, J. W., & Jackson, P. W. (1962). Creativity and intelligence: Explorations with gifted students. Oxford, England: Wiley.Google Scholar
Gilhooly, K. J., Georgiou, G. J., Garrison, J., Reston, J. D., & Sirota, M. (2012). Don’t wait to incubate: Immediate versus delayed incubation in divergent thinking. Memory and Cognition, 40, 966975.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gino, F., & Ariely, D. (2012). The dark side of creativity: original thinkers can be more dishonest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 445459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gocłowska, M. A. & Crisp, R. J. (2013). On counter-stereotypes and creative cognition: When interventions for reducing prejudice can boost divergent thinking. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 8, 7279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gruber, H. E. (1993). Creativity in the moral domain: Ought implies can implies create. Creativity Research Journal, 6, 315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guilford, J. P. (1968). Creativity, intelligence and their educational implications. San Diego, CA: EDITS/Knapp.Google Scholar
Hao, N., Liu, M., Ku, Y., Hu, Y., & Runco, M. A. (2015). Verbal divergent thinking facilitated by a pleasurable incubation interval. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9, 286295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, D. M. (1975). Effects of explicit instructions to “be creative” on the psychological meaning of divergent thinking test scores. Journal of Personality, 43, 434454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hass, R. W. (2015). Feasibility of online divergent thinking assessment. Computers in Human Behavior, 46, 8593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Fischer, R., & Christoffels, I. K. (2011). Bilingualism and creativity: Benefits in convergent thinking come with losses in divergent thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00273CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, E., O’Neil, H. F., & Peng, Y. (2016). Effects of explicit instructions, metacognition, and motivation on creative performance. Creativity Research Journal, 28, 3345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isaksen, S. G. & Treffinger, D. J. (1985). Creative problem solving: The basic course. New York, NY: Bearly Limited.Google Scholar
Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Dunst, B., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). The relationship between intelligence and creativity: New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence, 41, 212221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jauk, E., Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Tackling creativity at its roots: Evidence for different patterns of EEG alpha activity related to convergent and divergent modes of task processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 84, 219225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeon, K. N., Moon, S. M., & French, B. (2011). Differential effects of divergent thinking, domain knowledge, and interest on creative performance in art and math. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 6071.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johns, G. A. & Morse, L. W. (1997). Divergent thinking as a function of time and prompting to “be creative” in undergraduates. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 31, 156165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karwowski, M., Dul, J., Gralewski, J., Jauk, E., Jankowska, D. M., Gajda, A., Chruszczewski, M. H., & Benedek, M. (2016). Is creativity without intelligence possible? A Necessary Condition analysis. Intelligence, 57, 105117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karwowski, M. & Gralewski, J. (2013). Threshold hypothesis: Fact or artifact? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 8, 2533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, A. N. & Poag, J. R. (1979). Sex differences in instructions to “be creative” on divergent and nondivergent test scores. Journal of Personality, 47, 518530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufmann, G., & Vosburg, S. K. (2002). The effects of mood on early and late idea production. Creativity Research Journal, 14, 317330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kharkhurin, A. V. (2009). The role of bilingualism in creative performance on divergent thinking and invented alien creatures tests. Journal of Creative Behavior, 43, 5971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, D. (2016). Role of cognitive flexibility in bilingualism and creativity. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Georgia.Google Scholar
Kleibeuker, S. W., De Dreu, C. K., & Crone, E. A. (2013). The development of creative cognition across adolescence: Distinct trajectories for insight and divergent thinking. Developmental Science, 16, 212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleibeuker, S. W., Stevenson, C. E., van der Aar, L., Overgaauw, S., van Duijvenvoorde, A. C., & Crone, E. A. (2017). Training in the adolescent brain: An fMRI training study on divergent thinking. Developmental Psychology, 53, 353365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, J. Y., Bain, S. K., & McCallum, R. S. (2007). Improving creative problem-solving in a sample of third culture kids. School Psychology International, 28, 449463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S. A. & Dow, G. T. (2011). Malevolent creativity: Does personality influence malicious divergent thinking? Creativity Research Journal, 23, 7382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, C. & Lovatt, P. J. (2013). Breaking away from set patterns of thinking: Improvisation and divergent thinking. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 9, 4658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lissitz, R. W. & Willhoft, J. L. (1985). A methodological study of the Torrance Tests of Creativity. Journal of Educational Measurement, 22, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKinnon, D. W. (1962). The nature and nurture of creative talent. American Psychologist, 17, 484495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0046541CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madore, K. P., Addis, D. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2015). Creativity and memory: Effects of an episodic-specificity induction on divergent thinking. Psychological Science, 26, 14611468. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615591863CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madore, K. P., Jing, H. G., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Divergent creative thinking in young and older adults: Extending the effects of an episodic specificity induction. Memory and Cognition, 44, 974988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martindale, C. (1973). COUNT: A PL/I program for content analysis of natural language. Behavioral Science, 18, 148.Google Scholar
Maslow, A. H. (1976). The farther reaches of human nature. New York, NY: Penguin.Google Scholar
Massimiliano, P. (2015). The effects of age on divergent thinking and creative objects production: A cross-sectional study. High Ability Studies, 26, 93104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
May, R. (1984). The courage to create. New York, NY: Bantam Books.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 12581265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaren, R. B. (1993). The dark side of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 6, 137144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMullan, W. E. (1976). Creative individuals: Paradoxical personages. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 10, 265275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/j.2162–6057.1976.tb00148.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mednick, S. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milgram, R. M., & Milgram, N. (1976). Creative thinking and creative performance in Israeli students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 255258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milgram, R. M., & Rabkin, L. (1980). Developmental test of Mednick’s associative hierarchies of original thinking. Developmental Psychology, 16, 157158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, M., Runco, M. A., Acar, S., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2013). Reanalysis of genetic data and rethinking dopamine’s relationship with creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 25, 147148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ni, M., Yang, L., Chen, J., Chen, H., & Li, X. (2014). How to improve divergent thinking capability by information technology and extenics. Procedia Computer Science, 31, 158164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okuda, S. M., Runco, M. A., & Berger, D. E. (1991). Creativity and the finding and solving of real-world problems. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 9, 4553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osborn, A. (1953). Applied imagination. New York, NY: Charles Scribners Sons.Google Scholar
Palmiero, M., Di Giacomo, D., & Passafiume, D. (2014). Divergent thinking and age-related changes. Creativity Research Journal, 26, 456460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parnes, S. J. (1992). Source book for creative problem solvingBuffalo, NY: Creative Foundation.Google Scholar
Parnes, S. J., & Noller, R. B. (1972). Applied creativity: The creative studies project. Part II: Result of the two year program. Journal of Creative Behavior, 6, 164186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patrick, C. (1935). Creative thought in poets. Archives of Psychology, 26, 1 74.Google Scholar
Patrick, C. (1937). Creative thought in artists. Journal of Psychology, 5, 3573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patrick, C. (1941). Whole and part relationship in creative thought. American Journal of Psychology, 54, 128 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paulus, P. B., Kohn, N. W., & Arditti, L. E. (2011). Effects of quantity and quality instructions on brainstorming. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 45, 3846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plucker, J. A. (2001). Introduction to the Special Issue: Commemorating Guilford. Creativity Research Journal, 13(3–4), 247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15326934CRJ1334_02CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plucker, J. A., Qian, M., & Schmalensee, S. L. (2014). Is what you see what you really get? Comparison of scoring techniques in the assessment of real-world divergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 26, 135143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preckel, F., Wermer, C., & Spinath, F. M. (2011). The interrelationship between speeded and unspeeded divergent thinking and reasoning, and the role of mental speed. Intelligence, 39, 378388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puccio, G. J., Burnett, C., Acar, S., Yudess, J. A., Holinger, M., & Cabra, J. F. (2018). Creative problem-solving in small groups: the effects of creativity training on idea generation, solution creativity, and leadership effectiveness. The Journal of Creative Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puccio, G. J., Murdock, M. C., & Mance, M. (2011). Creative leadership: Skills that drive change (2nd edn). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Rawlings, D., & Toogood, A. (1997). Using a “taboo response” measure to examine the relationship between divergent thinking and psychoticism. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 6168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reuter, M., Roth, S., Holve, K., & Hennig, J. (2006). Identification of first candidate genes for creativity: A pilot study. Brain Research, 1069, 190197. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.04CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhodes, M. (1961). An analysis of creativity. Phi Delta Kappan, 42, 305310.Google Scholar
Ricciardelli, L. A. (1992). Creativity and bilingualism. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 26, 242254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, A. G. (1986). Two factors of creativity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 63, 379384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ritter, S. M., Damian, R. I., Simonton, D. K., van Baaren, R. B., Strick, M., Derks, J., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2012). Diversifying experiences enhance cognitive flexibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 961964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2007). A hierarchical framework for the study of creativity. New Horizons in Education, 55, 19.Google Scholar
Runco, M A. (1986). Flexibility and originality in children’s divergent thinking. Journal of Psychology, 120, 345352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1989). The creativity of children's art. Child Study Journal, 19, 177189.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1991). The evaluative, valuative, and divergent thinking of children. Journal of Creative Behavior, 25, 311319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1993). Moral creativity: Intentional and unconventional. Creativity Research Journal, 6, 1728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1996). Objectivity in creativity research. In Montuori, A. (ed.), Unusual associates: A festschrift for Frank Barron (pp. 6979). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1999). Divergent thinking. In Runco, M. A. & Pritzker, S. R. (eds.). Encyclopedia of creativity (Vol. 1, pp. 577582). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2016). Commentary: Overview of developmental perspectives on creativity and realization of potential. In Barbot, B. (ed.), Perspectives on creativity development. New directions for child and adolescent development (Vol. 151, pp. 97109). San Francisco, CAJossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (ed.). (1994). Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., Abdulla, A. M., & Paek, S.-H. (2016). Which test of divergent thinking is best? Creativity: Theories–Research–Applications, 3, 418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Acar, S. (2010). Do tests of divergent thinking have an experiential bias? Psychology of Art, Creativity, and Aesthetics, 4, 144–148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018969CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Albert, R. S. (1985). The reliability and validity of ideational originality in the divergent thinking of academically gifted and nongifted children. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 45, 483501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. & Basadur, M. (1993). Assessing ideational and evaluative skills and creative styles and attitudes. Creativity and Innovation Management, 2, 166173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Chand, I. (1995). Cognition and creativity. Educational Psychology Review, 7, 243267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Charles, R. E. (1993). Judgments of originality and appropriateness as predictors of creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 537546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Charles, R. (1997). Developmental trends in creativity. In Runco, M. A. (Ed.), Creativity research handbook (vol. 1, pp. 113-150). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., Illies, J. J., & Eisenman, R. (2005). Creativity, originality, and appropriateness: What do explicit instructions tell us about their relationships? Journal of Creative Behavior, 39, 137148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2017.1302758CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., Illies, J. J., & Reiter-Ralmon, R. (2005). Explicit instructions to be creative and original. The International Journal of Creativity & Problem Solving, 15, 515.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. & Jaeger, G. (2012). The standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 9296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., McCarthy, K. A., & Svensen, E. (1994). Judgments of the creativity of artwork from students and professional artists. Journal of Psychology, 128, 2331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Mraz, W. (1992). Scoring divergent thinking tests using total ideational output and a creativity index. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52, 213221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., Noble, E. P, Reiter-Palmon, R., Acar, S., Ritchie, T., & Yurkovich, J. M. (2011). The genetic basis of creativity and ideational fluency. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 376380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., Okuda, S. M., & Thurston, B. J. (1987). The psychometric properties of four systems for scoring divergent thinking tests. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 5, 149156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. & Smith, W. R. (1992). Interpersonal and intrapersonal evaluations of creative ideas. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 295302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. & Vega, L. (1990). Evaluating the creativity of children’s ideas. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 5, 439452.Google Scholar
Sackett, P. R., Zedeck, S., & Fogli, L. (1988). Relations between measures of typical and maximum job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 482486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwind, C., Buder, J., Cress, U., & Hesse, F. W. (2012). Preference-inconsistent recommendations: An effective approach for reducing confirmation bias and stimulating divergent thinking? Computers and Education, 58, 787796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, G., Leritz, L. E., & Mumford, M. D. (2004a). The effectiveness of creativity training: A quantitative review. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 361388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, G., Leritz, L. E., & Mumford, M. D. (2004b). Types of creativity training: Approaches and their effectiveness. Journal of Creative Behavior, 38, 149179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shi, B., Qian, M., Lu, Y., Plucker, J. A., & Lin, C. (2012). The relationship between migration and Chinese children’s divergent thinking. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 106111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Martin, C., & Nusbaum, E. C. (2009). A snapshot of creativity: Evaluating a quick and simple method for assessing divergent thinking. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 4, 7985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, A. & Bock, O. (2016). Influence of divergent and convergent thinking on visuomotor adaptation in young and older adults. Human Movement Science, 46, 2329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonton, D. K. (2007). The creative process in Picasso’s Guernica sketches: Monotonic improvements versus nonmonotonic variants. Creativity Research Journal, 19(4), 329344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, G. F. (1998). Idea generation techniques: A formulary of active ingredients. Journal of Creative Behavior, 32, 107134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sowden, P. T., Clements, L., Redlich, C., & Lewis, C. (2015). Improvisation facilitates divergent thinking and creativity: Realizing a benefit of primary school arts education. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9, 128138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, J., Chen, Q., Zhang, Q., Li, Y., Li, H., Wei, D., … & Qiu, J. (2016). Training your brain to be more creative: Brain functional and structural changes induced by divergent thinking training. Human Brain Mapping, 37, 33753387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sviderskaya, N. E. (2011). The EEG spatial pattern and psychophysiological characteristics of divergent and convergent thinking in humans. Human Physiology, 37, 3138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takeuchi, H., Tomita, H., Taki, Y., Kikuchi, Y., Ono, C., Yu, Z., & Miyauchi, C. M. (2015). The associations among the dopamine D2 receptor Taq1, emotional intelligence, creative potential measured by divergent thinking, and motivational state and these associations’ sex differences. Frontiers in Psychology, 6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torrance, E. P. (1962). Guiding creative talent. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1966). The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: Norms—Technical manual (research edition). Princeton, NJ: Personnel Press.Google Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1968). A longitudinal examination of the fourth grade slump in creativity. Gifted Child Quarterly, 12, 195199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1974). The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking-Norms-Technical Manual Research Edition-Verbal Tests, Forms A and B- Figural Tests, Forms A and B. Princeton, NJ: Personnel Press.Google Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1995). Why fly: A philosophy of creativity. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (2008). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking Norms-Technical Manual, Verbal (Streamlined) Forms A & B. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing.Google Scholar
Torrance, E. P. & Hall, L. K. (1980). Assessing the further reaches of creative potential. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 14, 119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/j.2162–6057.1980.tb00220.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torrance, E. P. & Safter, H. T. (1989). The long range predictive validity of the Just Suppose Test. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 23, 219223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11, 138146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van de Kamp, M. T., Admiraal, W., van Drie, J. & Rijlaarsdam, G. (2015). Enhancing divergent thinking in visual arts education: Effects of explicit instruction of meta-cognition. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 4758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viriyayudhakorn, K., Kunifuji, S., & Ogawa, M. (2011). A comparison of four association engines in divergent thinking support systems on Wikipedia. In Knowledge, information, and creativity support systems (pp.226237). Heidelberg: Springer Berlin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walczyk, J. J., Runco, M. A., Tripp, S. M., & Smith, C. E. (2008). The creativity of lying: Divergent thinking and ideational correlates of the resolution of social dilemmas. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 328342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, B. R. & Jackson, C. J. (2014). How the five-factor model and revised reinforcement sensitivity theory predict divergent thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 57, 5458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, C. E. & Russ, S. W. (2015). Pretend play, divergent thinking, and math achievement in girls: A longitudinal study. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9, 296305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallach, M. A.Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children: A study of the creativity-intelligence distinction. Oxford: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York, NY: Harcourt.Google Scholar
Ward, W. C. (1969). Creativity and environmental cues in nursery school children. Developmental Psychology, 1, 543547. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0027977CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, S. & Graves, R. E. (2002). Are creativity and schizotypy products of a right hemisphere bias? Brain and Cognition, 49, 138151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisberg, R. W. & Hass, R. (2007). We are all partly right: Comment on Simonton. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 345360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welling, H. (2007). Four mental operations in creative cognition: The importance of abstraction. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 163177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, K. M. & Mieg, H. A. (2010). Cognitive determinants of the success of inventors: Complex problem solving and deliberate use of divergent and convergent thinking. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22, 443462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamada, Y. & Nagai, M. (2015). Positive mood enhances divergent but not convergent thinking. Japanese Psychological Research, 57, 281287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, C & Li, X. (2012). Research progress in extension innovation method and its applications. Industrial Engineering Journal, 15, 131137.Google Scholar
Yi, X., Plucker, J. A., & Guo, J. (2015). Modeling influences on divergent thinking and artistic creativity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 16, 6268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoruk, S. & Runco, M. A. (2014). The neuroscience of divergent thinking. Activitas Nervosa Superior, 56, 116. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03379602Google Scholar
Yu, Q., Zhang, S., & Zhang, J. H. (2017). Association of dopamine D2 receptor gene with creative ideation. Creativity Research Journal, 29, 108113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziv, A. (1976). Facilitating effects of humor on creativity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 318322. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022- 0663.68.3.318CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×