Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-m8s7h Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-21T10:56:20.965Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Data interrelations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2014

R. S. Clymo
Affiliation:
Queen Mary University of London
Get access

Summary

Interpreting P values

Suppose you have just finished the measurements in a simple experiment to compare the abundance of weeds on 1-m2 plots sprayed either with a weedkiller in solution or with the same volume of water alone. You adopt the null hypothesis: that the weedkiller has no effect, and make a suitable test of this hypothesis (t-test or U-test, as appropriate) obtaining the result P = 0.04. That is to say that, if the hypothesis is true, then in a large number of similar trials one would expect to have got mean values at least as different as these were in about 4% of trials. The statistics stops at this point. So what next?

Anything further is a judgement by you and your readers. A convention has grown that for routine work of no special importance, if P ≤ 0.05 then, as practical people we think we will make fastest progress by concluding provisionally that the hypothesis is false while accepting that on average 1 in 20 of these provisional conclusions will be incorrect. But the value 0.05 is arbitrary.

  • If we had only four digits on each hand it seems likely that the value would be 3 / 82 (= 0.047 in base 10) rather than 5 / 102 = 0.050.

  • If the experiments are important and cannot be repeated for some reason (expense, no more material, unavailable equipment) then you may argue that you are going to reject the hypothesis provisionally with P as high as 0.1 (for example).

  • If your life depends on avoiding wrongly rejecting the hypothesis then you might be inclined to require that P be ≪ 0.000 000 1.

  • And if your life depends on avoiding wrongly rejecting the hypothesis yet there is a reward of 1 000 000 €/£/$ for correctly rejecting it, then you might be inclined to gamble at P < 0.000 1 or even P < 0.001. This is the sort of subconscious judgement you make every time you get into a car: the weighing of advantages and risks of an adverse outcome.

Type
Chapter
Information
Reporting Research
A Biologist's Guide to Articles, Talks, and Posters
, pp. 243 - 260
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2014

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Data interrelations
  • R. S. Clymo, Queen Mary University of London
  • Book: Reporting Research
  • Online publication: 05 October 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107284234.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Data interrelations
  • R. S. Clymo, Queen Mary University of London
  • Book: Reporting Research
  • Online publication: 05 October 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107284234.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Data interrelations
  • R. S. Clymo, Queen Mary University of London
  • Book: Reporting Research
  • Online publication: 05 October 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107284234.012
Available formats
×