Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T16:24:48.386Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Culture and the Development of Regulatory Competence: Chinese–U.S. Comparisons

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 July 2009

Sheryl L. Olson
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Arnold J. Sameroff
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Get access

Summary

One of the most stunning demonstrations of differences in behavior regulation across cultures can be found by simply walking into preschool or early elementary classrooms in Chinese societies (including Hong Kong, Taiwan, and Singapore) versus those in the United States. Before even making it to the center of the classroom, one is immediately struck by the differences – found in classroom after classroom, school after school – in the appearance of the “cubbies” and other entry areas. In both Chinese and American preschools, children have cubbies in which they can place their personal belongings (school bags and the like) and outdoor clothing. In both, these cubbies tend to be arranged as a line of boxes or hooks on a wall near the entrance to the room. In both, there are names and pictures of the children on the cubbies in addition to their belongings. When empty, they are almost identical.

But from the minute the first child arrives at school, the differences begin to appear, and they magnify with each additional child who comes into the classroom. What is the difference? In the few seconds (this rarely lasts even as long as a minute in either place) between parents dropping off their child and the child entering into the classroom proper, children's belongings get placed in the cubby.

Type
Chapter

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 49(1), 5–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829–839.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauer, A. M., Quas, J. A., & Boyce, W. T. (2002). Associations between physiological reactivity and children's behavior: Advantages of a multisystem approach. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 23(2), 102–113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumrind, D. (1966). Effects of authoritative control on child behavior. Child Development, 37(4), 887–907.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumrind, D. (1968). Authoritarian vs. authoritative parental control. Adolescence, 3(11), 255–272.Google Scholar
Begley, T. M., Lee, C., & Hui, C. (2006). Organizational level as a moderator of the relationship between justice perceptions and work-related reactions. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27(6), 705–721.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, C., Granger, D., & Razza, R. P. (2005). Cortisol reactivity is positively related to executive function in preschool children attending Head Start. Child Development, 76(3), 554–567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, K. A., Goldsmith, H. H., & Davidson, R. J. (2005). Cardiac reactivity is associated with changes in negative emotion in 24-month-olds. Developmental Psychobiology, 46(2), 118–132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calkins, S. D. (1997). Cardiac vagal tone indices of temperamental reactivity and behavioral regulation in young children. Developmental Psychobiology, 31(2), 125–135.3.0.CO;2-M>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. (2002). Self-regulatory processes in early personality development: A multilevel approach to the study of childhood social withdrawal and aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 477–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos, J. J., Frankel, C. B., & Camras, L. (2004). On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Development, 75(2), 377–394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camras, L. A., Bakeman, R., Chen, Y., Norris, K., & Cain, T. R. (2006). Culture, ethnicity, and children's facial expressions: A study of European American, mainland Chinese, Chinese American, and adopted Chinese girls. Emotion, 6(1), 103–114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camras, L. A., Meng, Z., Ujiie, T., Dharamsi, S., Miyake, K., Oster, H., et al. (2002). Observing emotion in infants: Facial expression, body behavior, and rater judgments of responses to an expectancy-violating event. Emotion, 2(2), 179–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camras, L. A., Oster, H., Campos, J., Campos, R., Ujiie, T., Miyake, K., et al. (1998). Production of emotional facial expressions in European American, Japanese, and Chinese infants. Developmental Psychology, 34(4), 616–628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camras, L. A., Oster, H., Campos, J. J., Bakeman, R., Ekman, P., Campos, J. J., et al. (2003). Emotional facial expressions in European-American, Japanese, and Chinese infants. In Emotions inside out: 130 years after Darwin's: The expression of the emotions in man and animals (pp. 135–151). New York: New YorkUniversity Press.Google Scholar
Camras, L. A., Oster, H., Campos, J. J., & Miyake, K. (1992). Japanese and American infants' responses to arm restraint. Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 578–583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1105–1122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11(2), 73–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., Elder, G. H., & Bem, D. J. (1988). Moving away from the world: Life-course patterns of shy children. Developmental Psychology, 24(6), 824–831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caudill, W., & Weinstein, H. (1969). Maternal care and infant behavior in Japan and America. Psychiatry, 32, 12–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, L., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., & McBride-Chang, C. (2003). Harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation and aggression. Journal of Family Psychology, 17(4), 598–606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training. Child Development, 65(4), 1111–1119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chao, R. K. (2001). Extending research on the consequences of parenting style for Chinese Americans and European Americans. Child Development, 72(6), 1832–1843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chao, R. K., & Kim, K. (2000). Parenting differences among immigrant Chinese fathers and mothers in the United States. Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 1(1), 71–91.Google Scholar
Chao, R. K., Taylor, R. D., & Wang, M. C. (2000). Cultural explanations for the role of parenting in the school success of Asian-American children. In Wang, M. C. & Taylor, R. D. (Eds.), Resilience across contexts: Family, work, culture, and community (pp. 333–363). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chen, C., & Stevenson, H. W. (1995). Motivation and mathematics achievement: A comparative study of Asian-American, Caucasian-American, and East Asian high school students. Child Development, 66(4), 1215–1234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Cen, G., Li, D., & He, Y. (2005). Social functioning and adjustment in Chinese children: The imprint of historical time. Child Development, 76(1), 182–195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, X., Chen, H., Wang, L., & Liu, M. (2002). Noncompliance and child-rearing attitudes as predictors of aggressive behavior: A longitudinal study in Chinese children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(3), 225–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., & Columbus, F. (2000). Social and emotional development in Chinese children and adolescents: A contextual cross-cultural perspective. In Columbus, F. (Ed.), Advances in psychology research, Vol. 1 (pp. 229–251). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., & Stewart, S. L. (1998). Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 34(4), 677–686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, X., He, Y., Oliveira, A. M., Lo Coco, A., Zappulla, C., Kaspar, V., et al. (2004). Loneliness and social adaptation in Brazilian, Canadian, Chinese and Italian children: A multi-national comparative study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(8), 1373–1384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, X., He, Y., & Li, D. (2004). Self-perceptions of social competence and self-worth in Chinese children: Relations with social and school performance. Social Development, 13(4), 570–589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Liu, M., Li, B., Cen, G., Chen, H., & Wang, L. (2000). Maternal authoritative and authoritarian attitudes and mother-child interactions and relationships in urban China. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24(1), 119–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., & Li, B. (1995a). Social and school adjustment of shy and aggressive children in China. Development and Psychopathology, 7(2), 337–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., & Li, Z.-y. (1995b). Social functioning and adjustment in Chinese children: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 31(4), 531–539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., & Li, B.-S. (1997). Maternal acceptance and social and school adjustment in Chinese children: A four-year longitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 43(4), 663–681.Google Scholar
Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., Li, B.-S., & Li, D. (1999). Adolescent outcomes of social functioning in Chinese children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23(1), 199–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., Liu, M., Chen, H., Wang, L., Li, D., et al. (2003). Compliance in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(5), 428–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Swartzman, L. C., Kazarian, S. S., & Evans, D. R. (2001). Health beliefs and experiences in Asian cultures. In Kazarian, S. S. & Evans, D. R. (Eds.), Handbook of cultural health psychology (pp. 389–410). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Wang, L., Chen, H., & Liu, M. (2002). Noncompliance and child-rearing attitudes as predictors of aggressive behavior: A longitudinal study in Chinese children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(3), 225–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Wang, L., & DeSouza, A. (2006). Temperament, socio-emotional functioning and peer relationships in Chinese and North American children. In Chen, D. F. X. & Schneider, B. (Eds.), Peer relations in cultural context (pp. 123–147). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Zappulla, C., Coco, A. L., Schneider, B., Kaspar, V., Oliveira, A. M., et al. (2004). Self-perceptions of competence in Brazilian, Canadian, Chinese and Italian children: Relations with social and school adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28(2), 129–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chia, R. C., Wuensch, K. L., Childers, J., & Chuang, C. J. (1994). A comparison of family values among Chinese, Mexican, and American college students. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 9(2), 249–258.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Ackerman, B. P., & Izard, C. E. (1995). Emotions and emotion regulation in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 7(1), 1–1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97(3), 332–361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, L. L., Bernard, R. S., McClellan, C. B., Piazza-Waggoner, C., Taylor, B. K., & MacLaren, J. E. (2006). Topical anesthesia versus distraction for infants' immunization distress: Evaluation with 6-month follow-up. Children's Health Care, 35(2), 103–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Children Development, 75(2), 318–333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, P. M., Teti, L. O., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2003). Mutual emotion regulation and the stability of conduct problems between preschool and early school age. Development and Psychopathology, 15(1), 1–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crystal, D. S., Chen, C., Fuligni, A. J., & Stevenson, H. W. (1994). Psychological maladjustment and academic achievement: A cross-cultural study of Japanese, Chinese, and American high school students. Child Development, 65(3), 738–753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crystal, D. S., & Stevenson, H. W. (1991). Mothers' perceptions of children's problems with mathematics: A cross-national comparison. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(3), 372–376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037–2078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deater-Deckard, K., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Externalizing behavior problems and discipline revisited: Nonlinear effects and variation by culture, context, and gender. Psychological Inquiry, 8(3), 161–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dettling, A. C., Gunnar, M. R., & Donzella, B. (1999). Cortisol levels of young children in full-day child care centers: Relations with age and temperament. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 24, 519–536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dettling, A. C., Parker, S. W., Lane, S. K., Sebanc, A., & Gunnar, M. R. (2000). Quality of care and temperament determine whether cortisol levels rise over the day for children in full-day care. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 25, 819–836.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A. (2005). Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (with hyperactivity). Development and Psychopathology, 17(3), 807–825.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A., Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I. M. (2006). The early development of executive functions. In Bialystok, E. & Craik, F. I. M. (Eds.), Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change (pp. 70–95). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A., Gibson, K. R., & Petersen, A. C. (1991). Frontal lobe involvement in cognitive changes during the first year of life. In Gibson, K. R. & Petersen, A. C. (Eds.), Brain maturation and cognitive development: Comparative and cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 127–180). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Diamond, A., & Gilbert, J. (1989). Development as progressive inhibitory control of action: Retrieval of a contiguous object. Cognitive Development, 4(3), 223–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A., Stuss, D. T., & Knight, R. T. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In Stuss, D. T. & Knight, R. T. (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 466–503). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donzella, B., Gunnar, M. R., Krueger, W. K., & Alwin, J. (2000). Cortisol and vagal tone responses to competitive challenge in preschoolers: Associations with temperament. Developmental Psychology, 37(4), 209–220.3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eid, M., & Diener, E. (2001). Norms for experiencing emotions in different cultures: Inter- and intranational differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 869–885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., & Reiser, M. (2000). Dispositional emotionality and regulation: Their role in predicting quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 78(1), 136–157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., & Fabes, R. A. (1997). The relations of regulation and emotionality to resiliency and competence social functioning in elementary school children. Child Development, 68(2), 295–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., & Liew, J. (2005). Relations among positive parenting, children's effortful control, and externalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 76(5), 1055–1071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felt, B. T., Mollen, E., Diaz, S., Renaud, E., Zeglis, M., Wheatcroft, G., et al. (2000). Behavioral interventions reduce infant distress at immunization. Archives of Pediatric & Adolescent Medicine, 154, 719–724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flynn, E., O'Malley, C., & Wood, D. (2004). A longitudinal, microgenetic study of the emergence of false belief understanding and inhibition skills. Developmental Science, 7(1), 103–115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (1993). Pathways to aggression and social withdrawal: Interactions among temperament, attachment, and regulation. In Asendorpf, K. H. R. J. B. (Ed.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood (pp. 81–100). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fox, N. A., & Henderson, H. A. (1999). Does infancy matter? Predicting social behavior from infant temperament. Infant Behavior & Development, 22(4), 445–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N. A., Polak, C. P., DelCarmen-Wiggins, R., & Carter, A. (2004). The role of sensory reactivity in understanding infant temperament. In DelCarmen-Wiggins, R. (Ed.), Handbook of infant, toddler, and preschool mental health assessment (pp. 105–119). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fox, N. A., & Reznick, J. S. (1989). Heart-rate variability and behavioral reactivity: Individual differences in autonomic patterning and their relation to infant and child temperament. In Reznick, J. S. (Ed.), Perspectives on behavioral inhibition (pp. 177–195). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Freedman, D. G., & Freedman, N. A. (1969). Differences in behavior between Chinese-American and European-American newborns. Nature, 224, 1227.Google ScholarPubMed
Garrett-Peters, P. T., & Fox, N. A. (2007). Cross-cultural differences in children's emotional reactions to a disappointing situation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31(2), 161–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman-Rakic, P. S., Grafman, J., Holyoak, K. J., & Boller, F. (1995). Architecture of the prefrontal cortex and the central executive. In Grafman, J., Holyoak, K. J., & Boller, F. (Eds.), Structure and functions of the human prefrontal cortex (pp. 71–83). New York: New YorkAcademy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., Bates, J. E., & Wachs, T. D. (1994). Psychoendocrine studies of temperament and stress in early childhood: Expanding current models. In Bates, J. E. & Wachs, T. D. (Eds.), Temperament: Individual differences at the interface of biology and behavior (pp. 175–198). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., & Nelson, C. A. (1994). Event-related potentials in year-old infants: Relations with emotionality and cortisol. Child Development, 65(1), 80–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., Tout, K., Haan, M., Pierce, S., & Stansbury, K. (1997). Temperament, social competence, and adrenocortical activity in preschoolers. Developmental Psychobiology, 31(1), 65–85.3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harkness, S., Super, C. M., Rubin, K. H., & Chung, O. B. (2006). Themes and variations: Parental ethnotheories in Western cultures. In Rubin, K. H. (Ed.), Parenting beliefs, behaviors, and parent-child relations: A cross-cultural perspective (pp. 61–79). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Ho, D. Y. F., & Bond, M. H. (1986). Chinese patterns of socialization: A critical review. In Soseki, N., Bourdaghs, M., Ueda, A., & Murphy, J. (Eds.), The psychology of the Chinese people (pp. 1–37). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hong, Y.-y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-Y., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55(7), 709–720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., Arcus, D., Snidman, N., & Feng, W. Y. (1994). Reactivity in infants: A cross-national comparison. Developmental Psychology, 30(3), 342–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., Fox, N. A., Eisenberg, N., Damon, W., & Lerner, R. M. (2006). Biology, culture, and temperamental biases. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) and Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 167–225). New York: John Wiley & Sons Inc.Google Scholar
Kagan, J., Kearsley, R. B., & Zelazo, P. R. (1978). Infancy: Its place in human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. S. (1989). The concept of behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. In Reznick, J. S. (Ed.), Perspectives on behavioral inhibition (pp. 1–23). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Keller, H., Abels, M., Lamm, B., Yovsi, R. D., Voelker, S., & Lakhani, A. (2005). Ecocultural effects on early infant care: A study in Cameroon, India, and Germany. Ethos, 33(4), 512–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., Lamm, B., Abels, M., Yovsi, R., Borke, J. R., Jensen, H., et al. (2006). Cultural models, socialization goals, and parenting ethnotheories: A multicultural analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37(2), 155–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerr, M., Lambert, W. W., & Bem, D. J. (1996). Life course sequelae of childhood shyness in Sweden: Comparison with the United States. Developmental Psychology, 32(6), 1100–1105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessen, W. (1975). Childhood in China. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M. J., Lee, K., Muir, D. W., Xu, F., Fu, G., et al. (1998). The still-face effect in Chinese and Canadian 3- to 6-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 34(4), 629–639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Kurokawa, M. (2000). Culture, emotion, and well-being: Good feelings in Japan and the United States. Cognition & Emotion, 14(1), 93–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleinman, A., McHugh, S., & Vallis, T. M. (1986). Illness meanings and illness behavior. In McHugh, S. & Vallis, T. M. (Eds.), Illness behavior: A multidisciplinary model (pp. 149–160). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kloo, D., & Perner, J. (2003). Training transfer between card sorting and false belief understanding: Helping children apply conflicting descriptions. Child Development, 74(6), 1823–1839.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Development, 64(2), 325–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohnstamm, G. A., Halverson, C. F., Mervielde, I., & Havill, V. L. (1998). Parental descriptions of child personality: Developmental antecedents of the Big Five?Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view. Developmental Psychology, 25(3), 343–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kumakiri, C., Kodama, K., Shimizu, E., Yamanouchi, N., Okada, S.-I., Noda, S., et al. (1999). Study of the association between serotonin transporter gene regulatory region polymorphism and personality traits in a Japanese population. Neuroscience Letters, 263, 205–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lansford, J. E., Chang, L., Dodge, K. A., Malone, P. S., Oburu, P., Palmerus, K., et al. (2005). Physical discipline and children's adjustment: Cultural normativeness as a moderator. Child Development, 76(6), 1234–1246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, B. A. (1999). Pain management in neonates. Acta Paediatrica, 88, 1301–1310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leung, J. P. (1998). Emotions and mental health in Chinese people. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 7(2), 115–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levenson, R. W. (2003). Blood, sweat, and fears: The autonomic architecture of emotion. In Ekman, P., Campos, J. J., Davidson, R. J., & Waal, F. B. M. (Eds.), Emotions inside out: 130 years after Darwin: The expression of the emotions in man and animals (pp. 348–366). New York: New YorkUniversity Press.Google Scholar
LeVine, R. A. (1974). Parental goals: A cross-cultural view. Teachers College Record, 76(2), 226–239.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2002). Cortisol response to embarrassment and shame. Child Development, 73(4), 1034–1045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, M., Ramsay, D. S., & Kawakami, K. (1993). Differences between Japanese infants and Caucasian American infants in behavioral and cortisol response to inoculation. Child Development, 64(6), 1722–1731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lezak, M. D. (1993). Newer contributions to the neuropsychological assessment of executive functions. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 8(1), 24–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, J. (2001). Chinese conceptualization of learning. Ethos, 29(2), 111–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieber, E., Fung, H., & Leung, P. W.-L. (2006). Chinese child-rearing beliefs: Key dimensions and contributions to the development of culture-appropriate assessment. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 9(2), 140–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lim, S.-L., & Lim, B. K. (2004). Parenting style and child outcomes in Chinese and immigrant Chinese families: Current findings and cross-cultural considerations in conceptualization and research. Marriage & Family Review, 35(3), 21–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, C.-Y. C., & Fu, V. R. (1990). A comparison of child-rearing practices among Chinese, immigrant Chinese, and Caucasian-American parents. Child Development, 61(2), 429–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, K. M. (1981). Traditional Chinese medical beliefs and their relevance for mental illness and psychiatry. In Kleinman, A. & Lin, T. Y. (Eds.), Normal and abnormal behavior in Chinese culture (pp. 95–11). Dordrecht: D. Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, M., Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., Zheng, S., Cui, L., Li, D., et al. (2005). Autonomy- vs. connectedness-oriented parenting behaviors in Chinese and Canadian mothers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(6), 489–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez, N. L. (2006). Individual differences in the activation and regulation of the limbic-hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis after stress: Implications for emotion regulation research. Ann Arbor, MI: ProQuest Information & Learning.Google Scholar
Lyon, G. R., & Krasnegor, N. A. (1996). Attention, memory, and executive function. Baltimore: Paul H Brookes.Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mauss, I. B., Levenson, R. W., McCarter, L., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The tie that binds?Coherence among emotion experience, behavior, and physiology. Emotion, 5(2), 175–190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. G. (2002). Bringing culture to basic psychological theory – Beyond individualism and collectivism: Comment on Oyserman et al. (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128(1), 97–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2001). Childhood predictors differentiate life-course persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways among males and females. Development and Psychopathology, 13(2), 355–375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakamura, M., Ueno, S., Sano, A., & Tanabe, H. (2000). The human serotonin transporter gene linked polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) shows ten novel allelic variants. Molecular Psychiatry, 5(1), 32–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, D. A., Hart, C. H., Yang, C., Olsen, J. A., & Jin, S. (2006). Aversive parenting in China: Associations with child physical and relational aggression. Child Development, 77(3), 554–572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T. (2001). Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder?Psychological Bulletin, 127(5), 571–598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, S. L., Kashiwagi, K., & Crystal, D. (2001). Concepts of adaptive and maladaptive child behavior: A comparison of U. S. and Japanese mothers of preschool-age children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(1), 43–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, S. L., Sameroff, A. J., Kerr, D. C. R., Lopez, N. L., & Wellman, H. M. (2005). Developmental foundations of externalizing problems in young children: The role of effortful control. Development and Psychopathology, 17(1), 25–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, S. L., Schilling, E. M., & Bates, J. E. (1999). Measurement of impulsivity: Construct coherence, longitudinal stability, and relationship with externalizing problems in middle childhood and adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27(2), 151–165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pan, Y. (1995). Power behind linguistic behavior: Analysis of politeness phenomena in Chinese official settings. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 14(4), 462–481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12(3), 427–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potter, S. H. (1988). The cultural construction of emotion in rural Chinese social life. Ethos, 16, 181–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quas, J., Hong, M., Alkon, A., & Boyce, W. T. (2000). Dissociations between psychobiologic reactivity and emotional expression in children. Developmental Psychobiology, 37(3), 153–175.3.0.CO;2-Y>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raver, C. C. (2004). Placing emotional self-regulation in sociocultural and socioeconomic contexts. Child Development, 75(2), 346–353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rickman, M. D., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). Personality and behavior in parents of temperamentally inhibited and uninhibited children. Developmental Psychology, 30(3), 346–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ridderinkhof, K. R., Molen, M. W., Band, G. P. H., & Bashore, T. R. (1997). Sources of interference from irrelevant information: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65(3), 315–341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothbart, M. K., Bates, J. E., Damon, W., & Eisenberg, N. (1998). Temperament. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 105–176). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Bates, J. E., Eisenberg, N., Damon, W., & Lerner, R. M. (2006). Temperament. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 99–166). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Kohnstamm, G. A., Bates, J. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (1989). Temperament in childhood: A framework. In Kohnstamm, G. A., Bates, J. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 59–73). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., Kieras, J., McCartney, K., & Phillips, D. (2006). Temperament, attention, and the development of self-regulation. In McCartney, K. & Phillips, D. (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of early childhood development (pp. 338–357). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, K. H., Asendorpf, J. B., Rubin, K. H., & Asendorpf, J. B. (1993). Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood: Conceptual and definitional issues. In Rubin, K. H. & Asendorph, J. B. (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood (pp. 3–17). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (1995). Emotionality, emotion regulation, and preschoolers' social adaptation. Development and Psychopathology, 7(1), 49–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, K. H., Hemphill, S. A., Chen, X., Hastings, P., Sanson, A., LoCoco, A., et al. (2006). Parenting beliefs and behaviors: Initial findings from the International Consortium for the Study of Social and Emotional Development (ICSSED). In Rubin, K. H. (Ed.), Parenting beliefs, behaviors, and parent-child relations: A cross-cultural perspective (pp. 81–103). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2005). The development of executive attention: Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 573–594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2004). Attentional control and self-regulation. In Baumeister, R. F. & Vohs, K. D. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 283–300). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., Yik, M. S. M., & Bond, M. H. (1996). Emotion among the Chinese. In Bond, M. H. (Ed.), The handbook of Chinese psychology (pp. 166–188). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2006). Gene-environment interplay and psychopathology: Multiple varieties but real effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47 (3/4), 226–261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saarni, C. (1998). Issues of cultural meaningfulness in emotional development. Developmental Psychology, 34(4), 647–652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17(1), 74–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Schulkin, J., & Gold, P. W. (1999). Behavioral and psychophysiological correlates of self-presentation in temperamentally shy children. Developmental Psychobiology, 35(2), 119–135.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, C. E., Wright, C. I., Shin, L. M., Kagan, J., & Rauch, S. L. (2003). Inhibited and uninhibited infants “grown up”: Adult amygdalar response to novelty. Science, 300(5627), 1952–1953.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwarz, B., Schäfermeier, E., Trommsdorff, G., Friedlmeier, W., Chakkarath, P., & Schwarz, B. (2005). Relations between value orientation, child-rearing goals, and parenting: A comparison of German and South Korean mothers. In Friedlmeier, W. (Ed.), Culture and human development: The importance of cross-cultural research for the social sciences (pp. 203–230). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). Reactive aggression among maltreated children: The contributions of attention and emotion dysregulation. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27(4), 381–395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shweder, R. A., Goodnow, J., Hatano, G., LeVine, R. A., Markus, H., & Miller, P. (1998). The cultural psychology of development: One mind, many mentalities. In Damon, W. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development: Vol. 1 (5th ed., pp. 865–937). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Snidman, N., Kagan, J., Riordan, L., & Shannon, D. C. (1995). Cardiac function and behavioral reactivity during infancy. Psychophysiology, 32(3), 199–207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soto, J. A., Levenson, R. W., & Ebling, R. (2005). Cultures of moderation and expression: Emotional experience, behavior, and physiology in Chinese Americans and Mexican Americans. Emotion, 5(2), 154–165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevenson, H. W., Lee, S.-y., Chen, C., & Stigler, J. W. (1990). Contexts of achievement: A study of American, Chinese, and Japanese children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 55(1), 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stifter, C. A., Spinrad, T. L., & Braungart-Rieker, J. M. (1999). Toward a developmental model of child compliance: The role of emotion regulation in infancy. Child Development, 70(1), 21–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stigler, J. W., & Perry, M. (1988). Mathematics learning in Japanese, Chinese, and American classrooms. New Directions for Child Development, 41, 27–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stuss, D. T. (1992). Biological and psychological development of executive functions. Brain and Cognition, 20(1), 8–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A conceptualization at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9(4), 545–569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (2002). Culture structures the environment for development. Human Development, 45(4), 270–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Super, C. M., Harkness, S., Berry, J. W., Dasen, P. R., & Saraswathi, T. S. (1997). The cultural structuring of child development. In Berry, J. W., Dasen, P. R., & Saraswathi, T. S. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, Vol. 2: Basic processes and human development (2nd ed., pp. 1–39). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Sy, S. R., Fang, G., & Huntsinger, C. S. (2003). Formal instruction and kindergarten achievement in China and the United States. Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 4(2), 247–267.Google Scholar
Sy, S. R., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2005). Parent beliefs and children's achievement trajectories during the transition to school in Asian American and European American families. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(6), 505–515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tang, T. N., & Dion, K. L. (1999). Gender and acculturation in relation to traditionalism: Perceptions of self and parents among Chinese students. Sex Roles, 41(1), 17–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tardif, T., So, C. W.-C., & Kaciroti, N. (2007). Language and false belief: Evidence for general, not specific, effects in Cantonese-speaking preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 43(2), 318–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2), 25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Greenport, CT: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (2001). Individualism-collectivism and personality. Journal of Personality, 69(6), 907–924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, J. L., Chentsova-Dutton, Y., Freire-Bebeau, L., & Przymus, D. E. (2002). Emotional expression and physiology in European Americans and Hmong Americans. Emotion, 2(4), 380–397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B., & Fung, H. H. (2006). Cultural variation in affect valuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(2), 288–307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsai, J. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Cultural influences of emotional responding: Chinese American and European American dating couples during interpersonal conflict. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28(5), 600–625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, J. L., Levenson, R. W., & McCoy, K. (2006). Cultural and temperamental variation in emotional response. Emotion, 6(3), 484–497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuvblad, C., Grann, M., & Lichtenstein, P. (2006). Heritability for adolescent antisocial behavior differs with socioeconomic status: Gene-environment interaction. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(7), 734–743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wan, C., Chiu, C.-Y., Tam, K.-P., Lee, S.-l., Lau, I. Y.-M., & Peng, S. (2007). Perceived cultural importance and actual self-importance of values in cultural identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(2), 337–354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Q. (2001). “Did you have fun?” American and Chinese mother-child conversations about shared emotional experiences. Cognitive Development, 16(2), 693–715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Q., & Fivush, R. (2005). Mother-child conversations of emotionally salient events: Exploring the functions of emotional reminiscing in European-American and Chinese families. Social Development, 14(3), 473–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Z., Chen, H., & Chen, X. (2002). 2–4-year-old children's behavioral inhibition as predictor of later strange peer interactions. Psychological Science (China), 25(1), 53–56.Google Scholar
Watamura, S. E., Donzella, B., Alwin, J., & Gunnar, M. R. (2003). Morning-to-afternoon increases in cortisol concentrations for infants and toddlers at child care: Age differences and behavioral correlates. Child Development, 74(4), 1006–1020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watamura, S. E., Sebanc, A. M., & Gunnar, M. R. (2002). Rising cortisol at childcare: Relations with nap, rest, and temperament. Developmental Psychobiology, 40(1), 33–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, M. I., LeVine, R. A., Stevenson, H. W., Azuma, H., & Hakuta, K. (1986). What is an 'ii ko' (good child)? In Stevenson, H., Azuma, H., & Hakuta, K. (Eds.), Child development and education in Japan (pp. 55–62). New York: W H Freeman.Google Scholar
Wu, C., & Chao, R. K. (2005). Intergenerational cultural conflicts in norms of parental warmth among Chinese American immigrants. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(6), 516–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, D. Y. H., & Bond, M. H. (1996). Chinese childhood socialization. In Bond, M. H. (Ed.), The handbook of Chinese psychology (pp. 143–154). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wu, D. Y. H., Tseng, W. S., & Wu, W. S. T. a. D. Y. H. (1985). Introduction: The characteristics of Chinese culture. In Tseng, W. S. (Ed.), Chinese culture and mental health (pp. 3–13). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, Y., Farver, J. A. M., Zhang, Z., Zeng, Q., Yu, L., & Cai, B. (2005). Mainland Chinese parenting styles and parent-child interaction. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(6), 524–531.Google Scholar
Yang, K.-S., & Bond, M. H. (1986). Chinese personality and its change. In Bond, M. H. (Ed.), The psychology of the Chinese people (pp. 106–170). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Yang, K.-S., Kim, U., Yang, K.-S., & Hwang, K.-K. (2006). Indigenous personality research: The Chinese case. In Kim, U., Yang, K.-S., & Hwang, K.-K. (Eds.), Indigenous and cultural psychology: Understanding people in context (pp. 285–314). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, Y., Kohnstamm, G., Slotboom, A.-M., Elphick, E., & Cheung, P. C. (2002). Chinese and Dutch parents' perceptions of their children's personality. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163(2), 165–178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., Wang, Y., & Reiser, M. (2004). Chinese children's effortful control and dispositional anger/frustration: Relations to parenting styles and children's social functioning. Developmental Psychology, 40(3), 352–366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×