Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T17:23:45.380Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Context Matters: Exploring Definitions of a Poorly Modulated Stress Response

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 July 2009

Sheryl L. Olson
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Arnold J. Sameroff
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Get access

Summary

There are an increasing number of studies assessing the early development of physiological and emotion self-regulation and the risk for psychopathology (Calkins & Dedmon, 2000; Calkins & Fox, 2002; Keenan, 2000; Shaw et al., 1997). The capacity to regulate emotion and behavior in early childhood has been shown to be extremely important in the development of adaptive and appropriate social behaviors in the preschool and school years (Calkins, 1994; Eisenberg et al., 1995, 1996). Children of parents with psychopathology are at greater risk for psychopathology and even in their infancy can exhibit behavioral differences from other children (Cohn & Tronick, 1983; Field, 1995).

Developmental psychopathology is a discipline that is organized around the incorporation of developmental principles into the study of the etiology of psychopathology. The program of research described in this chapter is informed by this framework and as a result is focused on this question: How early can we tell that a child is on a pathway toward deficient behavioral and emotional functioning? We propose that poorly modulated responses to stimuli in the first months of life may indicate risk for the development of later behavioral and emotional problems. In this chapter we present an overview of the theoretical underpinnings of this program of research, descriptive data on individual differences in the neonate's cortisol response to two different stressors, and an examination of the relations between behavior and cortisol response, with the goal of exploring how to operationally define suboptimal patterns of stress reactivity in human infants.

Type
Chapter

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bachmann, A. W., Sedgley, T. L., Jackson, R. V., Gibson, J. N., Young, R., & Torpy, D. J. (2005). Glucocorticoid receptor polymorphisms and post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(3), 297–306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, J. E. (1992). Infant Characteristics Questionnaire. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University.Google Scholar
Beeghly, M., & Tronick, E. Z. (1994). Effects of prenatal exposure to cocaine in early infancy: Toxic effects on the process of mutual regulation. Infant Mental Health Journal, 15(2), 158–175.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brazelton, T. B., & Nugent, J. K. (1995). Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (3rd. ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D. (Ed.). (1994). Origins and outcomes of individual differences in emotion regulation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2–3), 53–72.CrossRefPubMed
Calkins, S. D., & Dedmon, S. E. (2000). Physiological and behavioral regulation in two-year-old children with aggressive/destructive behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28(2), 103–118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (2002). Self-regulatory processes in early personality development: A multilevel approach to the study of childhood social withdrawal and aggression. Development & Psychopathology, 14(3), 477–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coe, C. L., Kramer, M., Czeh, B., Gould, E., & Reeves, A. J. (2003). Prenatal stress diminishes neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of juvenile rhesus monkeys. Society of Biological Psychiatry, 54, 1025–1034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1983). Three-month-old infants' reaction to simulated maternal depression. Child Development, 54(1), 185–193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delamater, A. M., & Lahey, B. B. (1983). Physiological correlates of conduct problems and anxiety in hyperactive and learning disabled children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11(1), 85–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R., Murphy, B., Maszk, P., Smith, M., & Karbon, M. (1995). The role of emotionality and regulation in children's social functioning: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 66(5), 1360–1384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Karbon, M., Murphy, B. C., Wosinski, M., Polazzi, L., et al. (1996). The relations of children's dispositional prosocial behavior to emotionality, regulation, and social functioning. Child Development, 67(3), 974–992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferry, B., Roozendaal, B., & McGaugh, J. L. (1999). Role of norepinephrine in mediating stress hormone regulation of long-term memory storage: A critical involvement of the amygdala. Society of Biological Psychiatry, 46, 1140–1152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T. (1995). Infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flinn, M. V., & England, B. G. (1995). Childhood stress and family environment. Current Anthropology, 36, 854–866.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granger, D. A., Serbin, L. A., Schwartzman, A., Lehoux, P., Cooperman, J., Ikeda, S., et al. (1998). Children's salivary cortisol, internalising behaviour problems, and family environment: Results from the Concordia Longitudinal Risk Project. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 22(4), 707–728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., Brodersen, L., Krueger, K., & Rigatuso, J. (1996). Dampening of adrenocortical responses during infancy: Normative changes and individual differences. Child Development, 67(3), 877–889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., Connors, J., & Isenjee, J. (1988). Adrenocortical activity and behavioral distress in human newborns. Developmental Psychobiology, 21(4), 297–310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., Connors, J., & Isenjee, J. (1989). Lack of stability in neonatal adrenocortical reactivity because of rapid habituation of the adrenocortical response. Developmental Psychobiology, 22(3), 221–233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., Hertsgaard, L., Larson, M., & Rigatuso, J. (1992). Cortisol and behavioral responses to repeated stressors in the human newborn. Developmental Psychobiology, 24(7), 487–505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., Larson, M. C., Hertsgaard, L., Harris, M. L., & Broderson, L. (1992). The stressfulness of separation among nine-month-old infants: Effects of social context variables and infant temperament. Child Development, 63, 290–303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., Porter, F. L., Wolf, C. M., Rigatuso, J., & Larson, M. C. (1995). Neonatal stress reactivity: Predictions to later emotional temperament. Child Development, 66, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huizink, A. C., Mulder, E. J., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2004). Prenatal stress and risk for psychopathology: Specific effects or induction of general susceptibility. Psychological Bulletin, 130(1), 115–142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johns, J. M., & Noonan, L. R. (1995). Prenatal cocaine exposure affects social behavior in Sprague-Dawley rats. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 17, 569–576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, K. (1978). Discriminating among normal infants by multivariate analysis of Brazelton scores: Lumping and smoothing. Organization and stability of newborn behavior: A commentary on the Brazelton Neonatal Behavior Assessment Scale. Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development, 43, 60–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, K. (2000). Emotion dysregulation as a risk factor for child psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7(4), 418–434.Google Scholar
Keenan, K., Gunthorpe, D., & Young, D. (2002). Patterns of cortisol reactivity in African-American neonates from low-income environments. Developmental Psychobiology, 41, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keenan, K., & Shaw, D. S. (1994). The development of aggression in toddlers: A study of low-income families. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22, 53–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative emotion. Developmental Psychology, 25, 343–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. S. (1995a). Developmental changes in infants' responses to stress. Child Development, (66), 657–670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. S. (1995b). Stability and change in cortisol and behavioral response to stress during the first 18 months of life. Developmental Psychobiology, 28(8), 419–428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, D., Diorio, J., Tannenbaum, B., Caldji, C., Francis, D., Freedman, A., et al. (1997). Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science, 277, 1659–1662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lou, H. C., Hansen, D., Nordentoft, M., Pryds, O., Jensen, F., Nim, J., et al. (1994). Prenatal stressors of human life affect fetal brain development. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 36(9), 826–932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maccari, S., Darnaudery, M., Morley-Fletcher, S., Zuena, A., Cinque, C., & Reeth, O. (2003). Prenatal stress and long-term consequences: Implications of glucocorticoid hormones. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 27(1–2), 119–127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayes, L. C., Bornstein, M. H., Chawarska, K., & Granger, R. H. (1996). Impaired regulation of arousal in 3-month-old infants exposed prenatally to cocaine and other drugs. Development and Psychopathology, 8(1), 29–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBurnett, K., Lahey, B. B., Rathouz, P. J., & Loeber, R. (1991). Anxiety, inhibition, and conduct disorder in children: II. Relation to salivary cortisol. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 192–196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBurnett, K., Lahey, B. B., Rathouz, P. J., & Loeber, R. (2000). Low salivary cortisol and persistent aggression in boys referred for disruptive behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 38–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connor, T. G., Ben-Shlomo, Y., Heron, J., Golding, J., Adams, D., & Glover, V. (2005). Prenatal anxiety predicts individual differences in cortisol in pre-adolescent children. Biological Psychiatry, 58(3), 211–217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olds, D. L., Kitzman, H., Cole, R., Robinson, J., Sidora, K., Luckey, D. W., et al. (2004). Effects of nurse home-visiting on maternal life course and child development: Age 6 follow-up results of a randomized trial. Pediatrics, 114(6), 1550–1559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plagemann, A. (2005). Perinatal programming and functional teratogenesis: Impact on body weight regulation and obesity. Physiology & Behavior, 86(5), 661–668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramsay, D. S., & Lewis, M. (1994). Developmental change in infant cortisol and behavioral response to inoculation. Child Development, 65, 1491–1502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Posner, M. I. (1994). A psychobiological approach to the development of temperament. In Bates, J. E. & T. Wachs, D. (Eds.), Temperament: Individual differences at the interface of biology and behavior (pp. 83–116). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapolsky, R. M., Krey, L. C., & McEwen, B. S. (1984). Glucocorticoid-sensitive hippocampal neurons are involved in terminating the adrenocortical stress response. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 81, 6174–6177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapolsky, R. M., Romero, L. M., & Munck, A. U. (2000). How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine Reviews, 21, 55–89.Google ScholarPubMed
Schneider, M. L. (1992). Delayed object permanence development in prenatally stressed rhesus monkey infants (Macaca mulatta). Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 12(2), 96–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, M. L., & Coe, C. L. (1993). Repeated social stress during pregnancy impairs neuromotor development of the primate infant. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 14(2), 81–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, M. L., Coe, C. L., & Lubach, G. R. (1992). Endocrine activation mimics the adverse effects of prenatal stress on the neuromotor development of the infant primate. Developmental Psychobiology, 25(6), 427–439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, M. L., & Moore, C. F. (2000). Effect of prenatal stress on development: A nonhuman primate model. In Nelson, C. A. (Ed.), The Minnesota Symposium on child psychology (pp. 201–244). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Shaw, D. S., Bell, R. Q., & Gilliom, M. (2000). A truly early starter model of antisocial behavior revisited. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 3(3), 155–172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, D. S., Keenan, K., Vondra, J. I., Delliquardi, E., Giovannelli, J., et al. (1997). Antecedents of preschool children's internalizing problems: A longitudinal study of low-income families. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(12), 1760–1767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spangler, G., & Grossmann, K. E. (1993). Biobehavioral organization in securely and insecurely attached infants. Child Development, 64, 1439–1450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spangler, G., Schieche, M., Ilg, U., Maier, U., & Ackermann, C. (1994). Maternal sensitivity as an external organizer for biobehavioral regulation in infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 27(7), 425–437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
St. James-Roberts, I., & Plewis, I. (1996). Individual differences, daily fluctuations, and developmental changes in amounts of infant waking, fussing, crying, feeding, and sleeping. Child Development, 67(5), 2527–2540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, R. M., & Gratton, A. (2002). Prefrontal cortical regulation of hypothalamic pituitary-adrenal function in the rat and implications for psychopathology. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27, 99–114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tonkiss, J., Shumsky, J. S., Shultz, P. L., Almeida, S. S., & Galler, J. R. (1995). Prenatal cocaine but not prenatal nutrition affects foster mother-pup interactions in rats. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 17, 601–608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergh, B. R., Mulder, E. J., Mennes, M., & Glover, V. (2005). Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: Links and possible mechanisms. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 29(2), 237–258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wadhwa, P. D. (2004). Psychoneuroendocrine processes in human pregnancy influence fetal development and health. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 724–743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wadhwa, P. D., Sandman, C. A., Porto, M., Dunket-Schetter, C., & Garite, T. J. (1993). The association between prenatal stress and infant birth weight and gestational age at birth: A prospective study. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 169, 858–865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wakschlag, L. S., & Hans, S. L. (1999). Relation of maternal responsiveness during infancy to the development of behavior problems in high-risk youths. Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 569–579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weaver, I. C., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D'Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R., et al. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7(8), 847–854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstock, M. (1997). Does prenatal stress impair coping and regulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis?Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 21, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×