Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-m42fx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T12:27:52.489Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Drawing production, drawing re-experience and drawing re-cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Josephine Ross
Affiliation:
University of Stirling
Chris Lange-Küttner
Affiliation:
London Metropolitan University
Annie Vinter
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, France
Get access

Summary

Referring to the literature concerned with recognition of drawings, Ross suggests that self-drawings may be seen as advanced forms of the mirror test of self-recognition, and, as such, are likely to reveal the content of self-knowledge. She explores this proposition through two instructive experiments carried out with children aged 3 to 9 years. The first study establishes that the quality of self-drawings as well as the ability to recognize one's own drawings are related to self-awareness as measured by mirror recognition. The second experiment offers a deeper understanding of these relationships and shows that drawing recognition constitutes an act of self-recognition in itself. In this experiment, besides producing drawings, children were asked either to trace over their peers' drawings with a pencil (a drawing task involving a certain level of self-engagement) or to examine them visually while also hearing a verbal description. Whereas traced drawings were recognized as such after sizeable delays, those only visually examined were not. Ross concludes that ‘children's retrospective analysis of their own drawings has the potential to reveal their self-awareness’.

the maxim ‘Children draw what they know, rather than what they see’ is a formative and recurring topic in the study of children's art. This characteristic, implying a communicative or expressive function for children's drawings, has led many to reason that early artwork can be used to study cognitive development.

Type
Chapter
Information
Drawing and the Non-Verbal Mind
A Life-Span Perspective
, pp. 42 - 62
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self-image reactions before the age of two. Developmental Psychobiology, 5, 297–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bensur, B., Eliot, J. and Hedge, L. (1997). Cognitive correlates of the complexity of children's drawings. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 1079–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burkitt, E., Barrett, M. and Davis, A. (2003). Children's color choices for completing drawings of affectively characterised topics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 445–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, M. V. (1992). Children's drawings. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Duval, T. S. and Wicklund, , , R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Eng, H. (1931). The psychology of children's drawings. New York: Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar
Engelkamp, J. and Zimmer, H. D. (1997). Sensory factors in memory for subject-performed tasks. Acta Psychologica, 96, 43–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, N. H. (1972). Process and product in children's drawing. Perception, 1, 123–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, N. H. (1980). Strategies of representation. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gellert, E. (1968). Comparison of children's self-drawing with their drawings of other persons. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 26, 123–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodnow, J. (1977). Children drawing. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J. and Musham, C. (1981). Avoiding and seeking self-focused attention. Journal of Research in Personality, 15, 191–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. and Hayne, H. (1999). Young children's recognition and description of their own and others' drawings. Developmental Science, 2, 476–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, D. B. (1963). Children's drawings as measures of intellectual maturity. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Hartley, J. L., Somerville, S. C., Cziesch Jensen, D. and Eliefa, C. C. (1982). Abstraction of individual styles from the drawings of five-year-old children. Child Development, 53, 1193–1214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kellogg, R. (1970). Analysing children's art. Palo Alto, CA: National Press Books.Google Scholar
Kerschensteiner, D. G. (1905). Die Entwickelung der zeichnerischen Begabung. Munich: Gerber.Google Scholar
Lange-Küttner, C. and Thomas, G. V. (1995). Drawing and looking: theoretical approaches to pictorial representation in children. London: Pearson/Prentice Hall (distributed by www.amazon.co.uk).Google Scholar
Luquet, G. (1913). Les dessins d'un enfant [The drawings of a child]. Paris: Alcan.Google Scholar
Nolan, E., Adams, S. and Kagan, J. (1980a). Children's recognition memory for their drawings. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 137, 11–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolan, E. and Kagan, J. (1980b). Recognition of self and self's products in preschool children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 137, 285–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolan, E. and Kagan, J. (1981). Memory for products in pre-school children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 138, 15–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ricci, C. (1894). The art of little children. Translated by L. Maitland. Pedagogical Seminary, 3, 302–7. (Original work published 1887.)Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J. and Duval, T. S. (2001). Objective self-awareness theory: recent progress and enduring problems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 230–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stacey, J. T. and Ross, B. M. (1975). Scheme and schema in children's memory of their own drawings. Developmental Psychology, 11, 37–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VanSommers, P. (1984). Drawing and cognition: descriptive and experimental studies of graphic production processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
White, R. and Johnson, B. (1930). Children's choices in modern art. Child Development, 1, 347–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×