Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-03T21:35:40.170Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 February 2016

Michael S. Humphreys
Affiliation:
University of Queensland
Kerry A. Chalmers
Affiliation:
University of Newcastle, New South Wales
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1996). Two-year-olds learn words for absent objects and actions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 7993.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1972). Recognition and retrieval processes in free recall. Psychological Review, 79, 97123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1973). Human associative memory. Washington, DC: V. H. Winston & Sons.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., Bothell, D., Lebiere, C., & Matessa, M. (1998). An integrative theory of list memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 341380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, M. C., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2000). Retrieval-induced forgetting: Evidence for a recall-specific mechanism. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 522530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asch, S. E. (1969). A reformulation of the problem of associations. American Psychologist, 24, 92102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, R. C., & Juola, J. F. (1974). Search and decision processes in recognition memory. In Krantz, D. H., Atkinson, R. C., Luce, R. D., & Suppes, P. (Eds.), Contemporary developments in mathematical psychology: Vol. 1. Learning, memory, and thinking (pp. 243293). San Francisco, CA: Freeman.Google Scholar
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In Spence, K. W. & Spence, J. T. (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 89195). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Aue, W. R., Criss, A. H., & Fischetti, N. W. (2012). Associative information in memory: Evidence from cued recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 109222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Averell, L., & Heathcote, A. (2009). Long term implicit and explicit memory for briefly studied words. In Taatgen, A. & van Rijn, H. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 267281). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Averell, L., & Heathcote, A. (2011). The form of the forgetting curve and the fate of memories. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 55, 2535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azuma, T., Williams, E. J., & Davie, J. E. (2004). Paws + cause = pause? Memory load and memory blends in homophone recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 723728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bain, J. D., & Humphreys, M. S. (1988). Relational context: Independent cues, meanings or configurations? In Davies, G. M. & Thomson, D. M. (Eds.), Memory in context: Context in memory (pp. 97137). Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Bain, J. D., & Humphreys, M. S. (1989). Instructional reinstatement of context: The forgotten prerequisite. In McConkey, K. & Bennett, A. (Eds.), Proceedings of the XXIV international congress of psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 227235). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Baker, L. M., & Elliott, D. N. (1948). Controlled and free association times with identical stimulus and response words. American Journal of Psychology, 61, 535539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banaji, M. R., & Crowder, R. G. (1989). The bankruptcy of everyday memory. American Psychologist, 44, 11851193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banks, W. P. (1970). Signal detection theory and human memory. Psychological Bulletin, 74, 8199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, J. M., & Underwood, B. J. (1959). Fate of first-list associations in transfer theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 97105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: An experimental and social study. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Battig, W. F., & Brackett, H. R. (1961). Comparison of anticipation and recall methods in paired-associate learning. Psychological Reports, 1, 5965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 12521265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Begg, I. (1982). Imagery, organization, and discriminative processes. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 273290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Begg, I. (1983). Imagery instructions and the organization of memory. In Yuille, J. C. (Ed.), Imagery, memory, and cognition (pp. 91115). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bischof, N. (1985). Das Rätzel Ödipus [The Oedipus riddle]. Munich: Piper.Google Scholar
Bischof-Köhler, D. (1985). Zur Phylogenese menschlicher motivation [On the phylogeny of human motivation]. In Eckensberger, L. H. & Lantermann, E. D. (Eds.), Emotion und ReXexivität (pp. 347). Vienna: Urban & Schwarzenberg.Google Scholar
Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1992). A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In Healy, A., Kosslyn, S., & Shiffrin, R. (Eds.), From learning processes to cognitive processes: Essays in honor of William K. Estes (Vol. 2, pp. 3567). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bjork, R. A., Dunlosky, J., & Kornell, N. (2013). Self regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 417444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodner, G. E., & Lindsay, D. S. (2003). Remembering and knowing in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 563580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bodner, G. E., & Taikh, A. (2012). Reassessing the basis of the production effect in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 17111719.Google ScholarPubMed
Bodner, G. E., Taikh, A., & Fawcett, J. M. (2014). Assessing the costs and benefits of production in recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 149154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bower, G. H. (1961). Application of a model to paired-associate learning. Psychometrika, 26, 255280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyer, P. (2009). What are memories for? Functions of recall in cognition and culture. In Boyer, P. & Wertsch, J. V. (Eds.), Memory in mind and culture (pp. 332). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brady, T. F., Konkle, T., Alvarez, G. A., & Oliva, A. (2008). Visual long-term memory has a massive storage capacity for object details. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 1432514329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Mojardin, A. H. (1999). Conjoint recognition. Psychological Review, 106, 160179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1971). The abstraction of linguistic ideas. Cognitive Psychology, 2, 331350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broadbent, D. E. (1971). Decision and stress. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Brooks, L. R., Norman, G. R., & Allen, S. W. (1991). Role of specific similarity in a medical diagnostic task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 278287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, A. S., Schilling, H. E. H., & Hockensmith, M. L. (1999). The negative suggestion effect: Pondering incorrect alternatives may be hazardous to your knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 756764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5, 7399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchler, N. G., Light, L. L., & Reder, L. M. (2008). Memory for items and associations: Distinct representations and processes in associative recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 183199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchler, N. G., & Reder, L. L. (2007). Modelling age-related memory deficits: A two parameter solution. Psychology and Aging, 22, 104121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchwald, A. M. (1969). Effects of “right” and “wrong” on subsequent behaviour: A new interpretation. Psychological Review, 76, 132143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buratto, L. G., & Lamberts, K. (2008). List strength effect without list length effect in recognition memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 218226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butterworth, P. (2000). Task by materials interaction characterise unitization in episodic memory. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, the University of Queensland, Australia.Google Scholar
Carey, M., & Reder, L. M. (2003). A dual-process account of the list-length and strength-based mirror effects in recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 231248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassiday, K. W., Kelly, M. H., & Sharoni, L. J. (1999). Inferring gender from name phonology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 362381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., & Wixted, D. R. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chalmers, K. A., & Humphreys, M. S. (1998). Role of generalized and episode specific memories in the word frequency effect in recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 610632.Google Scholar
Chalmers, K. A., & Turon, H. E. (2012). Source monitoring for color information: Accuracy is higher for intrinsic than extrinsic features. Abstracts of the 53nd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, 17, 203.Google Scholar
Chappell, M., & Humphreys, M. S. (1994). An auto-associative neural network for sparse representations: Analysis and application to models of recognition and cued recall. Psychological Review, 101, 103128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chechile, R. A. (2006). Memory hazard functions: A vehicle for theory development and test. Psychological Review, 113, 3156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chechile, R., & Butler, K. (1975). Storage and retrieval changes that occur in development and release of PI. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 430437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clare, L., & Jones, R. S. P. (2008). Errorless learning in the rehabilitation of memory impairment: A critical review. Neuropsychology Review, 18, 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, S. E. (2012a). Costs and benefits of eyewitness identification reform: Psychological science and public policy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 238259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, S. E. (2012b). Eyewitness identification reform: Data, theory, and due process. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 279283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayton, N. S., Yu, K. S., & Dickinson, A. (2001). Scrub jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens) form integrated memories of the multiple features of caching episodes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 1729.Google ScholarPubMed
Cohn, M., & Moscovitch, M. (2007). Dissociating measures of associative memory: Evidence and theoretical implications. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 436454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collier, R. (2012). Hospital-induced delirium hits hard. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 184, 2324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, M. A., Gardiner, J. M., Perfect, T. J., Anderson, S. J., & Cohen, G. M. (1997). Changes in memory awareness during learning: The acquisition of knowledge by psychology undergraduates. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 393413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, M. A., & Gathercole, S. E. (1987). Modality and long-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 341361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coover, J. E. (1927). Metaphysics and the incredulity of psychologists. In Murchison, C. (Ed.), The case for and against psychical belief (pp. 229264). Reprint edition, 1975. New York: Arno Press.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2002). From hand to mouth: The origins of language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornwell, T. B., & Humphreys, M. S. (2013). Memory for sponsorship relationships: A critical juncture in thinking. Psychology & Marketing, 30, 394407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coventry, K. R., & Clibbens, J. (2002). Does complex behaviour imply complex cognitive abilities? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Criss, A. H., Malmberg, K. J., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2011). Output interference in recognition memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 316326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Criss, A. H., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2004). Context noise and item noise jointly determine recognition memory: A comment on Dennis and Humphreys (2001). Psychological Review, 111, 800807.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Argembeau, A. D., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 342350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalton, P. (1993). The role of stimulus familiarity in context-dependent recognition. Memory & Cognition, 21, 223234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, E. H., & Cofer, C. N. (1968). Some determinants of controlled association times. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78, 200207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, K. M., & Benjamin, A. S. (2014). Retrieval speeds context fluctuation: Why semantic generation enhances later learning but hinders prior learning. Memory & Cognition, 42, 10491062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 1722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehue, T. (2000). From deception trials to control reagents: The introduction of the control group about a century ago. American Psychologist, 55, 264268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, S., & Chapman, A. (2010). The inverse list length effect: A challenge for pure exemplar models of recognition memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 416424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennis, S., & Humphreys, M. S. (2001). A context noise model of episodic recognition memory. Psychological Review, 108, 452478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennis, S., Lee, M. D., & Kinnell, A. (2008). Bayesian analysis of recognition memory: The case of the list-length effect. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 361376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dessalles, J. L. (2007). Storing events to retell them. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 321322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dillon, R. F., & Bittner, L. A. (1975). Analysis of retrieval cues and release from proactive inhibition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 616622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodson, C. S., & Schacter, D. L. (2001). “If I had said it I would have remembered it”: Reducing false memories with a distinctiveness heuristic. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 155161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dommeyer, F. C. (1975). Psychical research at Stanford University. The Journal of Parapsychology, 39, 173205.Google Scholar
Dosher, B. A., & Rosedale, G. S. (1991). Judgments of semantic and episodic relatedness: Common time-course and failure of segregation. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 125160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dubé, C., Payne, L., Sekuler, R., & Rotello, C. M. (2013). Paying attention to attention in recognition memory: Insights from models and electrophysiology. Psychological Science, 24, 23982408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dulaney, D. E. Jr. (1962). The place of hypotheses and intentions. In Eriksen, C. W. (Ed.), Behavior and awareness (pp. 102129). Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Dunn, J. C. (2004). Remember know: A matter of confidence. Psychological Review, 111, 524542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J. C. (2008). The dimensionality of the remember-know task: A state-trace analysis. Psychological Review, 115, 426446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dyne, A. M. (1992). An evaluation of the global matching models of recognition memory. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, the University of Queensland, Australia.Google Scholar
Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Eckert, E., & Kanak, J. N. (1974). Verbal discrimination learning: A review of the acquisition, transfer, and retention literature. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 582607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egan, J. P. (1958). Recognition memory and the operating characteristic (Tech. Note AFCRC-TN-58-51). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University, Hearing and Communication Laboratory.Google Scholar
Eich, J. M. (1982). A composite holographic associative recall model. Psychological Review, 89, 627661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekstrand, B. R., Wallace, W. P., & Underwood, B. J. (1966). A frequency theory of verbal discrimination learning. Psychological Review, 73, 566578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esrov, L. V. (1974). A frequency interpretation of the Bransford and Franks’ effect. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA.Google Scholar
Estes, W. K. (1950). Towards a statistical theory of learning. Psychological Review, 57, 94107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estes, W. K. (1976). The cognitive side of probability learning. Psychological Review, 83, 3764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fawcett, J. M. (2013). The production effect benefits performance in between-subject designs: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychologia, 142, 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, L. K., Barber, S. J., Rajaram, S., Ornstein, P. A., & Marsh, E. J. (2013). Creating illusions of knowledge: Learning errors that contradict prior knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandez, A., & Glenberg, A. M. (1985). Changing environmental context does not reliably affect memory. Memory & Cognition, 13, 333345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finkenauer, C., Luminet, O., Gisle, L., El-Ahmadi, A., van der Linden, M., & Philippot, P. (1998). Flashbulb memories and the underlying mechanisms of their formation: Toward an emotional integrative model. Memory & Cognition, 26, 516531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flexser, A. L., & Tulving, E. (1978). Retrieval independence in recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 85, 153171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, M. J., Seeberger, L. C., & O’Reilly, R. C. (2004). By carrot or by stick: Cognitive reinforcement learning in Parkinsonism. Science, 306, 19401943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, W. J. (2005). Developmental and cognitive perspectives on humans’ sense of the times of past and future events. Learning and Motivation, 36, 145158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardiner, J. M. (1988). Functional aspects or recollective experience. Memory & Cognition, 16, 309313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardiner, J. M., Craik, E. I. M., & Birtwistle, J. (1972). Retrieval cues and release from proactive inhibition. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 11, 778783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garry, M., Manning, C. G., Loftus, E. F., & Sherman, S. J. (1996). Imagination inflation: Imagining a childhood event inflates confidence that it occurred. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 208214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E., & Conway, M. A. (1988). Exploring long-term modality effects: Vocalization leads to best retention. Memory & Cognition, 16, 110119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillund, G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 91, 167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giovanello, K. S., Keane, M. M., & Verfaille, M. (2006). The contribution of familiarity to associative memory in amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 44, 18591865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glanzer, M., & Adams, J. K. (1985). The mirror effect in recognition memory. Memory & Cognition, 13, 820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1975). Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: Land and underwater. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 325331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1980). When does context influence recognition memory? British Journal of Psychology, 71, 99104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopher, D., & Kimchi, R. (1989). Engineering psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 43l455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopher, D., & Koriat, A. (1999). Bridging the gap between basic and applied research on the cognitive regulation of performance. In Gopher, D. & Koriat, A. (Eds.), Attention and performance XVII – Cognitive regulation of performance: Theory and application (pp. 314). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorfein, D. S. (1987). Explaining context effects in short-term memory. In Gorfein, D. S. & Hoffman, R. R. (Eds.), Memory and learning: The Ebbinghaus Centennial conference (pp. 153172). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goss, A. E., & Nodine, C. F. (1965). Paired-associates learning: The role of meaningfulness, similarity, and familiarization. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, D. L., & Knowlton, B. J. (2014). The role of visual imagery in autobiographical memory. Memory & Cognition, 42, 922934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, R. L. (1989). Immediate serial recall of mixed-modality lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 266274.Google ScholarPubMed
Grimaldi, P. J., & Karpicke, J. D. (2012). When and why do retrieval attempts enhance subsequent encoding? Memory & Cognition, 40, 505513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guthrie, E. R. (1940). Association and the law of effect. Psychological Review, 47, 127148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (2010). Relational knowledge: The foundation of higher cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 497505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, J. W., Humphreys, M. S., & Wilson, K. P. (1983). Differences in long-term retention in relation to early school achievement. American Journal of Psychology, 96, 267287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Handy, J. D., & Smith, S. M. (2012). Triggering memory recovery: Effects of direct and incidental cueing. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 17111724.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L., Goldstein, D., & Toppino, T. (1977). Frequency and the conference of referential validity. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 107112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. (1984). Automatic processing of fundamental information: The case of frequency of occurrence. American Psychologist, 39, 13721388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haskins, A. L., Yonelinas, A. P., Quamme, J. R., & Ranganath, C. (2008). Perirhinal cortex supports encoding and familiarity-based recognition of novel associations. Neuron, 59, 554560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hays, M. J., Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2013). When and why a failed test potentiates the effectiveness of subsequent study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 39, 290296.Google Scholar
Heathcote, A., Raymond, F., & Dunn, J. (2006). Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: Evidence from ROC curves. Journal of Memory & Language, 55, 495514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henson, R. N., Eckstein, D., Waszak, F., Frings, C., & Horner, A. J. (2014). Stimulus-response bindings in priming. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 376384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbert, D. M. B., & Burt, J. S. (2004). What do students remember? Episodic memory and the development of schematization. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 7788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higham, P. A., & Vokey, J. R. (2004). Illusory recollection and dual process models of recognition memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 57, 714744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hintzman, D. L. (1984). MINERVA 2: A simulation model of human memory. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 16, 96101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1986). Schema abstraction in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, 93, 411428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1993). Twenty-five years of learning and memory: Was the cognitive revolution a mistake? In Meyer, D. E. & Kornblum, S (Eds.), Attention and performance X1V (pp. 359391). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L., Curran, T., & Oppy, B. (1992). Effects of similarity and repetition on memory: Registration without learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 667680.Google ScholarPubMed
Hintzman, D. L., Summers, J. J., Eki, N. T., & Moore, M. D. (1975). Voluntary attention and the spacing effect. Memory, and Cognition, 3, 576580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hockley, W. E. (1991). Recognition memory for item and associative information: A comparison of forgetting rates. In Hockley, W. E. & Lewandowsky, S. (Eds.), Relating theory and data: Essays on human memory in honor of Bennet B. Murdock (pp. 227248). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hockley, W. E. (1992). Item versus associative information: Further comparisons of forgetting rates. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 13211330.Google Scholar
Hockley, W. E. (2008). The effects of environmental context on recognition memory and claims of remembering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 14121429.Google ScholarPubMed
Hopkins, R. H., & Edwards, R. E. (1972). Pronunciation effects in recognition memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 534537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horowitz, L. W., & Manelis, L. (1972). Toward a theory of redintegrative memory: Adjective-noun phrases. In Bower, G. H. & Spence, J. T. (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 6, pp. 193224). Oxford: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Howard, M. W., Jing, B., Rao, V. A., Provyn, J. P., & Datey, A. V. (2009). Bridging the gap: Transitive associations between items presented in similar temporal contexts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 391407.Google ScholarPubMed
Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (1999). Contextual variability and serial position effects in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 923941.Google ScholarPubMed
Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (2002). A distributed representation of temporal context. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 46, 269299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, T., Loft, S., & Humphreys, M. S. (2014). Internalizing versus externalizing control: Different ways to perform a time-based prospective memory task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 10641071.Google ScholarPubMed
Hull, C. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1952). A behavior system: An introduction to behavior theory concerning the individual organism. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Humphreys, K. R., Menzies, H., & Lake, J. K. (2010). Repeated speech errors: Evidence for learning. Cognition, 117, 151165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, M. S. (1972). Recall and repetition in modified right-wrong paradigm. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 573582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, M. S. (1975). The derivation of endpoint and distance effects in linear orderings from frequency information. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 496505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, M. S. (1976a). Frequency and recency in cued recall: The interaction of old and new learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 413422.Google Scholar
Humphreys, M. S. (1976b). Relational information and the context effect in recognition memory. Memory & Cognition, 4, 221232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, M. S. (1978). Item and relational information: A case for context independent retrieval. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 17, 179187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., & Bain, J. D. (1983). Recognition memory: A cue and information analysis. Memory & Cognition, 11, 583600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, M. S., & Bain, J. D. (1985). Cue repetition and retrieval blocking. American Journal of Psychology, 98, 559568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., Bain, J. D., & Burt, J. S. (1989). Episodically unique and generalized memories: Applications to human and animal amnesics. In Lewandowsky, S., Dunn, J. C. & Kirsner, K. (Eds.), Implicit memory: Theoretical issues (pp. 139158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., Bain, J. D., & Pike, R. (1989). Different ways to cue a coherent memory system: A theory for episodic, semantic and procedural tasks. Psychological Review, 96, 208233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., & Bowyer, P. A. (1981). Cued recall and cue recognition: How similar are the processes. American Journal of Psychology, 94, 6584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., Burt, J. S., & Lawrence, S. (2001). Expecting dirt but saying dart: The creation of a blend memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 820826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, M. S., Cornwell, T. B., McAlister, A. R., Kelly, S. J., Quinn, E. A., & Murray, K. L. (2010). Sponsorship, ambushing and counter-strategy: Effects upon memory for sponsor and event. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 16, 96108.Google ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, M. S., Dennis, S., Chalmers, K. A., & Finnigan, S. (2000). Dual processes in recognition: Does a focus on measurement operations provide a sufficient foundation? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 593603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, M. S., & Galbraith, R. C. (1975). Forward and backward associations in cued recall: Predictions from the encoding specificity principle. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1, 702710.Google Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., & Greeno, J. G. (1970). Interpretation of the two-stage analysis of paired associated memorizing. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 7, 275292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., & Kashima, Y. (2002). Connectionism and self: Distributed representational systems and their implications for self and identity. In Kashima, Y., Foddy, M., & Platow, M.(Eds.), Self and identity: Personal, social, and symbolic (pp. 2754). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., Maguire, A. M., McFarlane, K. A., Burt, J. S., Bolland, S. W., Murray, K. L., & Dunn, R. (2010). Using maintenance rehearsal to explore recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 147159.Google ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, M. S., & Murray, K. L. (2011). Episodic, generalized, and semantic memory tests: Switching and strength effects. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 65, 192199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, M. S., Murray, K. L., & Koh, J. Y. (2014). Reinstating higher order properties of a study list by retrieving a list item. Memory & Cognition, 42, 570582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, M. S., Murray, K. L., & Maguire, A. M. (2009). Contexts and control operations used in accessing list-specific, generalized, and semantic memories. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 311337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, M. S., Pike, R., Bain, J. D., & Tehan, G. (1989). Global matching: A comparison of the SAM, Minerva II, Matrix, and TODAM models. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 33, 3667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., Tangen, J. M., Cornwell, T. B., Quinn, E. A., & Murray, K. L. (2010). Unintended effects of memory on decision making: A breakdown in access control. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 400415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., Tehan, G., O’Shea, A., & Bolland, S. W. (2000). Target similarity effects: Support for the parallel distributed processing assumptions. Memory & Cognition, 28, 798811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, M. S., Wiles, J., & Bain, J. D. (1993). Memory retrieval with two cues: Think of intersecting sets. In Meyer, D. E. & Kornblum, S. (Eds.), Attention and performance XIV: Synergies in experimental psychology, artificial intelligence, and cognitive neuropsychology–A Silver jubilee (pp. 489508). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., Wiles, J., & Dennis, S. (1994). Toward a theory of human memory: Data structures and access processes. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 17, 655692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, R. R. (1995). The subtlety of distinctiveness: What von Restorff really did. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 105112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, R. R. (2006). The concept of distinctiveness in memory research. In Hunt, R. R. & Worthen, J. B. (Eds.), Distinctiveness and memory (pp. 325). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, R. R. (2013). Precision in memory through distinctive processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 1015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, R. R., & Einstein, G. O. (1981). Relational and item specific information in memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 497514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, R. R., & Lamb, C. A. (2001). What causes the isolation effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 13591366.Google ScholarPubMed
Isaac, G. (1978). The food sharing behavior of protohuman hominids. Scientific American, 238(4), 90108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izawa, C. (1971). Massed and spaced practice in paired-associate learning: List versus item distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 1021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izawa, C. (1977). Theoretical and empirical performance differences between study-test and anticipation methods: Retention interval hypothesis versus spaced practice effects. Japanese Psychological Research, 19, 3138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izawa, C. (1985). A test of the differences between anticipation and study-test methods of paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 165184.Google ScholarPubMed
Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacoby, L. L., Bishara, A. J., & Hessels, S. (2007). Probabilistic retroactive interference. The role of accessibility bias in interference effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 200216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacoby, L. L., Debner, J. A., & Hay, J. F. (2001). Proactive interference, accessibility bias, and process dissociations: Valid subjective reports of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 686700.Google ScholarPubMed
Jacoby, L. L., & Hollingshead, A. (1990). Toward a generate/recognize model of performance on direct and indirect tests of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 433454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jang, Y., & Huber, D. E. (2008). Context retrieval and context change in free recall: Recalling from long-term memory drives list isolation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 112127.Google ScholarPubMed
Johansson, P., Hall, L., & Sikström, S., & Olsson, A. (2005). Failure to detect mismatches between intention and outcome in a simple decision task. Science, 310, 116119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, G. J. (1991). A distinctiveness model of serial learning. Psychological Review, 98, 204217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, W. A., Hawley, K. J., & Elliott, J. M. G. (1991). Contribution of perceptual fluency to recognition judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 210223.Google ScholarPubMed
Jordan, M. I., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1992). Forward models: Supervised learning with a distal teacher. Cognitive Science, 16, 307354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jung, J. (1962). Transfer of training as a function of degree of first-list learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1, 197199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalish, M., Griffiths, T. L., & Lewandowsky, S. (2007). Iterated learning: Intergenerational knowledge transmission reveals inductive biases. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 288294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kausler, D. H. (1974). Psychology of verbal learning and memory. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kello, C. T., & Plaut, D. C. (2000). Strategic control in word reading: Evidence from speeded responding in the tempo-naming task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 719750.Google ScholarPubMed
Keppel, G., & Underwood, B. J. (1962). Proactive inhibition in short-term retention of single items. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1, 153161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimball, D. R., Sloutsky, V. M., & Kahana, M. J. (2007). The fSAM model of false recall. Psychological Review, 114, 954993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinnell, A., & Dennis, S. (2011). The list length effect in recognition memory: An analysis of potential confounds. Memory & Cognition, 39, 348363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinnell, A., & Dennis, S. (2012). The role of stimulus type in list length effects in recognition memory. Memory & Cognition, 40, 311325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W. (1967). Learning, memory, and conceptual processes. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1970). Models for free recall and recognition. In Norman, D. A. (Ed.), Models of human memory (pp. 331373). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatzky, R. L. (1984). Armchair theorists have more fun. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (2002). Memory and temporal experience: The effects of episodic memory loss. Social Cognition, 20, 353379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohlers, W. (1941). On the nature of associations. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 84, 489502.Google Scholar
Kornell, N., Hays, M. J., & Bjork, R. A. (2009). Unsuccessful retrieval attempts enhance subsequent learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 989998.Google ScholarPubMed
Lachman, R., & Naus, M. (1984). The episodic/semantic continuum in an evolved machine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 244246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewandowsky, S., Dunn, J. C., & Kirsner, K. (1989). Implicit memory: Theoretical issues. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lewandowsky, S., Geiger, S. M., & Oberauer, K. (2008). Interference-based forgetting in verbal short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 200222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Light, L. L., & Carter-Sobell, L. (1970). Effects of changed semantic context on recognition memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 9, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd, M. E., Hartman, A., Ngo, C. T., Ruser, N., Westerman, D. L., & Miller, J. K. (2015). Not enough familiarity for fluency: Definitional encoding increases familiarity but does not lead to fluency attribution in associative recognition. Memory & Cognition, 43, 3948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loft, S., Humphreys, M., & Neal, A. (2004). The influence of memory for prior instances on performance in a conflict detection task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 173187.Google Scholar
Loft, S., Humphreys, M. S., & Whitney, S. J. (2008). Control of access to memory: The use of task interference as a behavioral probe. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 465479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 1931.Google ScholarPubMed
Logan, F. A. (1960). Incentive. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Lowndes, G., & Savage, G. (2007). Early detection of memory impairment in Alzheimer’s disease: A neurocognitve perspective on assessment. Neuropsychological Review, 17, 193202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, P. A., & MacLeod, C. M. (1998). The influence of attention at encoding on direct and indirect remembering. Acta Psychologica, 98, 291310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLeod, C. M., Gopie, N., Hourihan, K. L., Neary, K. R., & Ozubko, J. D. (2010). The production effect: Delineation of a problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 671685.Google Scholar
Maguire, A. M. (2005). False alarms in episodic recognition: An examination of base-rate, similarity-based, and comprehensive theories. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Queensland, Australia.Google Scholar
Malmberg, K. J. (2008). Recognition memory: A review of the critical findings and an integrated theory for relating them. Cognitive Psychology, 57, 335384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malmberg, K. J., & Nelson, T. O. (2003). The word frequency effect for recognition memory and the elevated-attention hypothesis. Memory & Cognition, 31, 3543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malmberg, K. J., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2005). The “one-shot” hypothesis for context storage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 322336.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1967). Organization and memory. In Spence, K. W. & Spence, J. T. (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 1. Advances in research and theory (pp. 328372). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1972). Organization and recognition. In Tulving, E. & Donaldson, W. (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 146165). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of previous experience. Psychological Review, 87, 252271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1990). Activation, competition, and frequency in lexical access. In Altmann, G. T. M. (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives (pp. 148172). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Martin, E. (1968). Stimulus meaningfulness and paired-associate transfer: An encoding variability hypothesis. Psychological Review, 75, 421441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masters, R., & Maxwell, J. (2002). Was early man caught knapping during the cognitive (r)evolution? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mather, M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: An object based framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 3352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M., Henkel, L. A., & Johnson, M. K. (1997). Evaluating characteristics of false memories: Remember/know judgments and memory characteristics questionnaire compared. Memory & Cognition, 25, 826837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matute, H., Lipp, O. V., Vadillo, M. A., & Humphreys, M. S. (2011). Temporal contexts: Filling the gap between episodic memory and associative learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 660673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayes, A., Montaldi, D., & Migo, E. (2007). Associative memory and the medial temporal lobes. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11(3), 126135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAlister, A. R., Kelly, S. J., Humphreys, M. S., & Cornwell, T. B. (2012). Changes in sponsorship alliance and the communication implications of spontaneous recovery. Journal of Advertising, 41, 516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride, D. M., & Dosher, B. A. (1997). A comparison of forgetting in an implicit and explicit memory task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(4), 371392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L. (2001). Failures to learn and their remediation: A Hebbian account. In McClelland, J. L. & Siegler, R. S. (Eds.), Mechanisms of cognitive development: Behavioral and neural perspectives. Carnegie Mellon symposia on cognition (pp. 97121). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L. (2013). Incorporating rapid neocortical learning of new schema-consistent information into complementary learning systems theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 11901210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, J. L., & Chappell, M. (1998). Familiarity breeds differentiation: A subjective-likelihood approach to the effects of experience in recognition memory. Psychological Review, 105, 724760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O’Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the Hippocampus and Neocortex: Insights from the success and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102, 419457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCloskey, M., & Cohen, N. J. (1989). Catastrophic forgetting in connectionist networks: The problem of sequential learning. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 20, pp. 109165). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, M. (1985). Misleading postevent information and memory for events: Arguments and evidence against memory impairment hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCullers, J. C. (1965). Type of associative interference as a factor in verbal paired-associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 1216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McElree, B. (2006). Accessing recent events. In Ross, B. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 46, pp. 155200). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
McFarlane, K. M., & Humphreys, M. S. (2012). Maintenance-rehearsal: The key to the role attention plays in storage and forgetting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 10011018.Google Scholar
McGovern, J. B. (1964). Extinction of associations in four transfer paradigms. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 78(16), 593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melton, A. W. (1963). Implications of Short-term memory for a general theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mensink, G. J. M., & Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (1988). A model for interference and forgetting. Psychological Review, 95, 434455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mercado, E., Murray, S., Uyeyama, R., Pack, A. A., & Herman, L. M. (1998). Memory for recent actions in the bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): Repetition of arbitrary behaviors using an abstract rule. Animal Learning & Behavior, 26, 210218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metcalfe, J. (1990). A composite holographic associative recall model (CHARM) and blended memories eyewitness testimony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 145160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mewhort, D. J. K., & Johns, E. E. (2000). The extralist-feature effect: Evidence against item matching in short-term recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 129, 262284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mickes, L., Seal-Carlisle, T. M., & Wixted, J. T. (2013). Rethinking familiarity: Remember/Know judgments in free recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 333349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. A., & Selfridge, J. A. (1950). Verbal context and the recall of meaningful material. American Journal of Psychology, 63, 176185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monsell, S. (1991). The nature of the locus of word frequency effects in reading. In Besner, D. & Humphreys, G. W. (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 148197). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mullet, H. G., Umanath, S., & Marsh, E. J. (2014). Recent study, but not retrieval, of knowledge protects against learning errors. Memory & Cognition, 42, 12391249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulligan, N. W. (2008). Attention and memory. In Roediger, H. L. (Ed.), Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference (pp. 722). Oxford: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulligan, N. W., & Osborn, K. (2009). The modality-match effect in recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 564571.Google ScholarPubMed
Mulligan, N. W., Spataro, P., & Picklesimer, M. (2014). The attentional boost effect with verbal materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 10491063.Google ScholarPubMed
Murchison, C. (1927). The case for and against psychical belief. Reprint edition, 1975. New York: Arno Press, Inc.Google Scholar
Murdock, B. Jr B. (1969). Modality effects in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 665676.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murdock, B. B. (1974). Human memory: Theory and data. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Murdock, B. B. (1982). A theory for the storage and retrieval of item and associative information. Psychological Review, 89, 609626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murdock, B. B. (1993). Derivations for the chunking model. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 37, 421445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murnane, K., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1991). Interference and the representation of events in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 855874.Google ScholarPubMed
Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2000). Adult age differences in memory performance: Tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 11701187.Google ScholarPubMed
Naveh-Benjamin, M., Hussain, Z., Guez, J., & Bar-On, M. (2003). Adult age differences in episodic memory: Further support for an associative-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 826837.Google ScholarPubMed
Neisser, U. (1978). Memory: What are the important questions? In Gruneberg, M. M., Morris, P. E., & Sykes, R. N. (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 324). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, A. M., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2013). The co-evolution of knowledge and event memory. Psychological Review, 120, 356394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, D. L., Bajo, M. T., & Casanueva, D. (1985). Prior knowledge: The influence of natural category size as a function of intention and interval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 94105.Google Scholar
Nelson, D. L., & Brooks, D. H. (1974). Relative effectiveness of rhymes and synonyms as retrieval cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 503507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, D. L., & Friedrich, M. A. (1980). Encoding and cueing sounds and senses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning and Memory, 6, 717731.Google ScholarPubMed
Nelson, D. L., & Goodmon, L. B. (2002). Experiencing a word can prime its accessibility and its associative connections to related words. Memory & Cognition, 30, 380398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, D. L., Goodmon, L. B., & Ceo, D. (2007). How does delayed testing reduce effects of implicit memory: Context infusion or cuing with context? Memory & Cognition, 35, 10141043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, D. L., Kitto, K., Galea, D., McEvoy, C. L., & Bruza, P. D. (2013). How activation, entanglement, and searching a semantic network contribute to event memory. Memory & Cognition, 41, 797819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, D. L., & McEvoy, C. L. (1979). Encoding context and set size. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 292314.Google Scholar
Nelson, D. L., McKinney, V. M., Gee, N. R., & Janczura, G. A. (1998). Interpreting the influence of implicitly activated memories on recall and recognition. Psychological Review, 105, 299324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, D. L., Schreiber, T. A., & McEvoy, C. L. (1992). Processing implicit and explicit representations. Psychological Review, 99, 322348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, D. L., Walling, J. R., & McEvoy, C. L. (1979). Doubts about depth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 2444.Google Scholar
Nelson, D. L., Wheeler, J. W., Borden, R. C., & Brooks, D. H. (1974). Levels of processing and cuing: Sensory versus meaning features. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 971977.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111, 486511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, T. O. (1977). Repetition and depth of processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 151171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, F. J., & Loftus, E. F. (2012). Clarkian logic on trial. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nuttin, J., & Greenwald, A. G. (1968). Reward and punishment in human learning: Elements of a behaviour theory. Academic Press: London.Google Scholar
Osgood, C. E. (1949). The similarity paradox in human learning: A resolution. Psychological Review, 56, 132143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osth, A. F., & Dennis, S. (2015). Sources of interference in item and associative recognition memory. Psychological Review, 122, 260311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overton, R. C., & Adolphson, C. J. (1979). Multiple trace retrieval: A trace-to-trace retrieval process? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 485495.Google Scholar
Öztekin, I., Davachi, L., & McElree, B. (2010). Are representations in working memory distinct from representations in long-term memory? Neural evidence in support of a single store. Psychological Science, 21, 11231133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palermo, D. S., & Jenkins, J. J. (1964). Paired-associate learning as a function of the strength of links in the associative chain. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3, 406412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pavio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Peterson, W. W., Birdsall, T. G., & Fox, W. C. (1954). The theory of signal detectability. Proceedings of the IRE Professional Group on Information Theory, 4, 171212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pike, R. (1984). Comparison of convolution and matrix distributed memory systems for associative recall and recognition. Psychological Review, 91, 281294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poldrack, R. A., & Logan, G. (1997). Fluency and response speed in recognition judgments. Memory & Cognition, 25, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Postman, L., & Gray, W. D. (1979). Does imaginal encoding increase resistance to interference? American Journal of Psychology, 92, 215233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Postman, L., & Keppel, G. (1977). Conditions of cumulative proactive inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 376403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Postman, L., & Riley, D. A. (1957). A critique of Kohler’s theory of association. Psychological Review, 64, 6172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Postman, L., & Sassenrath, J. (1961). The automatic action of verbal rewards and punishments. Journal of General Psychology, 65, 109136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Postman, L., & Underwood, B. J. (1973). Critical issues in interference theory. Memory & Cognition, 1, 1940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potts, R. (1988). Early hominid activities at Olduvai. New York: Aldine de Gruyter, Hawthorne.Google Scholar
Pritchard, M. E., & Keenan, J. M. (2002). Does jury deliberation really improve jurors’ memories? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 589601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puff, C. R. (1979). Memory organization and structure. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Quamme, J. R., Yonelinas, A. P., & Norman, K. A. (2007). Effect of unitization on associative recognition in amnesia. Hippocampus, 17, 192200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981). Search for associative memory. Psychological Review, 88, 93134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratcliff, R., Clark, S. E., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1990). List-strength effect: I. Data and discussion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 163178.Google ScholarPubMed
Reber, A. S. (1969). Response perseveration in binary-choice recognition! Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 206214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reder, L. M., Nhouyvanisvong, A., Schunn, C. D., Ayers, M. S., Angstadt, P., & Hiraki, K. (2000). A mechanistic account of the mirror effect for word frequency: A computational model of Remember-Know judgments in a continuous recognition paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 294320.Google Scholar
Reitman, J. S., & Bower, G. H. (1973). Storage and later recognition of exemplars of concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 194206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renoult, L., Davidson, P. S. R., Palombo, D. J., Moscovitch, M., & Levine, B. (2012). Personal semantics: At the crossroads of semantic and episodic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 550558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F. (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 6499). New York: Appleton Century Crofts.Google Scholar
Rock, I. (1957). The role of repetition in associative learning. American Journal of Psychology, 70, 186193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roediger, H. L. III, & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 181210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roediger, H. L. III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803814.Google Scholar
Roediger, H. L. III, Putnam, A. L., & Smith, M. A. (2011). Ten benefits of testing and their applications to educational practice. In Mestre, J. P. & Ross, B. H., Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Cognition in Education, 53, 136.Google Scholar
Roediger, H. L. III, Weldon, M. S., Stadler, M. L., & Riegler, J. L. (1992). Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: Word fragment and word stem completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 12511269.Google ScholarPubMed
Rogers, T. T., & McClelland, J. L. (2004). Semantic cognition: A parallel distributed processing approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, D. C. (1995). Memory in oral traditions: The cognitive psychology of epic, ballads, and counting out rhymes. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, D. C. (2006). The basic-systems model of episodic memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 277311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, D. C., Hinton, S., & Wenzel, A. E. (1999). The precise time course of retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 5, 11611176.Google Scholar
Rubin, D. C., & Wallace, W. T. (1989). Rhyme and reason: Analysis of dual retrieval cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 698709.Google Scholar
Rundus, D. (1973). Negative effects of using list items as cues. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 4350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runquist, W. N. (1986). The effect of testing on the forgetting of related and unrelated associates. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 40, 6576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sahakyan, L., & Delaney, P. F. (2003). Can encoding differences explain the benefits of directed forgetting in the list method paradigm? Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 195206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sahakyan, L., & Delaney, P. F. (2005). Directed forgetting in incidental learning and recognition testing: Support for a two-factor account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 789801.Google ScholarPubMed
Sahakyan, L., & Kelley, C. M. (2002). A contextual change account of the directed forgetting effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 10641072.Google ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362, 773786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L., & Wiseman, A. L. (2006). Reducing memory errors: The distinctiveness heuristic. In Hunt, R. R. & Worthen, J. B. (Eds.), Distinctiveness and memory (pp. 89107). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schlanger, N. (1996). Understanding Levallois: Lithic technology and cognitive architecture. Cambridge Archeological Journal, 6, 231254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schooler, J. W., Gerhard, D., & Loftus, E. F. (1986). Qualities of the unreal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 171181.Google ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, G., Howard, M. W., Jing, B., & Kahana, M. J. (2005). Shadows of the past: Temporal retrieval effects in recognition memory. Psychological Science, 16, 898904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 20, 1121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sederberg, P. B., Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (2008). A context based theory of recency and contiguity in free recall. Psychological Review, 115, 893912.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheldon, S., & Moscovitch, M. (2012). The nature and time-course of medial temporal lobe contributions to semantic retrieval: An fMRI study on verbal fluency. Hippocampus, 22, 14511466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. N., & Teightsoonian, M. (1961). Retention of information under conditions approaching a steady state. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 302309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiffrin, R. M., Ratcliff, R., & Clark, S. E. (1990). List-strength effect: II. Theoretical mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 179195.Google ScholarPubMed
Shiffrin, R. M., & Steyvers, M. (1997). A model for recognition memory: REM–retrieving effectively from memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 145166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shuell, T. J. (1968). Retroactive inhibition free-recall learning of categorized lists. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, 797805.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1998). Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 644649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1958). Teaching machines. Science, 128, 969977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slamecka, N. J. (1966). Differentiation versus unlearning of verbal associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 822828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slamecka, N. J., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 592604.Google Scholar
Slotnick, S. D., & Dodson, C. S. (2005). Support for a continuous (single-process) model of recognition memory and source memory. Memory & Cognition, 33, 151170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. N., Wixted, J. T., & Squire, L. R. (2011). The hippocampus supports both recollection and familiarity when memory is strong. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1569315702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M. (1988). Environmental context-dependent memory. In Davies, G. M. & Thomson, D. M. (Eds.), Memory in context: Context in memory (pp. 1334). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Smith, S. M. (1994). Theoretical principles of context-dependent memory. In Morris, P. & Gruneberg, M. (Eds.), Theoretical aspects of memory (2nd ed., pp. 168195). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Smith, S. M., & Moynan, S. C. (2008). Forgetting and recovering the unforgettable. Psychological Science, 19, 462468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., & Vela, E. (2001). Environmental context-dependent memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 203220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snodgrass, J. G., Hirshman, E., & Fan, J. (1996). The sensory match effect in recognition memory: Perceptual fluency or episodic trace? Memory & Cognition, 24, 367383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spark, L. C. (2007). “Ask me again”: Differential effects of repeated questioning on the recognition and endorsement of autobiographical childhood events. Unpublished honours thesis, University of Queensland, Australia.Google Scholar
Spataro, P., Mulligan, N. W., & Rossi-Arnaud, C. (2013). Divided attention can enhance memory encoding: The attentional boost effect in implicit memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 12231231.Google ScholarPubMed
Spence, J. T., & Lair, C. V. (1965). Associative interference in the paired associate learning of remitted and nonremitted schizophrenics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 70, 119122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, K. W. (1956). Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spielberger, C. D. (1962). The role of awareness in verbal conditioning. In Eriksen, C. W. (Ed.), Behavior and awareness (pp. 73101). Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Spielberger, C. D., Bernstein, I. H., & Ratliff, R. G. (1966). Information and the incentive value of the reinforcing stimulus in verbal conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 2631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Staats, O. P. (1985). Associative processing during type 1 rehearsal. Unpublished honours thesis, University of Queensland, Australia.Google Scholar
Stark, C. E. L., & Okado, Y. (2003). Making memories without trying: Medial temporal lobe activity associated with incidental memory formation during recognition. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 67486753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Starns, J. J., & Hicks, J. L. (2013). Internal reinstatement hides cueing effects in source memory tasks. Memory and Cognition, 41, 953966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, V. E. (2002). Footloose and fossil-free no more: Evolutionary psychology needs archaeology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 421422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storm, B. C., Bjork, E. L., Bjork, R. A., & Nestojko, J. F. (2006). Is retrieval success a necessary condition for retrieval-induced forgetting? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 10231027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storm, B. C., Friedman, M. C., Murayama, K., & Bjork, R. A. (2014). On the transfer of prior tests or study events to subsequent study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 115124.Google ScholarPubMed
Stout, D. (2002a). Skill and cognition in stone tool production: An ethnographic case study from Irian Jaya. Current Anthropology, 43, 693722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stout, D. (2002b). Thinking and doing in cognitive archaeology: Giving skill its due. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 421422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strand, B. Z. (1970). Retroactive inhibition in free recall learning: Unlearning, or category size or? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 286290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T., & Busby, J. (2005). Making decisions with the future in mind: Developmental and comparative identification of mental time travel. Learning and Motivation, 36, 110125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swallow, K. M., & Jiang, Y. V. (2011). The role of timing in the attentional boost effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 389404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talamini, L. M., de Haan, L., Nieman, D. H., Linszen, D. H., & Meeter, M. (2010). Reduced context effects on retrieval in first episode schizophrenia. PLoS, ONE, 5, e10356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talarico, J. M., & Rubin, D. C. (2003). Confidence, not consistency, characterizes flashbulb memories. Psychological Science, 14, 455461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanner, W. P., & Swets, J. A. (1954). A decision making theory of visual detection. Psychological Review, 61, 401409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tassi, P., & Muzet, A. (2000). Sleep inertia. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 4, 341353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tehan, G. (2010). Associative relatedness enhances recall and produces false memories in immediate serial recall. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 266272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tehan, G., & Humphreys, M. S. (1988). Articulatory loop explanations of memory span and pronunciation rate correspondences: A cautionary note. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 26, 293296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tehan, G., & Humphreys, M. S. (1995). Transient phonemic codes and immunity to proactive interference. Memory & Cognition, 23, 181191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tehan, G., & Humphreys, M. S. (1996). Cueing effects in short-term recall. Memory & Cognition, 24, 719732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tehan, G., & Humphreys, M. S. (1998). Creating proactive interference in immediate recall: Building a dog from a dart, a mop, and a fig. Memory & Cognition, 26, 477489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tehan, G., Humphreys, M. S., Tolan, G. A., & Pitcher, C. (2004). The role of context in producing item interactions and false memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 107119.Google ScholarPubMed
Tell, P. M. (1971). Influence of vocalization on short term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 149156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomson, D. M. (1972). Context effects in recognition memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 497511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1931). Human learning. New York: Century.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolan, G. A., & Tehan, G. (1999). Determinants of short-term forgetting: Decay, retroactive interference, or proactive interference? International Journal of Psychology, 34, 285292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomback, D. F. (1980). How nutcrackers find their stores. Condor, 82, 1019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toppino, T. C., & Brochin, H. A. (1989). Learning from tests: The case of true-false examinations. Journal of Educational Research, 83, 119124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toppino, T. C., & Luipersbeck, S. M. (1993). Generality of the negative suggestion effect in objective tests. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 357362.Google Scholar
Tse, D., Langston, R. F., Kakeyama, M., Bethus, I., Spooner, P. A., Wood, E. R., & Morris, R. G. M. (2007). Schemas and memory consolidation. Science, 316, 7682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tse, D., Takeuchi, T., Kakeyama, M., Kajii, Y., Okuno, H., Tohyama, C., & Morris, R. G. M. (2011). Schema-dependent gene activation and memory encoding in neocortex. Science, 333, 891895.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. (1962). Subjective organization in free recall of unrelated words. Psychological Review, 69, 344354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In Tulving, E. & Donaldson, W. (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 382403). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 26, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E. (1995). Organization of memory: Quo vadis? In Gazzaniga, M. S. (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 839847). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E. (2005). Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In Terrace, H. S. & Metcalfe, J. (Eds.), The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness (pp. 356). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E., & Donaldson, W. (Eds.). (1972). Organization of memory. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E., Hayman, C. A., & Macdonald, C. A. (1991). Long-lasting perceptual priming and semantic learning in amnesia: A case experiment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 595617.Google ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E., & Madigan, S. A. (1970). Memory and verbal learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 21, 437484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E., & Pearlstone, Z. (1966). Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 381391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1971). Retrieval processes in recognition memory: Effects of associative context. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 87, 175184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80, 352373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, B. J. (1957). Interference and forgetting. Psychological Review, 64, 4960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Underwood, B. J. (1963). Stimulus selection in verbal learning. In Cofer, C. N. & Musgrave, B. S. (Eds.), Verbal behavior and learning. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Underwood, B. J. (1969). Some correlates of item repetition in free·recall learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 8394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, B. J. (1976). Recognition for pairs of words as a function of associative context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1, 404412.Google Scholar
Underwood, B. J. (1977). Temporal codes for memories. New York: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Underwood, B. J. (1978). Recognition memory as a function of the length of the study list. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 12, 8991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, B. J. (1983). Conceptual similarity and cumulative proactive inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 456461.Google Scholar
Underwood, B. J., Broder, P. K., & Zimmerman, J. (1973a). Associative matching and cumulative proactive inhibition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1, 48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, B. J., Broder, P. K., & Zimmerman, J. (1973b). Retention of verbal discrimination lists as a function of number of prior lists, word frequency, and type of list. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 101105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, B. J., & Humphreys, M. (1979). Context change and the role of meaning in word recognition. American Journal of Psychology, 92, 577609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Underwood, B. J., Jesse, F., & Ekstrand, B. R. (1964). Knowledge of rights and wrongs in verbal-discrimination learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3, 183186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, B. J., & Postman, L. (1960). Extraexperimental sources of interference in forgetting. Psychological Review, 67, 7395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, B. J., & Schulz, R. W. (1960). Meaningfulness and verbal learning. Chicago, IL: Lippencott.Google Scholar
Underwood, B. J., & Shaughnessy, J. J. (1975). Experimentation in psychology. Oxford: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Verde, M. F. (2004). Associative interference in recognition memory: A dual-process account. Memory & Cognition, 32, 12731283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voss, J. F. (1971). Are reinforcement concepts able to provide reinforcement for theorizing in human learning (pp. 3744). In Glaser, R. (Ed.), The nature of reinforcement. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Voss, J. F., Thompson, C. P., & Keegan, J. H. (1959). Acquisition of probabilistic paired associates as a function of S-R1 and S-R2 probability. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 390399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagman, J. B. (2002). Symmetry for the sake of symmetry or symmetry for the sake of behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 423424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waldum, E. R., & Sahakyan, L. (2012). A role for memory in prospective timing informs timing in prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 809826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, P., & Cuthbert, L. (1998). Remembering visual feature conjunctions: Visual memory for shape-colour conjunctions is object based. Visual Cognition, 5, 409455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, W. P. (1970). Consistency of emission order in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 5868.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, W. P., Stewart, M. T., & Malone, C. P. (1995). Recognition memory errors produced by implicit activation of word candidates during the processing of spoken words. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 417439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, W. P., Stewart, M. T., Schaffer, T. R., & Wison, J. A. (1998). Are false recognitions influenced by prerecognition processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 299315.Google ScholarPubMed
Warriner, A. B., & Humphreys, K. R. (2008). Learning to fail: Reoccurring tip-of-the-tongue states. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 535542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, O. C., & Watkins, M. J. (1975). Buildup of proactive inhibition as a cue-overload effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 1, 442452.Google Scholar
Weeks, C. S., Cornwell, T. B., & Humphreys, M. S. (2006). Conceptualizing sponsorship: An item and relational information account. In Kahle, L. R. & Kim, Chung-Hyun (Eds.), Creating images and the psychology of marketing communication (pp. 257276). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Weeks, C. S., Humphreys, M. S., & Hockley, W. E. (2007). Buffered forgetting: When targets and distracters are both forgotten. Memory & Cognition, 35, 12671282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, G. L., Steblay, N. K., & Dysart, J. E. (2012). Eyewitness identification reforms: Are suggestiveness-induced hits and guesses true hits? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 264271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickens, C. D., & Hollands, J. G. (1999). Engineering psychology and human performance. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Wickens, D. D. (1970). Encoding categories of words: An empirical approach. Psychological Review, 77, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickens, D. D. (1972). Characteristics of word encoding. In Melton, A. W. & Martin, E. (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp. 191235). Washington, DC: V. H. Winston and Sons.Google Scholar
Wickens, D. D., Born, D. G., & Allen, C. K. (1963). Proactive inhibition and item similarity in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 440445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wicklund, D. A., Palermo, D. S., & Jenkins, J. J. (1964). The effects of associative strength and response hierarchy on paired-associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3, 413420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilton, R. N. (1989). The structure of memory: Evidence concerning the recall of surface and background colour of shapes. Quarterly Journal of Psychology, 41A, 579598.Google Scholar
Wilton, R. N., & Mathieson, P. (1996). The supposed effect of interactive imagery in paired-associate learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 888900.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winograd, E., Karchmer, M. A., & Steele Russell, I. (1971). Role of encoding utilization in cued recognition memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 199206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wixted, J. T. (2004a). On common ground: Jost’s (1897) law of forgetting and Ribot’s (1881) law of retrograde amnesia. Psychological Review, 111, 864879.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wixted, J. T. (2004b). The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting. Annual Review of Psycholology, 55, 235269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wixted, J. T., & Mickes, L. (2010). A continuous dual-process model of remember/know judgments. Psychological Review, 117, 10251054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wixted, J. T., Mickes, L., & Squire, L. R. (2010). Measuring recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe. Hippocampus, 20, 11951205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolford, G. (1971). Function of distinct associations for paired-associate performance. Psychological Review, 78, 303313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, A., Leahy, W., Marcus, N., & Sweller, J. (2012). Cognitive load theory, the transient information effect and e-learning. Learning and Instruction, 22, 449457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodworth, R. S., & Schlosberg, H. (1954). Experimental psychology (Revised Edition). New York: Henry Holt and Company.Google Scholar
Wynn, T. (2002). Archaeology and cognitive evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 389438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wynn, T., & Coolidge, F. L. (2004). The expert neandertal mind. Journal of Human Evolution, 46, 467487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, J., & Malmberg, K. J. (2007). Modeling the effects of verbal and nonverbal pair strength on associative recognition, Memory & Cognition, 35, 526544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yates, F. A. (1992). The art of memory. London: Pimlico.Google Scholar
Yim, H., Dennis, S., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2013). The development of episodic memory: Items, contexts and relations. Psychological Science, 24, 21632172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yonelinas, A. P. (1999). Recognition memory for faces: When familiarity supports associative recognition judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 654661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of thirty years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yonelinas, A. P., & Jacoby, L. L. (1996). Noncriterial recollection: Familiarity as automatic, irrelevant recollection. Consciousness and Cognition, 5(1–2), 131141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yonelinas, A. P., & Levy, B. J. (2002). Dissociating familiarity from recollection in human recognition memory: Different rates of forgetting over short retention intervals. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 575582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, R. K. (1962). Tests of three hypotheses about the effective stimulus in serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 307313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaromb, F. M., Howard, M. W., Dolan, E. D., Tully, M., Wingfield, A., & Kahana, M. J. (2006). Temporal associations and prior-list intrusions in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 792804.Google ScholarPubMed
Zeelenberg, R., Shiffrin, R. M., & Raaijmakers, J. G. (1999). Priming in a free association task as a function of association directionality. Memory & Cognition, 27, 956961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Michael S. Humphreys, University of Queensland, Kerry A. Chalmers, University of Newcastle, New South Wales
  • Book: Thinking About Human Memory
  • Online publication: 05 February 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316091920.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Michael S. Humphreys, University of Queensland, Kerry A. Chalmers, University of Newcastle, New South Wales
  • Book: Thinking About Human Memory
  • Online publication: 05 February 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316091920.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Michael S. Humphreys, University of Queensland, Kerry A. Chalmers, University of Newcastle, New South Wales
  • Book: Thinking About Human Memory
  • Online publication: 05 February 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316091920.010
Available formats
×