Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
  • Cited by 8
Publisher:
Cambridge University Press
Online publication date:
February 2016
Print publication year:
2016
Online ISBN:
9781316091920

Book description

Thinking About Human Memory provides a novel analytical approach to understanding memory that considers the goals of the memory task, the cues and information available, the opportunity to learn, and interference from irrelevant information (noise). Each of the five chapters describing this approach introduces historical ideas and demonstrates how current thinking both differs from and is derived from them. These chapters also contain analyses of current problems designed to demonstrate the power of the approach. In a subsequent chapter, the authors discuss how memory is controlled by the environment, by others, and by ourselves, and then apply their insights to the problem solving of children, our hominin ancestors, and scrub jays. Finally, the questions of how to define episodic memory and how to investigate phylogenetic and developmental changes in memory are addressed. This book will appeal to memory researchers, including applied researchers, and advanced students.

Reviews

‘The authors provide a wide-ranging, scholarly discussion of learning and memory based on a new framework for task analysis. Particularly appealing is the emphasis on a synergistic relationship between controlled laboratory research and studies of everyday memory.’

Reed Hunt - University of Texas, San Antonio

‘Memory research over the last fifty years produced astonishing advances and Michael S. Humphreys stands at the forefront of many new discoveries. This 'must read' book provides a thoughtful analysis of his and related findings and offers new challenges to current thinking about the nature of memory.’

Douglas L. Nelson - Distinguished Research Professor, Emeritus, University of South Florida

'Humphreys and Chalmers provide an in-depth review of more than 100 years (more or less) of experimental psychology research into memory, mostly based in verbal learning research. I recommend this text for anyone who is interested in or studies long-term memory and is interested in understanding the history of this field. The Gold CION analysis provided a solid framework for digesting a great deal of experimental research in memory. I also found the examples in the later chapters to put research and theory into real-world settings are well constructed and helpful.'

Christopher A. Was Source: PsycCRITIQUES

'Humphreys and Chalmers's stimulating book sets out a novel and ambitious approach to thinking about human memory.'

Kourken Michaelian Source: American Journal of Psychology

Refine List

Actions for selected content:

Select all | Deselect all
  • View selected items
  • Export citations
  • Download PDF (zip)
  • Save to Kindle
  • Save to Dropbox
  • Save to Google Drive

Save Search

You can save your searches here and later view and run them again in "My saved searches".

Please provide a title, maximum of 40 characters.
×

Contents

References

Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1996). Two-year-olds learn words for absent objects and actions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 7993.
Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1972). Recognition and retrieval processes in free recall. Psychological Review, 79, 97123.
Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1973). Human associative memory. Washington, DC: V. H. Winston & Sons.
Anderson, J. R., Bothell, D., Lebiere, C., & Matessa, M. (1998). An integrative theory of list memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 341380.
Anderson, M. C., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2000). Retrieval-induced forgetting: Evidence for a recall-specific mechanism. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 522530.
Asch, S. E. (1969). A reformulation of the problem of associations. American Psychologist, 24, 92102.
Atkinson, R. C., & Juola, J. F. (1974). Search and decision processes in recognition memory. In Krantz, D. H., Atkinson, R. C., Luce, R. D., & Suppes, P. (Eds.), Contemporary developments in mathematical psychology: Vol. 1. Learning, memory, and thinking (pp. 243293). San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In Spence, K. W. & Spence, J. T. (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 89195). New York: Academic Press.
Aue, W. R., Criss, A. H., & Fischetti, N. W. (2012). Associative information in memory: Evidence from cued recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 109222.
Averell, L., & Heathcote, A. (2009). Long term implicit and explicit memory for briefly studied words. In Taatgen, A. & van Rijn, H. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 267281). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Averell, L., & Heathcote, A. (2011). The form of the forgetting curve and the fate of memories. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 55, 2535.
Azuma, T., Williams, E. J., & Davie, J. E. (2004). Paws + cause = pause? Memory load and memory blends in homophone recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 723728.
Bain, J. D., & Humphreys, M. S. (1988). Relational context: Independent cues, meanings or configurations? In Davies, G. M. & Thomson, D. M. (Eds.), Memory in context: Context in memory (pp. 97137). Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons.
Bain, J. D., & Humphreys, M. S. (1989). Instructional reinstatement of context: The forgotten prerequisite. In McConkey, K. & Bennett, A. (Eds.), Proceedings of the XXIV international congress of psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 227235). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Baker, L. M., & Elliott, D. N. (1948). Controlled and free association times with identical stimulus and response words. American Journal of Psychology, 61, 535539.
Banaji, M. R., & Crowder, R. G. (1989). The bankruptcy of everyday memory. American Psychologist, 44, 11851193.
Banks, W. P. (1970). Signal detection theory and human memory. Psychological Bulletin, 74, 8199.
Barnes, J. M., & Underwood, B. J. (1959). Fate of first-list associations in transfer theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 97105.
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: An experimental and social study. Cambridge University Press.
Battig, W. F., & Brackett, H. R. (1961). Comparison of anticipation and recall methods in paired-associate learning. Psychological Reports, 1, 5965.
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 12521265.
Begg, I. (1982). Imagery, organization, and discriminative processes. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 273290.
Begg, I. (1983). Imagery instructions and the organization of memory. In Yuille, J. C. (Ed.), Imagery, memory, and cognition (pp. 91115). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bischof, N. (1985). Das Rätzel Ödipus [The Oedipus riddle]. Munich: Piper.
Bischof-Köhler, D. (1985). Zur Phylogenese menschlicher motivation [On the phylogeny of human motivation]. In Eckensberger, L. H. & Lantermann, E. D. (Eds.), Emotion und ReXexivität (pp. 347). Vienna: Urban & Schwarzenberg.
Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1992). A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In Healy, A., Kosslyn, S., & Shiffrin, R. (Eds.), From learning processes to cognitive processes: Essays in honor of William K. Estes (Vol. 2, pp. 3567). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bjork, R. A., Dunlosky, J., & Kornell, N. (2013). Self regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 417444.
Bodner, G. E., & Lindsay, D. S. (2003). Remembering and knowing in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 563580.
Bodner, G. E., & Taikh, A. (2012). Reassessing the basis of the production effect in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 17111719.
Bodner, G. E., Taikh, A., & Fawcett, J. M. (2014). Assessing the costs and benefits of production in recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 149154.
Bower, G. H. (1961). Application of a model to paired-associate learning. Psychometrika, 26, 255280.
Boyer, P. (2009). What are memories for? Functions of recall in cognition and culture. In Boyer, P. & Wertsch, J. V. (Eds.), Memory in mind and culture (pp. 332). Cambridge University Press.
Brady, T. F., Konkle, T., Alvarez, G. A., & Oliva, A. (2008). Visual long-term memory has a massive storage capacity for object details. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 1432514329.
Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Mojardin, A. H. (1999). Conjoint recognition. Psychological Review, 106, 160179.
Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1971). The abstraction of linguistic ideas. Cognitive Psychology, 2, 331350.
Broadbent, D. E. (1971). Decision and stress. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Brooks, L. R., Norman, G. R., & Allen, S. W. (1991). Role of specific similarity in a medical diagnostic task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 278287.
Brown, A. S., Schilling, H. E. H., & Hockensmith, M. L. (1999). The negative suggestion effect: Pondering incorrect alternatives may be hazardous to your knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 756764.
Brown, R., & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5, 7399.
Buchler, N. G., Light, L. L., & Reder, L. M. (2008). Memory for items and associations: Distinct representations and processes in associative recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 183199.
Buchler, N. G., & Reder, L. L. (2007). Modelling age-related memory deficits: A two parameter solution. Psychology and Aging, 22, 104121.
Buchwald, A. M. (1969). Effects of “right” and “wrong” on subsequent behaviour: A new interpretation. Psychological Review, 76, 132143.
Buratto, L. G., & Lamberts, K. (2008). List strength effect without list length effect in recognition memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 218226.
Butterworth, P. (2000). Task by materials interaction characterise unitization in episodic memory. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, the University of Queensland, Australia.
Carey, M., & Reder, L. M. (2003). A dual-process account of the list-length and strength-based mirror effects in recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 231248.
Cassiday, K. W., Kelly, M. H., & Sharoni, L. J. (1999). Inferring gender from name phonology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 362381.
Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., & Wixted, D. R. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354380.
Chalmers, K. A., & Humphreys, M. S. (1998). Role of generalized and episode specific memories in the word frequency effect in recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 610632.
Chalmers, K. A., & Turon, H. E. (2012). Source monitoring for color information: Accuracy is higher for intrinsic than extrinsic features. Abstracts of the 53nd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, 17, 203.
Chappell, M., & Humphreys, M. S. (1994). An auto-associative neural network for sparse representations: Analysis and application to models of recognition and cued recall. Psychological Review, 101, 103128.
Chechile, R. A. (2006). Memory hazard functions: A vehicle for theory development and test. Psychological Review, 113, 3156.
Chechile, R., & Butler, K. (1975). Storage and retrieval changes that occur in development and release of PI. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 430437.
Clare, L., & Jones, R. S. P. (2008). Errorless learning in the rehabilitation of memory impairment: A critical review. Neuropsychology Review, 18, 123.
Clark, S. E. (2012a). Costs and benefits of eyewitness identification reform: Psychological science and public policy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 238259.
Clark, S. E. (2012b). Eyewitness identification reform: Data, theory, and due process. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 279283.
Clayton, N. S., Yu, K. S., & Dickinson, A. (2001). Scrub jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens) form integrated memories of the multiple features of caching episodes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 1729.
Cohn, M., & Moscovitch, M. (2007). Dissociating measures of associative memory: Evidence and theoretical implications. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 436454.
Collier, R. (2012). Hospital-induced delirium hits hard. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 184, 2324.
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407428.
Conway, M. A., Gardiner, J. M., Perfect, T. J., Anderson, S. J., & Cohen, G. M. (1997). Changes in memory awareness during learning: The acquisition of knowledge by psychology undergraduates. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 393413.
Conway, M. A., & Gathercole, S. E. (1987). Modality and long-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 341361.
Coover, J. E. (1927). Metaphysics and the incredulity of psychologists. In Murchison, C. (Ed.), The case for and against psychical belief (pp. 229264). Reprint edition, 1975. New York: Arno Press.
Corballis, M. C. (2002). From hand to mouth: The origins of language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Cornwell, T. B., & Humphreys, M. S. (2013). Memory for sponsorship relationships: A critical juncture in thinking. Psychology & Marketing, 30, 394407.
Coventry, K. R., & Clibbens, J. (2002). Does complex behaviour imply complex cognitive abilities? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 413.
Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671684.
Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268294.
Criss, A. H., Malmberg, K. J., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2011). Output interference in recognition memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 316326.
Criss, A. H., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2004). Context noise and item noise jointly determine recognition memory: A comment on Dennis and Humphreys (2001). Psychological Review, 111, 800807.
D’Argembeau, A. D., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 342350.
Dalton, P. (1993). The role of stimulus familiarity in context-dependent recognition. Memory & Cognition, 21, 223234.
Davidson, E. H., & Cofer, C. N. (1968). Some determinants of controlled association times. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78, 200207.
Davis, K. M., & Benjamin, A. S. (2014). Retrieval speeds context fluctuation: Why semantic generation enhances later learning but hinders prior learning. Memory & Cognition, 42, 10491062.
Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 1722.
Dehue, T. (2000). From deception trials to control reagents: The introduction of the control group about a century ago. American Psychologist, 55, 264268.
Dennis, S., & Chapman, A. (2010). The inverse list length effect: A challenge for pure exemplar models of recognition memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 416424.
Dennis, S., & Humphreys, M. S. (2001). A context noise model of episodic recognition memory. Psychological Review, 108, 452478.
Dennis, S., Lee, M. D., & Kinnell, A. (2008). Bayesian analysis of recognition memory: The case of the list-length effect. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 361376.
Dessalles, J. L. (2007). Storing events to retell them. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 321322.
Dillon, R. F., & Bittner, L. A. (1975). Analysis of retrieval cues and release from proactive inhibition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 616622.
Dodson, C. S., & Schacter, D. L. (2001). “If I had said it I would have remembered it”: Reducing false memories with a distinctiveness heuristic. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 155161.
Dommeyer, F. C. (1975). Psychical research at Stanford University. The Journal of Parapsychology, 39, 173205.
Dosher, B. A., & Rosedale, G. S. (1991). Judgments of semantic and episodic relatedness: Common time-course and failure of segregation. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 125160.
Dubé, C., Payne, L., Sekuler, R., & Rotello, C. M. (2013). Paying attention to attention in recognition memory: Insights from models and electrophysiology. Psychological Science, 24, 23982408.
Dulaney, D. E. Jr. (1962). The place of hypotheses and intentions. In Eriksen, C. W. (Ed.), Behavior and awareness (pp. 102129). Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Dunn, J. C. (2004). Remember know: A matter of confidence. Psychological Review, 111, 524542.
Dunn, J. C. (2008). The dimensionality of the remember-know task: A state-trace analysis. Psychological Review, 115, 426446.
Dyne, A. M. (1992). An evaluation of the global matching models of recognition memory. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, the University of Queensland, Australia.
Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. New York: Dover.
Eckert, E., & Kanak, J. N. (1974). Verbal discrimination learning: A review of the acquisition, transfer, and retention literature. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 582607.
Egan, J. P. (1958). Recognition memory and the operating characteristic (Tech. Note AFCRC-TN-58-51). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University, Hearing and Communication Laboratory.
Eich, J. M. (1982). A composite holographic associative recall model. Psychological Review, 89, 627661.
Ekstrand, B. R., Wallace, W. P., & Underwood, B. J. (1966). A frequency theory of verbal discrimination learning. Psychological Review, 73, 566578.
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211245.
Esrov, L. V. (1974). A frequency interpretation of the Bransford and Franks’ effect. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA.
Estes, W. K. (1950). Towards a statistical theory of learning. Psychological Review, 57, 94107.
Estes, W. K. (1976). The cognitive side of probability learning. Psychological Review, 83, 3764.
Fawcett, J. M. (2013). The production effect benefits performance in between-subject designs: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychologia, 142, 15.
Fazio, L. K., Barber, S. J., Rajaram, S., Ornstein, P. A., & Marsh, E. J. (2013). Creating illusions of knowledge: Learning errors that contradict prior knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 15.
Fernandez, A., & Glenberg, A. M. (1985). Changing environmental context does not reliably affect memory. Memory & Cognition, 13, 333345.
Finkenauer, C., Luminet, O., Gisle, L., El-Ahmadi, A., van der Linden, M., & Philippot, P. (1998). Flashbulb memories and the underlying mechanisms of their formation: Toward an emotional integrative model. Memory & Cognition, 26, 516531.
Flexser, A. L., & Tulving, E. (1978). Retrieval independence in recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 85, 153171.
Frank, M. J., Seeberger, L. C., & O’Reilly, R. C. (2004). By carrot or by stick: Cognitive reinforcement learning in Parkinsonism. Science, 306, 19401943.
Friedman, W. J. (2005). Developmental and cognitive perspectives on humans’ sense of the times of past and future events. Learning and Motivation, 36, 145158.
Gardiner, J. M. (1988). Functional aspects or recollective experience. Memory & Cognition, 16, 309313.
Gardiner, J. M., Craik, E. I. M., & Birtwistle, J. (1972). Retrieval cues and release from proactive inhibition. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 11, 778783.
Garry, M., Manning, C. G., Loftus, E. F., & Sherman, S. J. (1996). Imagination inflation: Imagining a childhood event inflates confidence that it occurred. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 208214.
Gathercole, S. E., & Conway, M. A. (1988). Exploring long-term modality effects: Vocalization leads to best retention. Memory & Cognition, 16, 110119.
Gillund, G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 91, 167.
Giovanello, K. S., Keane, M. M., & Verfaille, M. (2006). The contribution of familiarity to associative memory in amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 44, 18591865.
Glanzer, M., & Adams, J. K. (1985). The mirror effect in recognition memory. Memory & Cognition, 13, 820.
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 155.
Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1975). Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: Land and underwater. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 325331.
Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1980). When does context influence recognition memory? British Journal of Psychology, 71, 99104.
Gopher, D., & Kimchi, R. (1989). Engineering psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 43l455.
Gopher, D., & Koriat, A. (1999). Bridging the gap between basic and applied research on the cognitive regulation of performance. In Gopher, D. & Koriat, A. (Eds.), Attention and performance XVII – Cognitive regulation of performance: Theory and application (pp. 314). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gorfein, D. S. (1987). Explaining context effects in short-term memory. In Gorfein, D. S. & Hoffman, R. R. (Eds.), Memory and learning: The Ebbinghaus Centennial conference (pp. 153172). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Goss, A. E., & Nodine, C. F. (1965). Paired-associates learning: The role of meaningfulness, similarity, and familiarization. New York: Academic Press.
Greenberg, D. L., & Knowlton, B. J. (2014). The role of visual imagery in autobiographical memory. Memory & Cognition, 42, 922934.
Greene, R. L. (1989). Immediate serial recall of mixed-modality lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 266274.
Grimaldi, P. J., & Karpicke, J. D. (2012). When and why do retrieval attempts enhance subsequent encoding? Memory & Cognition, 40, 505513.
Guthrie, E. R. (1940). Association and the law of effect. Psychological Review, 47, 127148.
Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803865.
Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (2010). Relational knowledge: The foundation of higher cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 497505.
Hall, J. W., Humphreys, M. S., & Wilson, K. P. (1983). Differences in long-term retention in relation to early school achievement. American Journal of Psychology, 96, 267287.
Handy, J. D., & Smith, S. M. (2012). Triggering memory recovery: Effects of direct and incidental cueing. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 17111724.
Hasher, L., Goldstein, D., & Toppino, T. (1977). Frequency and the conference of referential validity. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 107112.
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. (1984). Automatic processing of fundamental information: The case of frequency of occurrence. American Psychologist, 39, 13721388.
Haskins, A. L., Yonelinas, A. P., Quamme, J. R., & Ranganath, C. (2008). Perirhinal cortex supports encoding and familiarity-based recognition of novel associations. Neuron, 59, 554560.
Hays, M. J., Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2013). When and why a failed test potentiates the effectiveness of subsequent study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 39, 290296.
Heathcote, A., Raymond, F., & Dunn, J. (2006). Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: Evidence from ROC curves. Journal of Memory & Language, 55, 495514.
Henson, R. N., Eckstein, D., Waszak, F., Frings, C., & Horner, A. J. (2014). Stimulus-response bindings in priming. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 376384.
Herbert, D. M. B., & Burt, J. S. (2004). What do students remember? Episodic memory and the development of schematization. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 7788.
Higham, P. A., & Vokey, J. R. (2004). Illusory recollection and dual process models of recognition memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 57, 714744.
Hintzman, D. L. (1984). MINERVA 2: A simulation model of human memory. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 16, 96101.
Hintzman, D. L. (1986). Schema abstraction in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, 93, 411428.
Hintzman, D. L. (1993). Twenty-five years of learning and memory: Was the cognitive revolution a mistake? In Meyer, D. E. & Kornblum, S (Eds.), Attention and performance X1V (pp. 359391). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.
Hintzman, D. L., Curran, T., & Oppy, B. (1992). Effects of similarity and repetition on memory: Registration without learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 667680.
Hintzman, D. L., Summers, J. J., Eki, N. T., & Moore, M. D. (1975). Voluntary attention and the spacing effect. Memory, and Cognition, 3, 576580.
Hockley, W. E. (1991). Recognition memory for item and associative information: A comparison of forgetting rates. In Hockley, W. E. & Lewandowsky, S. (Eds.), Relating theory and data: Essays on human memory in honor of Bennet B. Murdock (pp. 227248). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hockley, W. E. (1992). Item versus associative information: Further comparisons of forgetting rates. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 13211330.
Hockley, W. E. (2008). The effects of environmental context on recognition memory and claims of remembering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 14121429.
Hopkins, R. H., & Edwards, R. E. (1972). Pronunciation effects in recognition memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 534537.
Horowitz, L. W., & Manelis, L. (1972). Toward a theory of redintegrative memory: Adjective-noun phrases. In Bower, G. H. & Spence, J. T. (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 6, pp. 193224). Oxford: Academic Press.
Howard, M. W., Jing, B., Rao, V. A., Provyn, J. P., & Datey, A. V. (2009). Bridging the gap: Transitive associations between items presented in similar temporal contexts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 391407.
Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (1999). Contextual variability and serial position effects in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 923941.
Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (2002). A distributed representation of temporal context. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 46, 269299.
Huang, T., Loft, S., & Humphreys, M. S. (2014). Internalizing versus externalizing control: Different ways to perform a time-based prospective memory task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 10641071.
Hull, C. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Hull, C. L. (1952). A behavior system: An introduction to behavior theory concerning the individual organism. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Humphreys, K. R., Menzies, H., & Lake, J. K. (2010). Repeated speech errors: Evidence for learning. Cognition, 117, 151165.
Humphreys, M. S. (1972). Recall and repetition in modified right-wrong paradigm. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 573582.
Humphreys, M. S. (1975). The derivation of endpoint and distance effects in linear orderings from frequency information. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 496505.
Humphreys, M. S. (1976a). Frequency and recency in cued recall: The interaction of old and new learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 413422.
Humphreys, M. S. (1976b). Relational information and the context effect in recognition memory. Memory & Cognition, 4, 221232.
Humphreys, M. S. (1978). Item and relational information: A case for context independent retrieval. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 17, 179187.
Humphreys, M. S., & Bain, J. D. (1983). Recognition memory: A cue and information analysis. Memory & Cognition, 11, 583600.
Humphreys, M. S., & Bain, J. D. (1985). Cue repetition and retrieval blocking. American Journal of Psychology, 98, 559568.
Humphreys, M. S., Bain, J. D., & Burt, J. S. (1989). Episodically unique and generalized memories: Applications to human and animal amnesics. In Lewandowsky, S., Dunn, J. C. & Kirsner, K. (Eds.), Implicit memory: Theoretical issues (pp. 139158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Humphreys, M. S., Bain, J. D., & Pike, R. (1989). Different ways to cue a coherent memory system: A theory for episodic, semantic and procedural tasks. Psychological Review, 96, 208233.
Humphreys, M. S., & Bowyer, P. A. (1981). Cued recall and cue recognition: How similar are the processes. American Journal of Psychology, 94, 6584.
Humphreys, M. S., Burt, J. S., & Lawrence, S. (2001). Expecting dirt but saying dart: The creation of a blend memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 820826.
Humphreys, M. S., Cornwell, T. B., McAlister, A. R., Kelly, S. J., Quinn, E. A., & Murray, K. L. (2010). Sponsorship, ambushing and counter-strategy: Effects upon memory for sponsor and event. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 16, 96108.
Humphreys, M. S., Dennis, S., Chalmers, K. A., & Finnigan, S. (2000). Dual processes in recognition: Does a focus on measurement operations provide a sufficient foundation? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 593603.
Humphreys, M. S., & Galbraith, R. C. (1975). Forward and backward associations in cued recall: Predictions from the encoding specificity principle. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1, 702710.
Humphreys, M. S., & Greeno, J. G. (1970). Interpretation of the two-stage analysis of paired associated memorizing. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 7, 275292.
Humphreys, M. S., & Kashima, Y. (2002). Connectionism and self: Distributed representational systems and their implications for self and identity. In Kashima, Y., Foddy, M., & Platow, M.(Eds.), Self and identity: Personal, social, and symbolic (pp. 2754). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Humphreys, M. S., Maguire, A. M., McFarlane, K. A., Burt, J. S., Bolland, S. W., Murray, K. L., & Dunn, R. (2010). Using maintenance rehearsal to explore recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 147159.
Humphreys, M. S., & Murray, K. L. (2011). Episodic, generalized, and semantic memory tests: Switching and strength effects. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 65, 192199.
Humphreys, M. S., Murray, K. L., & Koh, J. Y. (2014). Reinstating higher order properties of a study list by retrieving a list item. Memory & Cognition, 42, 570582.
Humphreys, M. S., Murray, K. L., & Maguire, A. M. (2009). Contexts and control operations used in accessing list-specific, generalized, and semantic memories. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 311337.
Humphreys, M. S., Pike, R., Bain, J. D., & Tehan, G. (1989). Global matching: A comparison of the SAM, Minerva II, Matrix, and TODAM models. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 33, 3667.
Humphreys, M. S., Tangen, J. M., Cornwell, T. B., Quinn, E. A., & Murray, K. L. (2010). Unintended effects of memory on decision making: A breakdown in access control. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 400415.
Humphreys, M. S., Tehan, G., O’Shea, A., & Bolland, S. W. (2000). Target similarity effects: Support for the parallel distributed processing assumptions. Memory & Cognition, 28, 798811.
Humphreys, M. S., Wiles, J., & Bain, J. D. (1993). Memory retrieval with two cues: Think of intersecting sets. In Meyer, D. E. & Kornblum, S. (Eds.), Attention and performance XIV: Synergies in experimental psychology, artificial intelligence, and cognitive neuropsychology–A Silver jubilee (pp. 489508). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Humphreys, M. S., Wiles, J., & Dennis, S. (1994). Toward a theory of human memory: Data structures and access processes. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 17, 655692.
Hunt, R. R. (1995). The subtlety of distinctiveness: What von Restorff really did. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 105112.
Hunt, R. R. (2006). The concept of distinctiveness in memory research. In Hunt, R. R. & Worthen, J. B. (Eds.), Distinctiveness and memory (pp. 325). New York: Oxford University Press.
Hunt, R. R. (2013). Precision in memory through distinctive processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 1015.
Hunt, R. R., & Einstein, G. O. (1981). Relational and item specific information in memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 497514.
Hunt, R. R., & Lamb, C. A. (2001). What causes the isolation effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 13591366.
Isaac, G. (1978). The food sharing behavior of protohuman hominids. Scientific American, 238(4), 90108.
Izawa, C. (1971). Massed and spaced practice in paired-associate learning: List versus item distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 1021.
Izawa, C. (1977). Theoretical and empirical performance differences between study-test and anticipation methods: Retention interval hypothesis versus spaced practice effects. Japanese Psychological Research, 19, 3138.
Izawa, C. (1985). A test of the differences between anticipation and study-test methods of paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 165184.
Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513541.
Jacoby, L. L., Bishara, A. J., & Hessels, S. (2007). Probabilistic retroactive interference. The role of accessibility bias in interference effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 200216.
Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306340.
Jacoby, L. L., Debner, J. A., & Hay, J. F. (2001). Proactive interference, accessibility bias, and process dissociations: Valid subjective reports of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 686700.
Jacoby, L. L., & Hollingshead, A. (1990). Toward a generate/recognize model of performance on direct and indirect tests of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 433454.
Jang, Y., & Huber, D. E. (2008). Context retrieval and context change in free recall: Recalling from long-term memory drives list isolation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 112127.
Johansson, P., Hall, L., & Sikström, S., & Olsson, A. (2005). Failure to detect mismatches between intention and outcome in a simple decision task. Science, 310, 116119.
Johnson, G. J. (1991). A distinctiveness model of serial learning. Psychological Review, 98, 204217.
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 328.
Johnston, W. A., Hawley, K. J., & Elliott, J. M. G. (1991). Contribution of perceptual fluency to recognition judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 210223.
Jordan, M. I., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1992). Forward models: Supervised learning with a distal teacher. Cognitive Science, 16, 307354.
Jung, J. (1962). Transfer of training as a function of degree of first-list learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1, 197199.
Kalish, M., Griffiths, T. L., & Lewandowsky, S. (2007). Iterated learning: Intergenerational knowledge transmission reveals inductive biases. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 288294.
Kausler, D. H. (1974). Psychology of verbal learning and memory. New York: Academic Press.
Kello, C. T., & Plaut, D. C. (2000). Strategic control in word reading: Evidence from speeded responding in the tempo-naming task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 719750.
Keppel, G., & Underwood, B. J. (1962). Proactive inhibition in short-term retention of single items. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1, 153161.
Kimball, D. R., Sloutsky, V. M., & Kahana, M. J. (2007). The fSAM model of false recall. Psychological Review, 114, 954993.
Kinnell, A., & Dennis, S. (2011). The list length effect in recognition memory: An analysis of potential confounds. Memory & Cognition, 39, 348363.
Kinnell, A., & Dennis, S. (2012). The role of stimulus type in list length effects in recognition memory. Memory & Cognition, 40, 311325.
Kintsch, W. (1967). Learning, memory, and conceptual processes. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Kintsch, W. (1970). Models for free recall and recognition. In Norman, D. A. (Ed.), Models of human memory (pp. 331373). New York: Academic Press.
Klatzky, R. L. (1984). Armchair theorists have more fun. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 244.
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (2002). Memory and temporal experience: The effects of episodic memory loss. Social Cognition, 20, 353379.
Kohlers, W. (1941). On the nature of associations. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 84, 489502.
Kornell, N., Hays, M. J., & Bjork, R. A. (2009). Unsuccessful retrieval attempts enhance subsequent learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 989998.
Lachman, R., & Naus, M. (1984). The episodic/semantic continuum in an evolved machine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 244246.
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211240.
Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 175.
Lewandowsky, S., Dunn, J. C., & Kirsner, K. (1989). Implicit memory: Theoretical issues. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lewandowsky, S., Geiger, S. M., & Oberauer, K. (2008). Interference-based forgetting in verbal short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 200222.
Light, L. L., & Carter-Sobell, L. (1970). Effects of changed semantic context on recognition memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 9, 111.
Lloyd, M. E., Hartman, A., Ngo, C. T., Ruser, N., Westerman, D. L., & Miller, J. K. (2015). Not enough familiarity for fluency: Definitional encoding increases familiarity but does not lead to fluency attribution in associative recognition. Memory & Cognition, 43, 3948.
Loft, S., Humphreys, M., & Neal, A. (2004). The influence of memory for prior instances on performance in a conflict detection task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 173187.
Loft, S., Humphreys, M. S., & Whitney, S. J. (2008). Control of access to memory: The use of task interference as a behavioral probe. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 465479.
Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 1931.
Logan, F. A. (1960). Incentive. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Lowndes, G., & Savage, G. (2007). Early detection of memory impairment in Alzheimer’s disease: A neurocognitve perspective on assessment. Neuropsychological Review, 17, 193202.
MacDonald, P. A., & MacLeod, C. M. (1998). The influence of attention at encoding on direct and indirect remembering. Acta Psychologica, 98, 291310.
MacLeod, C. M., Gopie, N., Hourihan, K. L., Neary, K. R., & Ozubko, J. D. (2010). The production effect: Delineation of a problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 671685.
Maguire, A. M. (2005). False alarms in episodic recognition: An examination of base-rate, similarity-based, and comprehensive theories. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Queensland, Australia.
Malmberg, K. J. (2008). Recognition memory: A review of the critical findings and an integrated theory for relating them. Cognitive Psychology, 57, 335384.
Malmberg, K. J., & Nelson, T. O. (2003). The word frequency effect for recognition memory and the elevated-attention hypothesis. Memory & Cognition, 31, 3543.
Malmberg, K. J., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2005). The “one-shot” hypothesis for context storage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 322336.
Mandler, G. (1967). Organization and memory. In Spence, K. W. & Spence, J. T. (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 1. Advances in research and theory (pp. 328372). New York: Academic Press.
Mandler, G. (1972). Organization and recognition. In Tulving, E. & Donaldson, W. (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 146165). New York: Academic Press.
Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of previous experience. Psychological Review, 87, 252271.
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. New York: Freeman.
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1990). Activation, competition, and frequency in lexical access. In Altmann, G. T. M. (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives (pp. 148172). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Martin, E. (1968). Stimulus meaningfulness and paired-associate transfer: An encoding variability hypothesis. Psychological Review, 75, 421441.
Masters, R., & Maxwell, J. (2002). Was early man caught knapping during the cognitive (r)evolution? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 413.
Mather, M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: An object based framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 3352.
Mather, M., Henkel, L. A., & Johnson, M. K. (1997). Evaluating characteristics of false memories: Remember/know judgments and memory characteristics questionnaire compared. Memory & Cognition, 25, 826837.
Matute, H., Lipp, O. V., Vadillo, M. A., & Humphreys, M. S. (2011). Temporal contexts: Filling the gap between episodic memory and associative learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 660673.
Mayes, A., Montaldi, D., & Migo, E. (2007). Associative memory and the medial temporal lobes. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11(3), 126135.
McAlister, A. R., Kelly, S. J., Humphreys, M. S., & Cornwell, T. B. (2012). Changes in sponsorship alliance and the communication implications of spontaneous recovery. Journal of Advertising, 41, 516.
McBride, D. M., & Dosher, B. A. (1997). A comparison of forgetting in an implicit and explicit memory task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(4), 371392.
McClelland, J. L. (2001). Failures to learn and their remediation: A Hebbian account. In McClelland, J. L. & Siegler, R. S. (Eds.), Mechanisms of cognitive development: Behavioral and neural perspectives. Carnegie Mellon symposia on cognition (pp. 97121). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McClelland, J. L. (2013). Incorporating rapid neocortical learning of new schema-consistent information into complementary learning systems theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 11901210.
McClelland, J. L., & Chappell, M. (1998). Familiarity breeds differentiation: A subjective-likelihood approach to the effects of experience in recognition memory. Psychological Review, 105, 724760.
McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O’Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the Hippocampus and Neocortex: Insights from the success and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102, 419457.
McCloskey, M., & Cohen, N. J. (1989). Catastrophic forgetting in connectionist networks: The problem of sequential learning. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 20, pp. 109165). New York: Academic Press.
McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, M. (1985). Misleading postevent information and memory for events: Arguments and evidence against memory impairment hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 116.
McCullers, J. C. (1965). Type of associative interference as a factor in verbal paired-associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 1216.
McElree, B. (2006). Accessing recent events. In Ross, B. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 46, pp. 155200). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
McFarlane, K. M., & Humphreys, M. S. (2012). Maintenance-rehearsal: The key to the role attention plays in storage and forgetting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 10011018.
McGovern, J. B. (1964). Extinction of associations in four transfer paradigms. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 78(16), 593.
Melton, A. W. (1963). Implications of Short-term memory for a general theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 121.
Mensink, G. J. M., & Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (1988). A model for interference and forgetting. Psychological Review, 95, 434455.
Mercado, E., Murray, S., Uyeyama, R., Pack, A. A., & Herman, L. M. (1998). Memory for recent actions in the bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): Repetition of arbitrary behaviors using an abstract rule. Animal Learning & Behavior, 26, 210218.
Metcalfe, J. (1990). A composite holographic associative recall model (CHARM) and blended memories eyewitness testimony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 145160.
Mewhort, D. J. K., & Johns, E. E. (2000). The extralist-feature effect: Evidence against item matching in short-term recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 129, 262284.
Mickes, L., Seal-Carlisle, T. M., & Wixted, J. T. (2013). Rethinking familiarity: Remember/Know judgments in free recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 333349.
Miller, G. A., & Selfridge, J. A. (1950). Verbal context and the recall of meaningful material. American Journal of Psychology, 63, 176185.
Monsell, S. (1991). The nature of the locus of word frequency effects in reading. In Besner, D. & Humphreys, G. W. (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 148197). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mullet, H. G., Umanath, S., & Marsh, E. J. (2014). Recent study, but not retrieval, of knowledge protects against learning errors. Memory & Cognition, 42, 12391249.
Mulligan, N. W. (2008). Attention and memory. In Roediger, H. L. (Ed.), Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference (pp. 722). Oxford: Elsevier.
Mulligan, N. W., & Osborn, K. (2009). The modality-match effect in recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 564571.
Mulligan, N. W., Spataro, P., & Picklesimer, M. (2014). The attentional boost effect with verbal materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 10491063.
Murchison, C. (1927). The case for and against psychical belief. Reprint edition, 1975. New York: Arno Press, Inc.
Murdock, B. Jr B. (1969). Modality effects in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 665676.
Murdock, B. B. (1974). Human memory: Theory and data. New York: Wiley.
Murdock, B. B. (1982). A theory for the storage and retrieval of item and associative information. Psychological Review, 89, 609626.
Murdock, B. B. (1993). Derivations for the chunking model. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 37, 421445.
Murnane, K., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1991). Interference and the representation of events in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 855874.
Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2000). Adult age differences in memory performance: Tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 11701187.
Naveh-Benjamin, M., Hussain, Z., Guez, J., & Bar-On, M. (2003). Adult age differences in episodic memory: Further support for an associative-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 826837.
Neisser, U. (1978). Memory: What are the important questions? In Gruneberg, M. M., Morris, P. E., & Sykes, R. N. (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 324). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Nelson, A. M., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2013). The co-evolution of knowledge and event memory. Psychological Review, 120, 356394.
Nelson, D. L., Bajo, M. T., & Casanueva, D. (1985). Prior knowledge: The influence of natural category size as a function of intention and interval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 94105.
Nelson, D. L., & Brooks, D. H. (1974). Relative effectiveness of rhymes and synonyms as retrieval cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 503507.
Nelson, D. L., & Friedrich, M. A. (1980). Encoding and cueing sounds and senses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning and Memory, 6, 717731.
Nelson, D. L., & Goodmon, L. B. (2002). Experiencing a word can prime its accessibility and its associative connections to related words. Memory & Cognition, 30, 380398.
Nelson, D. L., Goodmon, L. B., & Ceo, D. (2007). How does delayed testing reduce effects of implicit memory: Context infusion or cuing with context? Memory & Cognition, 35, 10141043.
Nelson, D. L., Kitto, K., Galea, D., McEvoy, C. L., & Bruza, P. D. (2013). How activation, entanglement, and searching a semantic network contribute to event memory. Memory & Cognition, 41, 797819.
Nelson, D. L., & McEvoy, C. L. (1979). Encoding context and set size. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 292314.
Nelson, D. L., McKinney, V. M., Gee, N. R., & Janczura, G. A. (1998). Interpreting the influence of implicitly activated memories on recall and recognition. Psychological Review, 105, 299324.
Nelson, D. L., Schreiber, T. A., & McEvoy, C. L. (1992). Processing implicit and explicit representations. Psychological Review, 99, 322348.
Nelson, D. L., Walling, J. R., & McEvoy, C. L. (1979). Doubts about depth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 2444.
Nelson, D. L., Wheeler, J. W., Borden, R. C., & Brooks, D. H. (1974). Levels of processing and cuing: Sensory versus meaning features. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 971977.
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111, 486511.
Nelson, T. O. (1977). Repetition and depth of processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 151171.
Newman, F. J., & Loftus, E. F. (2012). Clarkian logic on trial. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 260.
Nuttin, J., & Greenwald, A. G. (1968). Reward and punishment in human learning: Elements of a behaviour theory. Academic Press: London.
Osgood, C. E. (1949). The similarity paradox in human learning: A resolution. Psychological Review, 56, 132143.
Osth, A. F., & Dennis, S. (2015). Sources of interference in item and associative recognition memory. Psychological Review, 122, 260311.
Overton, R. C., & Adolphson, C. J. (1979). Multiple trace retrieval: A trace-to-trace retrieval process? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 485495.
Öztekin, I., Davachi, L., & McElree, B. (2010). Are representations in working memory distinct from representations in long-term memory? Neural evidence in support of a single store. Psychological Science, 21, 11231133.
Palermo, D. S., & Jenkins, J. J. (1964). Paired-associate learning as a function of the strength of links in the associative chain. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3, 406412.
Pavio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Peterson, W. W., Birdsall, T. G., & Fox, W. C. (1954). The theory of signal detectability. Proceedings of the IRE Professional Group on Information Theory, 4, 171212.
Pike, R. (1984). Comparison of convolution and matrix distributed memory systems for associative recall and recognition. Psychological Review, 91, 281294.
Poldrack, R. A., & Logan, G. (1997). Fluency and response speed in recognition judgments. Memory & Cognition, 25, 110.
Postman, L., & Gray, W. D. (1979). Does imaginal encoding increase resistance to interference? American Journal of Psychology, 92, 215233.
Postman, L., & Keppel, G. (1977). Conditions of cumulative proactive inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 376403.
Postman, L., & Riley, D. A. (1957). A critique of Kohler’s theory of association. Psychological Review, 64, 6172.
Postman, L., & Sassenrath, J. (1961). The automatic action of verbal rewards and punishments. Journal of General Psychology, 65, 109136.
Postman, L., & Underwood, B. J. (1973). Critical issues in interference theory. Memory & Cognition, 1, 1940.
Potts, R. (1988). Early hominid activities at Olduvai. New York: Aldine de Gruyter, Hawthorne.
Pritchard, M. E., & Keenan, J. M. (2002). Does jury deliberation really improve jurors’ memories? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 589601.
Puff, C. R. (1979). Memory organization and structure. New York: Academic Press.
Quamme, J. R., Yonelinas, A. P., & Norman, K. A. (2007). Effect of unitization on associative recognition in amnesia. Hippocampus, 17, 192200.
Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981). Search for associative memory. Psychological Review, 88, 93134.
Ratcliff, R., Clark, S. E., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1990). List-strength effect: I. Data and discussion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 163178.
Reber, A. S. (1969). Response perseveration in binary-choice recognition! Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 206214.
Reder, L. M., Nhouyvanisvong, A., Schunn, C. D., Ayers, M. S., Angstadt, P., & Hiraki, K. (2000). A mechanistic account of the mirror effect for word frequency: A computational model of Remember-Know judgments in a continuous recognition paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 294320.
Reitman, J. S., & Bower, G. H. (1973). Storage and later recognition of exemplars of concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 194206.
Renoult, L., Davidson, P. S. R., Palombo, D. J., Moscovitch, M., & Levine, B. (2012). Personal semantics: At the crossroads of semantic and episodic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 550558.
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F. (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 6499). New York: Appleton Century Crofts.
Rock, I. (1957). The role of repetition in associative learning. American Journal of Psychology, 70, 186193.
Roediger, H. L. III, & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 181210.
Roediger, H. L. III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803814.
Roediger, H. L. III, Putnam, A. L., & Smith, M. A. (2011). Ten benefits of testing and their applications to educational practice. In Mestre, J. P. & Ross, B. H., Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Cognition in Education, 53, 136.
Roediger, H. L. III, Weldon, M. S., Stadler, M. L., & Riegler, J. L. (1992). Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: Word fragment and word stem completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 12511269.
Rogers, T. T., & McClelland, J. L. (2004). Semantic cognition: A parallel distributed processing approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Rubin, D. C. (1995). Memory in oral traditions: The cognitive psychology of epic, ballads, and counting out rhymes. New York: Oxford University Press.
Rubin, D. C. (2006). The basic-systems model of episodic memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 277311.
Rubin, D. C., Hinton, S., & Wenzel, A. E. (1999). The precise time course of retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 5, 11611176.
Rubin, D. C., & Wallace, W. T. (1989). Rhyme and reason: Analysis of dual retrieval cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 698709.
Rundus, D. (1973). Negative effects of using list items as cues. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 4350.
Runquist, W. N. (1986). The effect of testing on the forgetting of related and unrelated associates. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 40, 6576.
Sahakyan, L., & Delaney, P. F. (2003). Can encoding differences explain the benefits of directed forgetting in the list method paradigm? Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 195206.
Sahakyan, L., & Delaney, P. F. (2005). Directed forgetting in incidental learning and recognition testing: Support for a two-factor account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 789801.
Sahakyan, L., & Kelley, C. M. (2002). A contextual change account of the directed forgetting effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 10641072.
Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362, 773786.
Schacter, D. L., & Wiseman, A. L. (2006). Reducing memory errors: The distinctiveness heuristic. In Hunt, R. R. & Worthen, J. B. (Eds.), Distinctiveness and memory (pp. 89107). New York: Oxford University Press.
Schlanger, N. (1996). Understanding Levallois: Lithic technology and cognitive architecture. Cambridge Archeological Journal, 6, 231254.
Schooler, J. W., Gerhard, D., & Loftus, E. F. (1986). Qualities of the unreal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 171181.
Schwartz, G., Howard, M. W., Jing, B., & Kahana, M. J. (2005). Shadows of the past: Temporal retrieval effects in recognition memory. Psychological Science, 16, 898904.
Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 20, 1121.
Sederberg, P. B., Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (2008). A context based theory of recency and contiguity in free recall. Psychological Review, 115, 893912.
Sheldon, S., & Moscovitch, M. (2012). The nature and time-course of medial temporal lobe contributions to semantic retrieval: An fMRI study on verbal fluency. Hippocampus, 22, 14511466.
Shepard, R. N., & Teightsoonian, M. (1961). Retention of information under conditions approaching a steady state. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 302309.
Shiffrin, R. M., Ratcliff, R., & Clark, S. E. (1990). List-strength effect: II. Theoretical mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 179195.
Shiffrin, R. M., & Steyvers, M. (1997). A model for recognition memory: REM–retrieving effectively from memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 145166.
Shuell, T. J. (1968). Retroactive inhibition free-recall learning of categorized lists. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, 797805.
Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1998). Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 644649.
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century.
Skinner, B. F. (1958). Teaching machines. Science, 128, 969977.
Slamecka, N. J. (1966). Differentiation versus unlearning of verbal associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 822828.
Slamecka, N. J., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 592604.
Slotnick, S. D., & Dodson, C. S. (2005). Support for a continuous (single-process) model of recognition memory and source memory. Memory & Cognition, 33, 151170.
Smith, C. N., Wixted, J. T., & Squire, L. R. (2011). The hippocampus supports both recollection and familiarity when memory is strong. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1569315702.
Smith, S. M. (1988). Environmental context-dependent memory. In Davies, G. M. & Thomson, D. M. (Eds.), Memory in context: Context in memory (pp. 1334). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Smith, S. M. (1994). Theoretical principles of context-dependent memory. In Morris, P. & Gruneberg, M. (Eds.), Theoretical aspects of memory (2nd ed., pp. 168195). New York: Routledge.
Smith, S. M., & Moynan, S. C. (2008). Forgetting and recovering the unforgettable. Psychological Science, 19, 462468.
Smith, S. M., & Vela, E. (2001). Environmental context-dependent memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 203220.
Snodgrass, J. G., Hirshman, E., & Fan, J. (1996). The sensory match effect in recognition memory: Perceptual fluency or episodic trace? Memory & Cognition, 24, 367383.
Spark, L. C. (2007). “Ask me again”: Differential effects of repeated questioning on the recognition and endorsement of autobiographical childhood events. Unpublished honours thesis, University of Queensland, Australia.
Spataro, P., Mulligan, N. W., & Rossi-Arnaud, C. (2013). Divided attention can enhance memory encoding: The attentional boost effect in implicit memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 12231231.
Spence, J. T., & Lair, C. V. (1965). Associative interference in the paired associate learning of remitted and nonremitted schizophrenics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 70, 119122.
Spence, K. W. (1956). Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Spielberger, C. D. (1962). The role of awareness in verbal conditioning. In Eriksen, C. W. (Ed.), Behavior and awareness (pp. 73101). Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Spielberger, C. D., Bernstein, I. H., & Ratliff, R. G. (1966). Information and the incentive value of the reinforcing stimulus in verbal conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 2631.
Staats, O. P. (1985). Associative processing during type 1 rehearsal. Unpublished honours thesis, University of Queensland, Australia.
Stark, C. E. L., & Okado, Y. (2003). Making memories without trying: Medial temporal lobe activity associated with incidental memory formation during recognition. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 67486753.
Starns, J. J., & Hicks, J. L. (2013). Internal reinstatement hides cueing effects in source memory tasks. Memory and Cognition, 41, 953966.
Stone, V. E. (2002). Footloose and fossil-free no more: Evolutionary psychology needs archaeology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 421422.
Storm, B. C., Bjork, E. L., Bjork, R. A., & Nestojko, J. F. (2006). Is retrieval success a necessary condition for retrieval-induced forgetting? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 10231027.
Storm, B. C., Friedman, M. C., Murayama, K., & Bjork, R. A. (2014). On the transfer of prior tests or study events to subsequent study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 115124.
Stout, D. (2002a). Skill and cognition in stone tool production: An ethnographic case study from Irian Jaya. Current Anthropology, 43, 693722.
Stout, D. (2002b). Thinking and doing in cognitive archaeology: Giving skill its due. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 421422.
Strand, B. Z. (1970). Retroactive inhibition in free recall learning: Unlearning, or category size or? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 286290.
Suddendorf, T., & Busby, J. (2005). Making decisions with the future in mind: Developmental and comparative identification of mental time travel. Learning and Motivation, 36, 110125.
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299351.
Swallow, K. M., & Jiang, Y. V. (2011). The role of timing in the attentional boost effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 389404.
Talamini, L. M., de Haan, L., Nieman, D. H., Linszen, D. H., & Meeter, M. (2010). Reduced context effects on retrieval in first episode schizophrenia. PLoS, ONE, 5, e10356.
Talarico, J. M., & Rubin, D. C. (2003). Confidence, not consistency, characterizes flashbulb memories. Psychological Science, 14, 455461.
Tanner, W. P., & Swets, J. A. (1954). A decision making theory of visual detection. Psychological Review, 61, 401409.
Tassi, P., & Muzet, A. (2000). Sleep inertia. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 4, 341353.
Tehan, G. (2010). Associative relatedness enhances recall and produces false memories in immediate serial recall. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 266272.
Tehan, G., & Humphreys, M. S. (1988). Articulatory loop explanations of memory span and pronunciation rate correspondences: A cautionary note. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 26, 293296.
Tehan, G., & Humphreys, M. S. (1995). Transient phonemic codes and immunity to proactive interference. Memory & Cognition, 23, 181191.
Tehan, G., & Humphreys, M. S. (1996). Cueing effects in short-term recall. Memory & Cognition, 24, 719732.
Tehan, G., & Humphreys, M. S. (1998). Creating proactive interference in immediate recall: Building a dog from a dart, a mop, and a fig. Memory & Cognition, 26, 477489.
Tehan, G., Humphreys, M. S., Tolan, G. A., & Pitcher, C. (2004). The role of context in producing item interactions and false memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 107119.
Tell, P. M. (1971). Influence of vocalization on short term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 149156.
Thomson, D. M. (1972). Context effects in recognition memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 497511.
Thorndike, E. L. (1931). Human learning. New York: Century.
Tolan, G. A., & Tehan, G. (1999). Determinants of short-term forgetting: Decay, retroactive interference, or proactive interference? International Journal of Psychology, 34, 285292.
Tomback, D. F. (1980). How nutcrackers find their stores. Condor, 82, 1019.
Toppino, T. C., & Brochin, H. A. (1989). Learning from tests: The case of true-false examinations. Journal of Educational Research, 83, 119124.
Toppino, T. C., & Luipersbeck, S. M. (1993). Generality of the negative suggestion effect in objective tests. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 357362.
Tse, D., Langston, R. F., Kakeyama, M., Bethus, I., Spooner, P. A., Wood, E. R., & Morris, R. G. M. (2007). Schemas and memory consolidation. Science, 316, 7682.
Tse, D., Takeuchi, T., Kakeyama, M., Kajii, Y., Okuno, H., Tohyama, C., & Morris, R. G. M. (2011). Schema-dependent gene activation and memory encoding in neocortex. Science, 333, 891895.
Tulving, E. (1962). Subjective organization in free recall of unrelated words. Psychological Review, 69, 344354.
Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In Tulving, E. & Donaldson, W. (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 382403). New York: Academic Press.
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New York: Oxford University Press.
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 26, 112.
Tulving, E. (1995). Organization of memory: Quo vadis? In Gazzaniga, M. S. (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 839847). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 125.
Tulving, E. (2005). Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In Terrace, H. S. & Metcalfe, J. (Eds.), The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness (pp. 356). Oxford University Press.
Tulving, E., & Donaldson, W. (Eds.). (1972). Organization of memory. New York: Academic Press.
Tulving, E., Hayman, C. A., & Macdonald, C. A. (1991). Long-lasting perceptual priming and semantic learning in amnesia: A case experiment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 595617.
Tulving, E., & Madigan, S. A. (1970). Memory and verbal learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 21, 437484.
Tulving, E., & Pearlstone, Z. (1966). Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 381391.
Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1971). Retrieval processes in recognition memory: Effects of associative context. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 87, 175184.
Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80, 352373.
Underwood, B. J. (1957). Interference and forgetting. Psychological Review, 64, 4960.
Underwood, B. J. (1963). Stimulus selection in verbal learning. In Cofer, C. N. & Musgrave, B. S. (Eds.), Verbal behavior and learning. New York: McGraw Hill.
Underwood, B. J. (1969). Some correlates of item repetition in free·recall learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 8394.
Underwood, B. J. (1976). Recognition for pairs of words as a function of associative context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1, 404412.
Underwood, B. J. (1977). Temporal codes for memories. New York: Erlbaum.
Underwood, B. J. (1978). Recognition memory as a function of the length of the study list. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 12, 8991.
Underwood, B. J. (1983). Conceptual similarity and cumulative proactive inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 456461.
Underwood, B. J., Broder, P. K., & Zimmerman, J. (1973a). Associative matching and cumulative proactive inhibition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1, 48.
Underwood, B. J., Broder, P. K., & Zimmerman, J. (1973b). Retention of verbal discrimination lists as a function of number of prior lists, word frequency, and type of list. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 101105.
Underwood, B. J., & Humphreys, M. (1979). Context change and the role of meaning in word recognition. American Journal of Psychology, 92, 577609.
Underwood, B. J., Jesse, F., & Ekstrand, B. R. (1964). Knowledge of rights and wrongs in verbal-discrimination learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3, 183186.
Underwood, B. J., & Postman, L. (1960). Extraexperimental sources of interference in forgetting. Psychological Review, 67, 7395.
Underwood, B. J., & Schulz, R. W. (1960). Meaningfulness and verbal learning. Chicago, IL: Lippencott.
Underwood, B. J., & Shaughnessy, J. J. (1975). Experimentation in psychology. Oxford: John Wiley & Sons.
Verde, M. F. (2004). Associative interference in recognition memory: A dual-process account. Memory & Cognition, 32, 12731283.
Voss, J. F. (1971). Are reinforcement concepts able to provide reinforcement for theorizing in human learning (pp. 3744). In Glaser, R. (Ed.), The nature of reinforcement. New York: Academic Press.
Voss, J. F., Thompson, C. P., & Keegan, J. H. (1959). Acquisition of probabilistic paired associates as a function of S-R1 and S-R2 probability. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 390399.
Wagman, J. B. (2002). Symmetry for the sake of symmetry or symmetry for the sake of behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 423424.
Waldum, E. R., & Sahakyan, L. (2012). A role for memory in prospective timing informs timing in prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 809826.
Walker, P., & Cuthbert, L. (1998). Remembering visual feature conjunctions: Visual memory for shape-colour conjunctions is object based. Visual Cognition, 5, 409455.
Wallace, W. P. (1970). Consistency of emission order in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 5868.
Wallace, W. P., Stewart, M. T., & Malone, C. P. (1995). Recognition memory errors produced by implicit activation of word candidates during the processing of spoken words. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 417439.
Wallace, W. P., Stewart, M. T., Schaffer, T. R., & Wison, J. A. (1998). Are false recognitions influenced by prerecognition processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 299315.
Warriner, A. B., & Humphreys, K. R. (2008). Learning to fail: Reoccurring tip-of-the-tongue states. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 535542.
Watkins, O. C., & Watkins, M. J. (1975). Buildup of proactive inhibition as a cue-overload effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 1, 442452.
Weeks, C. S., Cornwell, T. B., & Humphreys, M. S. (2006). Conceptualizing sponsorship: An item and relational information account. In Kahle, L. R. & Kim, Chung-Hyun (Eds.), Creating images and the psychology of marketing communication (pp. 257276). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Weeks, C. S., Humphreys, M. S., & Hockley, W. E. (2007). Buffered forgetting: When targets and distracters are both forgotten. Memory & Cognition, 35, 12671282.
Wells, G. L., Steblay, N. K., & Dysart, J. E. (2012). Eyewitness identification reforms: Are suggestiveness-induced hits and guesses true hits? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 264271.
Wickens, C. D., & Hollands, J. G. (1999). Engineering psychology and human performance. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Wickens, D. D. (1970). Encoding categories of words: An empirical approach. Psychological Review, 77, 115.
Wickens, D. D. (1972). Characteristics of word encoding. In Melton, A. W. & Martin, E. (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp. 191235). Washington, DC: V. H. Winston and Sons.
Wickens, D. D., Born, D. G., & Allen, C. K. (1963). Proactive inhibition and item similarity in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 440445.
Wicklund, D. A., Palermo, D. S., & Jenkins, J. J. (1964). The effects of associative strength and response hierarchy on paired-associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3, 413420.
Wilton, R. N. (1989). The structure of memory: Evidence concerning the recall of surface and background colour of shapes. Quarterly Journal of Psychology, 41A, 579598.
Wilton, R. N., & Mathieson, P. (1996). The supposed effect of interactive imagery in paired-associate learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 888900.
Winograd, E., Karchmer, M. A., & Steele Russell, I. (1971). Role of encoding utilization in cued recognition memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 199206.
Wixted, J. T. (2004a). On common ground: Jost’s (1897) law of forgetting and Ribot’s (1881) law of retrograde amnesia. Psychological Review, 111, 864879.
Wixted, J. T. (2004b). The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting. Annual Review of Psycholology, 55, 235269.
Wixted, J. T., & Mickes, L. (2010). A continuous dual-process model of remember/know judgments. Psychological Review, 117, 10251054.
Wixted, J. T., Mickes, L., & Squire, L. R. (2010). Measuring recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe. Hippocampus, 20, 11951205.
Wolford, G. (1971). Function of distinct associations for paired-associate performance. Psychological Review, 78, 303313.
Wong, A., Leahy, W., Marcus, N., & Sweller, J. (2012). Cognitive load theory, the transient information effect and e-learning. Learning and Instruction, 22, 449457.
Woodworth, R. S., & Schlosberg, H. (1954). Experimental psychology (Revised Edition). New York: Henry Holt and Company.
Wynn, T. (2002). Archaeology and cognitive evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 389438.
Wynn, T., & Coolidge, F. L. (2004). The expert neandertal mind. Journal of Human Evolution, 46, 467487.
Xu, J., & Malmberg, K. J. (2007). Modeling the effects of verbal and nonverbal pair strength on associative recognition, Memory & Cognition, 35, 526544.
Yates, F. A. (1992). The art of memory. London: Pimlico.
Yim, H., Dennis, S., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2013). The development of episodic memory: Items, contexts and relations. Psychological Science, 24, 21632172.
Yonelinas, A. P. (1999). Recognition memory for faces: When familiarity supports associative recognition judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 654661.
Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of thirty years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441517.
Yonelinas, A. P., & Jacoby, L. L. (1996). Noncriterial recollection: Familiarity as automatic, irrelevant recollection. Consciousness and Cognition, 5(1–2), 131141.
Yonelinas, A. P., & Levy, B. J. (2002). Dissociating familiarity from recollection in human recognition memory: Different rates of forgetting over short retention intervals. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 575582.
Young, R. K. (1962). Tests of three hypotheses about the effective stimulus in serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 307313.
Zaromb, F. M., Howard, M. W., Dolan, E. D., Tully, M., Wingfield, A., & Kahana, M. J. (2006). Temporal associations and prior-list intrusions in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 792804.
Zeelenberg, R., Shiffrin, R. M., & Raaijmakers, J. G. (1999). Priming in a free association task as a function of association directionality. Memory & Cognition, 27, 956961.

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Book summary page views

Total views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between #date#. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.