Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-25wd4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T06:19:01.410Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part II - Neuropsychological Level of Trust

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 December 2021

Frank Krueger
Affiliation:
George Mason University, Virginia
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Abdellaoui, M., Baillon, A., Placido, L., & Wakker, P. P. (2011). The rich domain of uncertainty: Source functions and their experimental implementation. American Economic Review, 101(2), 695723. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.101.2.695CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1998). The human amygdala in social judgment. Nature, 393(6684), 470474. https://doi.org/10.1038/30982Google Scholar
Aimone, J. A., & Houser, D. (2011). Beneficial betrayal aversion. PLoS ONE, 6(3), Article e17725. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017725Google Scholar
Aimone, J. A., & Houser, D. (2012). What you don’t know won’t hurt you: A laboratory analysis of betrayal aversion. Experimental Economics, 15(4), 571588. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683–012-9314-zGoogle Scholar
Aimone, J. A., & Houser, D. (2013). Harnessing the benefits of betrayal aversion. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 89, 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2013.02.001Google Scholar
Aimone, J. A., & Houser, D. (2016). Neuroeconomics: A flourishing field. In Komlos, J. & Kelly, I. R. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of economics and human biology. (pp. 649666). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199389292.013.1Google Scholar
Aimone, J. A., Houser, D., & Weber, B. (2014). Neural signatures of betrayal aversion: An fMRI study of trust. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1782), Article 20132127. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.2127Google Scholar
Andreoni, J., & Miller, J. (2002). Giving according to GARP: An experimental test of the consistency of preferences for altruism. Econometrica, 70, 737753. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0262.00302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ashraf, N., Bohnet, I., & Piankov, N. (2006). Decomposing trust and trustworthiness. Experimental Economics, 9(3), 193208. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683–006-9122-4Google Scholar
Becker, G. M., DeGroot, M. H., & Marschak, J. (1964). Measuring utility by a single‐response sequential method. Behavioral Science, 9(3), 226232. https://doi.org/10.1002/bs.3830090304Google Scholar
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10(1), 122142. https://doi.org/10.1006/game.1995.1027Google Scholar
Blais, A. R., & Weber, E. U. (2006). A domain-specific risk-taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgment and Decision Making, 1(1), 3347. https://doi.org/10.1037/t13084–000Google Scholar
Bohnet, I., Greig, F., Herrmann, B., & Zeckhauser, R. (2008). Betrayal aversion: Evidence from Brazil, China, Oman, Switzerland, Turkey, and the United States. American Economic Review, 98(1), 294310. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.98.1.294CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohnet, I., Herrmann, B., & Zeckhauser, R. (2010). Trust and the reference points for trustworthiness in Gulf and Western countries. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 125(2), 811828. https://doi.org/10.1162/qjec.2010.125.2.811Google Scholar
Bohnet, I., & Zeckhauser, R. (2004). Trust, risk and betrayal. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 55(4), 467484. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2003.11.004Google Scholar
Butler, J. V., & Miller, J. B. (2018). Social risk and the dimensionality of intentions. Management Science, 64(6), 27872796. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2016.2694Google Scholar
Christopoulos, G. I., Tobler, P. N., Bossaerts, P., Dolan, R. J., & Schultz, W. (2009). Neural correlates of value, risk, and risk aversion contributing to decision making under risk. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(40), 1257412583. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2614-09.2009Google Scholar
Coleman, J. (1990) Foundation of social theory. The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(8), 655666. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn894Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nature Neuroscience, 3(10), 10491056. https://doi.org/10.1038/79871Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Anderson, J. E., Schlösser, T., Ehlebracht, D., & Fetchenhauer, D. (2014). Trust at zero acquaintance: More a matter of respect than expectation of reward. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(1), 122141. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036673Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Fetchenhauer, D., & Schlösser, T. (2019). Why people trust: Solved puzzles and open mysteries. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(4), 366371. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419838255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, C. C., & Grossman, P. J. (1996). Altruism in anonymous dictator games. Games and Economic Behavior, 16(2), 181191. https://doi.org/10.1006/game.1996.0081CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, C. C., & Wilson, R. K. (2004). Is trust a risky decision? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 55(4), 447465. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2003.11.003Google Scholar
Effron, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (2011). Reducing exposure to trust-related risks to avoid self-blame. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(2), 181192. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210393532Google Scholar
Elster, J. (2007) Explaining social behavior. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511806421Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B., & Tamir, D. (2009). Individual differences in risk preference predict neural responses during financial decision making. Brain Research, 1290, 2851. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2009.06.078CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairley, K., Sanfey, A., Vyrastekova, J., & Weitzel, U. (2016). Trust and risk revisited. Journal of Economic Psychology, 57, 7485. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2016.10.001Google Scholar
Fehr, E. (2009). On the economics and biology of trust. Journal of the European Economic Association, 7(2–3), 235266. https://doi.org/10.1162/JEEA.2009.7.2-3.235Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999) A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817868. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.106228Google Scholar
Fetchenhauer, D., & Dunning, D. (2012). Betrayal aversion versus principled trustfulness: How to explain risk avoidance and risky choices in trust games. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 81(2), 534541. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2011.07.017Google Scholar
Fetchenhauer, D., Lang, A. S., Ehlebracht, D., Schlösser, T., & Dunning, D. (2020). Does betrayal aversion really guide trust decisions towards strangers? Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 33(4), 556566. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2166Google Scholar
Gambetta, D. (1988). Trust: Making and breaking cooperative relations. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gershoff, A. D., & Koehler, J. J. (2011). Safety first? The role of emotion in safety product betrayal aversion. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(1), 140150. https://doi.org/10.1086/658883Google Scholar
Hardin, R. (2001) Conceptions and explanations of trust. In Cook, K. S. (Ed.), Trust in society (pp. 339). Russell Sage Foundation. http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.7758/9781610441322.5Google Scholar
Hardin, R. (2002). Trust and trustworthiness. Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Hardin, R. (2006). Trust. Polity Press.Google Scholar
Holt, C. A., & Laury, S. K. (2002). Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Review, 92(5), 16441655. https://doi.org/10.1257/000282802762024700CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hong, K., & Bohnet, I. (2007). Status and distrust: The relevance of inequality and betrayal aversion. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28(2), 197213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2006.06.003Google Scholar
Houser, D., Schunk, D., & Winter, J. (2010). Distinguishing trust from risk: An anatomy of the investment game. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 74(1–2), 7281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2010.01.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huettel, S. A., Stowe, C. J., Gordon, E. M., Warner, B. T., & Platt, M. L. (2006). Neural signatures of economic preferences for risk and ambiguity. Neuron, 49(5), 765775. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.01.024Google Scholar
Huster, R. J., Enriquez-Geppert, S., Lavallee, C. F., Falkenstein, M., & Herrmann, C. S. (2013). Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: Functional networks and cognitive contributions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 87(3), 217233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.001Google Scholar
Jung, W. H., Lee, S., Lerman, C., & Kable, J. W. (2018). Amygdala functional and structural connectivity predicts individual risk tolerance. Neuron, 98(2), 394404. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.019Google Scholar
Kerr, N. L. (1983). Motivation losses in small groups: A social dilemma analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(4), 819828. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.45.4.819CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim‐Spoon, J., Deater‐Deckard, K., Lauharatanahirun, N., et al. (2017). Neural interaction between risk sensitivity and cognitive control predicting health risk behaviors among late adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 27(3), 674682. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12295Google Scholar
Kirsch, P., Esslinger, C., Chen, Q., et al. (2005). Oxytocin modulates neural circuitry for social cognition and fear in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(49), 1148911493. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3984-05.2005Google Scholar
Knight, F. H. (1921). Risk, uncertainty and profit. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Koehler, J. J., & Gershoff, A. D. (2003). Betrayal aversion: When agents of protection become agents of harm. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 90(2), 244261. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749–5978(02)00518-6Google Scholar
Koscik, T. R., & Tranel, D. (2011). The human amygdala is necessary for developing and expressing normal interpersonal trust. Neuropsychologia, 49(4), 602611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.023Google Scholar
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435(7042), 673676. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03701Google Scholar
Lapinski, M. K., & Rimal, R. N. (2005). An explication of social norms. Communication Theory, 15(2), 127147. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2005.tb00329.xGoogle Scholar
Lauharatanahirun, N., Christopoulos, G. I., & King-Casas, B. (2012). Neural computations underlying social risk sensitivity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, Article 213. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00213Google Scholar
Lauharatanahirun, N., Maciejewski, D., Holmes, C., Deater‐Deckard, K., Kim‐Spoon, J., & King‐Casas, B. (2018). Neural correlates of risk processing among adolescents: Influences of parental monitoring and household chaos. Child Development, 89(3), 784796. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure and Function, 214(5–6), 655667. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429–010-0262-0Google Scholar
Mikolajczak, M., Gross, J. J., Lane, A., Corneille, O., de Timary, P., & Luminet, O. (2010). Oxytocin makes people trusting, not gullible. Psychological Science, 21(8), 10721074. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610377343CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mohr, P. N., Biele, G., & Heekeren, H. R. (2010). Neural processing of risk. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(19), 66136619. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0003-10.2010Google Scholar
Nave, G., Camerer, C., & McCullough, M. (2015). Does oxytocin increase trust in humans? A critical review of research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(6), 772789. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615600138Google Scholar
Quercia, S. (2016). Eliciting and measuring betrayal aversion using the BDM mechanism. Journal of the Economic Science Association, 2(1), 4859. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40881–015-0021-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rangel, A., Camerer, C., & Montague, P. R. (2008). A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(7), 545556. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2357Google Scholar
Rousseau, D., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R., & Camerer, C. (1998). Not so different after all: A cross-discipline view of trust. Academy of Management Review, 23(3), 393404. https://doi.org/10.5465/AMR.1998.926617Google Scholar
Sapienza, P., Toldra‐Simats, A., & Zingales, L. (2013). Understanding trust. The Economic Journal, 123(573), 13131332. https://doi.org/10.1111/ecoj.12036Google Scholar
Schechter, L. (2007). Traditional trust measurement and the risk confound: An experiment in rural Paraguay. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 62(2), 272292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2005.03.006Google Scholar
Sherif, M. (1936). The psychology of social norms. Harper & Brothers.Google Scholar
Simmons, A., Matthews, S. C., Stein, M. B., & Paulus, M. P. (2004). Anticipation of emotionally aversive visual stimuli activates right insula. Neuroreport, 15(14), 22612265. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200410050-00024Google Scholar
Smith, V. L. (1982). Microeconomic systems as an experimental science. The American Economic Review, 72(5), 923955. http://www.jstor.org/stable/1812014Google Scholar
Thielmann, I., & Hilbig, B. E. (2015). Trust: An integrative review from a person–situation perspective. Review of General Psychology, 19(3), 249277. https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000046CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobler, P. N., O’Doherty, J. P., Dolan, R. J., & Schultz, W. (2007). Reward value coding distinct from risk attitude-related uncertainty coding in human reward systems. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97(2), 16211632. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00745.2006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., & Chin, J. (2007). Feeling duped: Emotional, motivational, and cognitive aspects of being exploited by others. Review of General Psychology, 11(2), 127141. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.127Google Scholar
Wang, Y., Jing, Y., Zhang, Z., Lin, C., & Valadez, E. A. (2017). How dispositional social risk-seeking promotes trusting strangers: Evidence based on brain potentials and neural oscillations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(8), 11501163. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000328CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williamson, O. E. (1993). Calculativeness, trust, and economic organization. The Journal of Law and Economics, 36(1, Part 2), 453486. https://doi.org/10.1086/467284Google Scholar
Yamagishi, T. (2011). Trust: The evolutionary game of mind and society. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-53936-0Google Scholar
Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of error detection: Conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 111(4), 931959. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.931Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M., Kolin, E. A., Price, L., & Zoob, I. (1964). Development of a sensation-seeking scale. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 28(6), 477482. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040995Google Scholar

References

Aimone, J., Ball, S., & King-Casas, B. (2015). The betrayal aversion elicitation task: An individual level betrayal aversion measure. PLoS ONE, 10(9), Article e0137491. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137491Google Scholar
Aimone, J. A., & Houser, D. (2011). Beneficial betrayal aversion. PLoS ONE, 6(3), Article e17725. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017725Google Scholar
Aimone, J. A., & Houser, D. (2012). What you don’t know won’t hurt you: A laboratory analysis of betrayal aversion. Experimental Economics, 15(4), 571588. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1589146Google Scholar
Aimone, J. A., Houser, D., & Weber, B. (2014). Neural signatures of betrayal aversion: An fMRI study of trust. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1782), 20132127. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.2127Google Scholar
Alguacil, S., Tudela, P., & Ruz, M. (2015). Ignoring facial emotion expressions does not eliminate their influence on cooperation decisions. Psicológica, 36(2), 309335. www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=16941182006Google Scholar
Alós-Ferrer, C., & Farolfi, F. (2019). Trust games and beyond. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13(887), 114. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.00887CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrade, E. B., & Ho, T.-H. (2007). How is the boss’s mood today? I want a raise. Psychological Science, 18(8), 668671. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01956.xGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J. (1995). Warm-glow versus cold-prickle: The effects of positive and negative framing on cooperation in experiments. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 110(1), 121. https://doi.org/10.2307/2118508Google Scholar
Bechara, A., & Martin, E. M. (2004). Impaired decision making related to working memory deficits in individuals with substance addictions. Neuropsychology, 18(1), 152162. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.152Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Chernyak, S. V., Goodyear, K., Eickhoff, S. B., & Krueger, F. (2017). Neural signatures of trust in reciprocity: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 38(3), 12331248. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23451Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Feng, C., Camilleri, J., Eickhoff, S. B., & Krueger, F. (2018). The role of the anterior insula in social norm compliance and enforcement: Evidence from coordinate-based and functional connectivity meta-analyses. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 92, 378389. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.024Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Molter, F., & Park, S. Q. (2019). Neural representations of honesty predict future trust behavior. Nature Communications, 10(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467–019-13261-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10(1), 122142. https://doi.org/10.1006/game.1995.1027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohnet, I., Greig, F., Herrmann, B., & Zeckhauser, R. (2008). Betrayal aversion: Evidence from Brazil, China, Oman, Switzerland, Turkey, and the United States. American Economic Review, 98(1), 294310. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.98.1.294CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohnet, I., & Zeckhauser, R. (2004). Trust, risk and betrayal. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 55(4), 467484. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.478424Google Scholar
Bonnefon, J.-F., Hopfensitz, A., & De Neys, W. (2017). Can we detect cooperators by looking at their face? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(3), 276281. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417693352Google Scholar
Camerer, C. F. (2003). Behavioral game theory: Experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Campellone, T. R., & Kring, A. M. (2013). Who do you trust? The impact of facial emotion and behaviour on decision making. Cognition & Emotion, 27(4), 603620. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2012.726608Google Scholar
Capra, M. C. (2004). Mood-driven behavior in strategic interactions. American Economic Review, 94(2), 367372. https://doi.org/10.1257/0002828041301885Google Scholar
Caria, A., Sitaram, R., Veit, R., Begliomini, C., & Birbaumer, N. (2010). Volitional control of anterior insula activity modulates the response to aversive stimuli: A real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry, 68(5), 425432. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.020Google Scholar
Charness, G., Du, N., & Yang, C.-L. (2011). Trust and trustworthiness reputations in an investment game. Games and Economic Behavior, 72(2), 361375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.geb.2010.09.002Google Scholar
Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(2), 215224. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167299025002007Google Scholar
Cohn, A., Engelmann, J., Fehr, E., & Maréchal, M. A. (2015). Evidence for countercyclical risk aversion: An experiment with financial professionals. American Economic Review, 105(2), 860885. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.20131314Google Scholar
Craig, A. (2009). Emotional moments across time: A possible neural basis for time perception in the anterior insula. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 19331942. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0008Google Scholar
Cutler, J., & Campbell-Meiklejohn, D. (2019). A comparative fMRI meta-analysis of altruistic and strategic decisions to give. NeuroImage, 184, 227241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.009Google Scholar
De Hooge, I. E., Nelissen, R., Breugelmans, S. M., & Zeelenberg, M. (2011). What is moral about guilt? Acting “prosocially” at the disadvantage of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(3), 462473. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021459Google Scholar
De Neys, W., Hopfensitz, A., & Bonnefon, J.-F. (2017). Split-second trustworthiness detection from faces in an economic game. Experimental Psychology, 64(4), 231239. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000367Google Scholar
DeBruine, L. M. (2002). Facial resemblance enhances trust. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 269(1498), 13071312. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2002.2034Google Scholar
Delgado, M. R., Frank, R. H., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 16111618. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1575Google Scholar
Desmet, P. T., De Cremer, D., & Van Dijk, E. (2011). In money we trust? The use of financial compensations to repair trust in the aftermath of distributive harm. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 114(2), 7586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.10.006Google Scholar
Dunn, J. R., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2005). Feeling and believing: The influence of emotion on trust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5), 736748. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.736Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Fetchenhauer, D., & Schlösser, T. (2012). Trust as a social and emotional act: Noneconomic considerations in trust behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(3), 686694. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2011.09.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunning, D., Fetchenhauer, D., & Schlösser, T. (2017). The varying roles played by emotion in economic decision making. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 15, 3338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.05.006Google Scholar
Eckel, C. C., & Wilson, R. K. (2003). The human face of game theory. In Ostrom, E. & Walker, J. M. (Eds.), Trust and reciprocity: Interdisciplinary lessons from experimental research (pp. 245274). Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Efferson, C., & Vogt, S. (2013). Viewing men’s faces does not lead to accurate predictions of trustworthiness. Scientific Reports, 3, Article 1047. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep01047Google Scholar
Eimontaite, I., Nicolle, A., Schindler, I. C., & Goel, V. (2013). The effect of partner-directed emotion in social exchange decision making. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(469), 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00469Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (2003). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues. Malor Books.Google Scholar
Engell, A. D., Haxby, J. V., & Todorov, A. (2007). Implicit trustworthiness decisions: Automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 15081519. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1508Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B. (2010). Measuring trust in social neuroeconomics: A tutorial. Hermeneutische Blätter, 1(2), 225242.Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B., & Fehr, E. (2017). The neurobiology of trust and social decision: The important role of emotions. In Van Lange, P. A. M., Rockenbach, B., & Yamagishi, T. (Eds.), Trust in social dilemmas (pp. 3356). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B., & Hare, T. A. (2018). Emotions can bias decision making processes by promoting specific behavior. In Fox, A. S., Lapate, R. C., Shackman, A. J., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (2nd ed., pp. 355370). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B., Meyer, F., Fehr, E., & Ruff, C. C. (2015). Anticipatory anxiety disrupts neural valuation during risky choice. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(7), 30853099. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2880-14.2015Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B., Meyer, F., Ruff, C. C., & Fehr, E. (2019). The neural circuitry of affect-induced distortions of trust. Science Advances, 5(3), Article eaau3413. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aau3413Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B., Schmid, B., De Dreu, C. K., Chumbley, J., & Fehr, E. (2019). On the psychology and economics of antisocial personality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(26), 1278112786. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1820133116Google Scholar
Fehr, E. (2009). On the economics and biology of trust. Journal of the European Economic Association, 7(2–3), 235266. https://doi.org/10.1162/JEEA.2009.7.2-3.235Google Scholar
Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U., & Kosfeld, M. (2005). Neuroeconomic foundations of trust and social preferences: Initial evidence. American Economic Review, 95(2), 346351. https://doi.org/10.1257/000282805774669736Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2000). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. American Economic Review, 90(4), 980994. http://doi.org/10.1257.aer.90.4.980Google Scholar
FeldmanHall, O., Dunsmoor, J. E., Tompary, A., Hunter, L. E., Todorov, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2018). Stimulus generalization as a mechanism for learning to trust. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(7), E1690E1697. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1715227115Google Scholar
Ferrer, R. A., Grenen, E. G., & Taber, J. M. (2015). Effectiveness of internet-based affect induction procedures: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Emotion, 15(6), 752762. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fertuck, E. A., Fischer, S., & Beeney, J. (2018). Social cognition and borderline personality disorder: Splitting and trust impairment findings. Psychiatric Clinics, 41(4), 613632. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2018.07.003Google Scholar
Fetchenhauer, D., & Dunning, D. (2012). Betrayal aversion versus principled trustfulness: How to explain risk avoidance and risky choices in trust games. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 81(2), 534541. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2011.07.017Google Scholar
Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 3966. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.39Google Scholar
Franzen, N., Hagenhoff, M., Baer, N., et al. (2011). Superior “theory of mind” in borderline personality disorder: An analysis of interaction behavior in a virtual trust game. Psychiatry Research, 187(1–2), 224233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.11.012Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 365376. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerrards-Hesse, A., Spies, K., & Hesse, F. W. (1994). Experimental inductions of emotional states and their effectiveness: A review. British Journal of Psychology, 85(1), 5578. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02508.xGoogle Scholar
Göritz, A. S., & Moser, K. (2006). Web-based mood induction. Cognition and Emotion, 20(6), 887896. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930500405386CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gradin, V., Pérez, A., MacFarlane, J., et al. (2015). Abnormal brain responses to social fairness in depression: An fMRI study using the Ultimatum Game. Psychological Medicine, 45(6), 12411251. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291714002347Google Scholar
Gromann, P. M., Heslenfeld, D. J., Fett, A.-K., Joyce, D. W., Shergill, S. S., & Krabbendam, L. (2013). Trust versus paranoia: Abnormal response to social reward in psychotic illness. Brain, 136(6), 19681975. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt076Google Scholar
Güth, W., Schmittberger, R., & Schwarze, B. (1982). An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 3(4), 367388. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-2681(82)90011-7Google Scholar
Hallquist, M. N., & Pilkonis, P. A. (2012). Refining the phenotype of borderline personality disorder: Diagnostic criteria and beyond. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 3(3), 228246. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027953Google Scholar
Hare, T. A., Malmaud, J., & Rangel, A. (2011). Focusing attention on the health aspects of foods changes value signals in vmPFC and improves dietary choice. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(30), 1107711087. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.6383-10.2011Google Scholar
Harlé, K. M., Chang, L. J., van’t Wout, M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2012). The neural mechanisms of affect infusion in social economic decision making: A mediating role of the anterior insula. NeuroImage, 61(1), 3240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.027Google Scholar
Hellhammer, D. H., Wüst, S., & Kudielka, B. M. (2009). Salivary cortisol as a biomarker in stress research. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(2), 163171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.026Google Scholar
Hertel, G., Neuhof, J., Theuer, T., & Kerr, N. L. (2000). Mood effects on cooperation in small groups: Does positive mood simply lead to more cooperation? Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 441472. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999300402754Google Scholar
Hinson, J. M., Jameson, T. L., & Whitney, P. (2003). Impulsive decision making and working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(2), 298306. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.2.298Google Scholar
Hirschfeld, R., Montgomery, S. A., Keller, M. B., et al. (2000). Social functioning in depression: A review. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 61(4), 268275. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.v61n0405CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houser, D., Schunk, D., & Winter, J. (2010). Distinguishing trust from risk: An anatomy of the investment game. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 74(1–2), 7281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2010.01.002Google Scholar
Isaac, R. M., & Walker, J. M. (1988). Group size effects in public goods provision: The voluntary contributions mechanism. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 103(1), 179199. https://doi.org/10.2307/1882648Google Scholar
Jaeger, B., Evans, A. M., Stel, M., & Van Beest, I. (2019). Explaining the persistent influence of facial cues in social decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(6), 10081021. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000591Google Scholar
Jaeger, B., Oud, B., Williams, T., Krumhuber, E. G., Fehr, E., & Engelmann, J. B. (2020). Trustworthiness detection from faces: Does reliance on facial impressions pay off? PsyArXiv preprint. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ayqehGoogle Scholar
Johnson, N. D., & Mislin, A. A. (2011). Trust games: A meta-analysis. Journal of Economic Psychology, 32(5), 865889. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2011.05.007Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. H. (1986). Fairness and the assumptions of economics. Journal of Business, 59(4), S285S300. https://doi.org/10.1086/296367Google Scholar
Kang, Y., Williams, L. E., Clark, M. S., Gray, J. R., & Bargh, J. A. (2011). Physical temperature effects on trust behavior: The role of insula. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(4), 507515. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq077Google Scholar
Kausel, E. E., & Connolly, T. (2014). Do people have accurate beliefs about the behavioral consequences of incidental emotions? Evidence from trust games. Journal of Economic Psychology, 42, 96111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2014.02.002Google Scholar
Kenealy, P. (1988). Validation of a music mood induction procedure: Some preliminary findings. Cognition & Emotion, 2(1), 4148. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938808415228Google Scholar
King-Casas, B., Sharp, C., Lomax-Bream, L., Lohrenz, T., Fonagy, P., & Montague, P. R. (2008). The rupture and repair of cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Science, 321(5890), 806810. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1156902Google Scholar
Kirchsteiger, G., Rigotti, L., & Rustichini, A. (2006). Your morals might be your moods. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 59(2), 155172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2004.07.004Google Scholar
Klapper, A., Dotsch, R., Van Rooij, I., & Wigboldus, D. H. (2016). Do we spontaneously form stable trustworthiness impressions from facial appearance? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(5), 655664. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000062Google Scholar
Krueger, F., Bellucci, G., Xu, P., & Feng, C. (2020). The critical role of the right dorsal and ventral anterior insula in reciprocity: Evidence from the trust and ultimatum games. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 14(176), 16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00176Google Scholar
Krueger, F., Grafman, J., & McCabe, K. (2008). Neural correlates of economic game playing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1511), 38593874. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0165Google Scholar
Krumhuber, E., Manstead, A. S., Cosker, D., Marshall, D., Rosin, P. L., & Kappas, A. (2007). Facial dynamics as indicators of trustworthiness and cooperative behavior. Emotion, 7(4), 730735. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.730Google Scholar
Kugler, T., Ye, B., Motro, D., & Noussair, C. N. (2020). On trust and disgust: Evidence from face reading and virtual reality. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11(3), 317325. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550619856302Google Scholar
Lamm, C., & Singer, T. (2010). The role of anterior insular cortex in social emotions. Brain Structure and Function, 214(5–6), 579591. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429–010-0251-3Google Scholar
Lench, H. C., Flores, S. A., & Bench, S. W. (2011). Discrete emotions predict changes in cognition, judgment, experience, behavior, and physiology: A meta-analysis of experimental emotion elicitations. Psychological Bulletin, 137(5), 834855. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024244Google Scholar
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 473493. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999300402763Google Scholar
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., Valdesolo, P., & Kassam, K. S. (2015). Emotion and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 799823. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115043Google Scholar
Lim, S.-L., O’Doherty, J. P., & Rangel, A. (2011). The decision value computations in the vmPFC and striatum use a relative value code that is guided by visual attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(37), 1321413223. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1246-11.2011Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(3), 121143. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000446Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G., & Lerner, J. S. (2003). The role of affect in decision making. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective science (pp. 619642). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 267286. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.267Google Scholar
Loomes, G., & Sugden, R. (1982). Regret theory: An alternative theory of rational choice under uncertainty. The Economic Journal, 92(368), 805824. https://doi.org/10.2307/2232669Google Scholar
Loomes, G., & Sugden, R. (1986). Disappointment and dynamic consistency in choice under uncertainty. The Review of Economic Studies, 53(2), 271282. https://doi.org/10.2307/2297651Google Scholar
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131(6), 803855. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803Google Scholar
Maccallum, F., McConkey, K. M., Bryant, R. A., & Barnier, A. J. (2000). Specific autobiographical memory following hypnotically induced mood state. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 48(4), 361373. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207140008410366Google Scholar
Martinez, L. F., & Zeelenberg, M. (2015). Trust me (or not): Regret and disappointment in experimental economic games. Decision, 2(2), 118126. https://doi.org/10.1037/dec0000025Google Scholar
McCabe, K., Houser, D., Ryan, L., Smith, V., & Trouard, T. (2001). A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(20), 1183211835. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.211415698Google Scholar
Mellers, B. A., Haselhuhn, M. P., Tetlock, P. E., Silva, J. C., & Isen, A. M. (2010). Predicting behavior in economic games by looking through the eyes of the players. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 743755. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020280Google Scholar
Mellers, B. A., Schwartz, A., Ho, K., & Ritov, I. (1997). Decision affect theory: Emotional reactions to the outcomes of risky options. Psychological Science, 8(6), 423429. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00455.xGoogle Scholar
Mislin, A., Williams, L. V., & Shaughnessy, B. A. (2015). Motivating trust: Can mood and incentives increase interpersonal trust? Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 58, 1119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2015.06.001Google Scholar
Mulckhuyse, M., Crombez, G., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2013). Conditioned fear modulates visual selection. Emotion, 13(3), 529536. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031076Google Scholar
Mulckhuyse, M., Engelmann, J. B., Schutter, D. J., & Roelofs, K. (2017). Right posterior parietal cortex is involved in disengaging from threat: A 1-Hz rTMS study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(11), 18141822. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, C. D., & Tingley, D. (2016). The influence of emotion on trust. Political Analysis, 24(4), 492500. https://doi.org/10.1093/pan/mpw026Google Scholar
Nelissen, R. M., Dijker, A. J., & deVries, N. K. (2007). How to turn a hawk into a dove and vice versa: Interactions between emotions and goals in a give-some dilemma game. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(2), 280286. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.01.009Google Scholar
Niv, Y., Edlund, J. A., Dayan, P., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2012). Neural prediction errors reveal a risk-sensitive reinforcement-learning process in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(2), 551562. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5498-10.2012Google Scholar
Nolte, T., Bolling, D. Z., Hudac, C., Fonagy, P., Mayes, L. C., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2013). Brain mechanisms underlying the impact of attachment-related stress on social cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(816), 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00816Google Scholar
Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(2), 148158. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2317Google Scholar
Pham, M. T. (2007). Emotion and rationality: A critical review and interpretation of empirical evidence. Review of General Psychology, 11(2), 155178. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.155Google Scholar
Phelps, E. A., Lempert, K. M., & Sokol-Hessner, P. (2014). Emotion and decision making: Multiple modulatory neural circuits. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 37(1), 263287. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014119Google Scholar
Prochazkova, E., Prochazkova, L., Giffin, M. R., Scholte, H. S., De Dreu, C. K., & Kret, M. E. (2018). Pupil mimicry promotes trust through the theory-of-mind network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(31), E7265E7274. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1803916115Google Scholar
Quercia, S. (2016). Eliciting and measuring betrayal aversion using the BDM mechanism. Journal of the Economic Science Association, 2(1), 4859. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40881–015-0021-3Google Scholar
Rabin, M. (1993). Incorporating fairness into game theory and economics. The American Economic Review, 83(5), 12811302. www.jstor.org/stable/2117561?seq=1#metadata_info_tab_contentsGoogle Scholar
Rangel, A., Camerer, C., & Montague, P. R. (2008). A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(7), 545556. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2357Google Scholar
Rangel, A., & Hare, T. (2010). Neural computations associated with goal-directed choice. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20(2), 262270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2010.03.001Google Scholar
Rick, S., & Loewenstein, G. (2008). The role of emotion in economic behavior. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed.; pp. 138158). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rilling, J. K., Sanfey, A. G., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions. NeuroImage, 22(4), 16941703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.015Google Scholar
Roelofs, K. (2017). Freeze for action: Neurobiological mechanisms in animal and human freezing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372(1718), 20160206. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0206Google Scholar
Roelofs, K., Putman, P., Schouten, S., Lange, W.-G., Volman, I., & Rinck, M. (2010). Gaze direction differentially affects avoidance tendencies to happy and angry faces in socially anxious individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(4), 290294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.008Google Scholar
Ruff, C. C., & Fehr, E. (2014). The neurobiology of rewards and values in social decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(8), 549562. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3776Google Scholar
Saadaoui, H., El Harbi, S., & Ibanez, L. (2019). Do people trust more when they are happy or when they are sad? Evidence from an experiment. Managerial and Decision Economics, 40(4), 374383. https://doi.org/10.1002/mde.3008Google Scholar
Sanfey, A. G. (2009). Expectations and social decision making: Biasing effects of prior knowledge on Ultimatum responses. Mind & Society, 8(1), 93107. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299–009-0053-6Google Scholar
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision making in the Ultimatum Game. Science, 300(5626), 17551758. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1082976Google Scholar
Scharlemann, J. P., Eckel, C. C., Kacelnik, A., & Wilson, R. K. (2001). The value of a smile: Game theory with a human face. Journal of Economic Psychology, 22(5), 617640. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167–4870(01)00059-9Google Scholar
Schlösser, T., Fetchenhauer, D., & Dunning, D. (2016). Trust against all odds? Emotional dynamics in trust behavior. Decision, 3(3), 216230. https://doi.org/10.1037/dec0000048Google Scholar
Schmitz, A., & Grillon, C. (2012). Assessing fear and anxiety in humans using the threat of predictable and unpredictable aversive events (the NPU-threat test). Nature Protocols, 7(3), 527532. https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2012.001Google Scholar
Schönberg, T., Daw, N. D., Joel, D., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2007). Reinforcement learning signals in the human striatum distinguish learners from nonlearners during reward-based decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(47), 1286012867. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2496-07.2007Google Scholar
Schultz, W. (2002). Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron, 36(2), 241263. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896–6273(02)00967-4Google Scholar
Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., & Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 934. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009Google Scholar
Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In Higgins, T. E. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 1; pp. 527561). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sripada, C. S., Angstadt, M., Banks, S., Nathan, P. J., Liberzon, I., & Phan, K. L. (2009). Functional neuroimaging of mentalizing during the trust game in social anxiety disorder. Neuroreport, 20(11), 984989. https://doi.org/10.1097/wnr.0b013e32832d0a67Google Scholar
Stanley, D. A., Sokol-Hessner, P., Fareri, D. S., et al. (2012). Race and reputation: Perceived racial group trustworthiness influences the neural correlates of trust decisions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1589), 744753. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0300Google Scholar
Tabibnia, G., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). The sunny side of fairness: Preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry). Psychological Science, 19(4), 339347. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02091.xGoogle Scholar
Thielmann, I., & Hilbig, B. E. (2015). Trust: An integrative review from a person–situation perspective. Review of General Psychology, 19(3), 249277. https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000046Google Scholar
Thielmann, I., Spadaro, G., & Balliet, D. (2020). Personality and prosocial behavior: A theoretical framework and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(1), 3039. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000217Google Scholar
Tognetti, A., Berticat, C., Raymond, M., & Faurie, C. (2013). Is cooperativeness readable in static facial features? An inter-cultural approach. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(6), 427432. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.08.002Google Scholar
Tortosa, M. I., Strizhko, T., Capizzi, M., & Ruz, M. (2013). Interpersonal effects of emotion in a multi-round trust game. Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology, 34(2), 179198.Google Scholar
Velten, J. E. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 6(4), 473482. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(68)90028-4Google Scholar
Verplaetse, J., Vanneste, S., & Braeckman, J. (2007). You can judge a book by its cover: The sequel. A kernel of truth in predictive cheating detection. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(4), 260271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.04.006Google Scholar
Wager, T. D., & Barrett, L. F. (2017). From affect to control: Functional specialization of the insula in motivation and regulation. BioRxiv, Article 102368. https://doi.org/10.1101/102368Google Scholar
Waytz, A., & Mitchell, J. P. (2011). Two mechanisms for simulating other minds: Dissociations between mirroring and self-projection. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(3), 197200. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411409007Google Scholar
Wehebrink, K. S., Koelkebeck, K., Piest, S., de Dreu, C. K., & Kret, M. E. (2018). Pupil mimicry and trust–implication for depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 97, 7076. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.11.007Google Scholar
Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17(7), 592598. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.xGoogle Scholar
Winston, J. S., Strange, B. A., O’Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nature Neuroscience, 5(3), 277283. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn816Google Scholar
Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24(1), 347. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903205698Google Scholar

References

Abrahao, B., Parigi, P., Gupta, A., & Cook, K. S. (2017). Reputation offsets trust judgments based on social biases among Airbnb users. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(37), 98489853. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1604234114Google Scholar
Adolphs, R. (2010). What does the amygdala contribute to social cognition?. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191(1), 4261. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05445.xGoogle Scholar
Adolphs, R., & Spezio, M. (2006). Role of the amygdala in processing visual social stimuli. In Progress in brain research (Vol. 156, pp. 363378). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079–6123(06)56020-0Google Scholar
Alexander, J. K., Hillier, A., Smith, R. M., Tivarus, M. E., & Beversdorf, D. Q. (2007). Beta-adrenergic modulation of cognitive flexibility during stress. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(3), 468478. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.468Google Scholar
Ames, D. L., & Fiske, S. T. (2013). Outcome dependency alters the neural substrates of impression formation. NeuroImage, 83, 599608. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.001Google Scholar
Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(4), 268277. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1884Google Scholar
Arnsten, A. F. T., Wang, M. J., & Paspalas, C. D. (2012). Neuromodulation of thought: Flexibilities and vulnerabilities in prefrontal cortical network synapses. Neuron, 76(1), 223239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.038Google Scholar
Balas, B., & Pacella, J. (2017). Trustworthiness perception is disrupted in artificial faces. Computers in Human Behavior, 77, 240248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.045Google Scholar
Baron, S. G., Gobbini, M. I., Engell, A. D., & Todorov, A. (2011). Amygdala and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex responses to appearance-based and behavior-based person impressions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(5), 572581. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq086Google Scholar
Bartra, O., McGuire, J. T., & Kable, J. W. (2013). The valuation system: A coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. NeuroImage, 76, 412427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.063Google Scholar
Bault, N., Pelloux, B., Fahrenfort, J. J., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Van Winden, F. (2015). Neural dynamics of social tie formation in economic decision making. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(6), 877884. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu138Google Scholar
Baumgartner, T., Heinrichs, M., Vonlanthen, A., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2008). Oxytocin shapes the neural circuitry of trust and trust adaptation in humans. Neuron, 58(4), 639650. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.009Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Chernyak, S. V., Goodyear, K., Eickhoff, S. B., & Krueger, F. (2017). Neural signatures of trust in reciprocity: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 38(3), 12331248. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23451Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Hahn, T., Deshpande, G., & Krueger, F. (2018). Functional connectivity of specific resting-state networks predicts trust and reciprocity in the trust game. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19(1), 165176. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-00654-3Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Molter, F., & Park, S. Q. (2019). Neural representations of honesty predict future trust behavior. Nature Communications, 10(1), Article 5184. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-13261-8Google Scholar
Benuzzi, F., Pugnaghi, M., Meletti, S., et al. (2007). Processing the socially relevant parts of faces. Brain Research Bulletin, 74(5), 344356. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.07.010Google Scholar
Berg Nødtvedt, K., Sjåstad, H., Skard, S., Thorbjørnsen, H., & Van Bavel, J. (2020). Racial bias in the sharing economy and the role of trust and self-congruence. Social Science Research Network. https://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3434463Google Scholar
Berger, J., Heinrichs, M., von Dawans, B., Way, B. M., & Chen, F. S. (2016). Cortisol modulates men’s affiliative responses to acute social stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 63, 19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.09.004Google Scholar
Bhanji, J. P., & Delgado, M. R. (2014). Perceived control influences neural responses to setbacks and promotes persistence. Neuron, 83(6), 13691375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.012Google Scholar
Bogdan, R., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2006). Acute stress reduces reward responsiveness: Implications for depression. Biological Psychiatry, 60(10), 11471154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.037Google Scholar
Bonnefon, J.-F., Hopfensitz, A., & De Neys, W. (2013). The modular nature of trustworthiness detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 143150. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028930Google Scholar
Brambilla, M., Biella, M., & Freeman, J. B. (2018). The influence of visual context on the evaluation of facial trustworthiness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 78, 3442. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.04.011Google Scholar
Brudner, E. G., Khalil, A., & Delgado, M. R. (2019, August). The upside of social media: The influence of peer ‘likes’ on adaptive social behavior. Flux Congress, New York, NY.Google Scholar
Buchanan, T. W., & Passarelli, T. O. (2020). How do stress and social closeness impact prosocial behavior? Experimental Psychology, 67(2), 123131. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000482Google Scholar
Bzdok, D., Langner, R., Caspers, S., et al. (2011). ALE meta-analysis on facial judgments of trustworthiness and attractiveness. Brain Structure and Function, 215(3–4), 209223. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0287-4Google Scholar
Campellone, T. R., & Kring, A. M. (2013). Who do you trust? The impact of facial emotion and behaviour on decision making. Cognition & Emotion, 27(4), 603620. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2012.726608Google Scholar
Chang, L. J., Doll, B. B., van’t Wout, M., Frank, M. J., & Sanfey, A. G. (2010). Seeing is believing: Trustworthiness as a dynamic belief. Cognitive Psychology, 61(2), 87105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.03.001Google Scholar
Chang, S.-A. A., & Baskin-Sommers, A. (2020). Living in a disadvantaged neighborhood affects neural processing of facial trustworthiness. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 409. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00409Google Scholar
Charness, G., Du, N., & Yang, C.-L. (2011). Trust and trustworthiness reputations in an investment game. Games and Economic Behavior, 72(2), 361375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.geb.2010.09.002Google Scholar
Chein, J., Albert, D., O’Brien, L., Uckert, K., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Developmental Science, 14(2), F1F10. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01035.xGoogle Scholar
Corral-Frías, N. S., Nikolova, Y. S., Michalski, L. J., Baranger, D. A. A., Hariri, A. R., & Bogdan, R. (2015). Stress-related anhedonia is associated with ventral striatum reactivity to reward and transdiagnostic psychiatric symptomatology. Psychological Medicine, 45(12), 26052617. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715000525Google Scholar
Daw, N. D., O’Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature, 441(7095), 876879. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04766Google Scholar
Declerck, C. H., Boone, C., Pauwels, L., Vogt, B., & Fehr, E. (2020). A registered replication study on oxytocin and trust. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(6), 646655. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0878-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delgado, M. R., Frank, R. H., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 16111618. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1575Google Scholar
De Neys, W., Hopfensitz, A., & Bonnefon, J. F. (2017). Split-second trustworthiness detection from faces in an economic game. Experimental Psychology, 64(4), 231239. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000367Google Scholar
Dillon, D. G., Holmes, A. J., Birk, J. L., Brooks, N., Lyons-Ruth, K., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2009). Childhood adversity is associated with left basal ganglia dysfunction during reward anticipation in adulthood. Biological Psychiatry, 66(3), 206213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.019Google Scholar
Ditzen, B., & Heinrichs, M. (2014). Psychobiology of social support: The social dimension of stress buffering. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 32(1), 149162. https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-139008Google Scholar
Duncko, R., Johnson, L., Merikangas, K., & Grillon, C. (2009). Working memory performance after acute exposure to the cold pressor stress in healthy volunteers. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 91(4), 377381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2009.01.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberger, N. I., Taylor, S. E., Gable, S. L., Hilmert, C. J., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses. NeuroImage, 35(4), 16011612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.038Google Scholar
Elzinga, B. M., & Roelofs, K. (2005). Cortisol-induced impairments of working memory require acute sympathetic activation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119(1), 98103. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.98Google Scholar
Engell, A. D., Haxby, J. V., & Todorov, A. (2007). Implicit trustworthiness decisions: Automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 15081519. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1508CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engelmann, J. B., Meyer, F., Ruff, C. C., & Fehr, E. (2019). The neural circuitry of affect-induced distortions of trust. Science Advances, 5(3), Article eaau3413. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aau3413Google Scholar
Everett, J. A. C., Pizarro, D. A., & Crockett, M. J. (2016). Inference of trustworthiness from intuitive moral judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(6), 772787. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000165Google Scholar
Ewing, L., Caulfield, F., Read, A., & Rhodes, G. (2015). Perceived trustworthiness of faces drives trust behaviour in children. Developmental Science, 18(2), 327334. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12218Google Scholar
Falvello, V., Vinson, M., Ferrari, C., & Todorov, A. (2015). The robustness of learning about the trustworthiness of other people. Social Cognition, 33(5), 368386. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2015.33.5.368Google Scholar
Fareri, D. S., Chang, L. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2012). Effects of direct social experience on trust decisions and neural reward circuitry. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6(148), 117. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fareri, D. S., Chang, L. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2015). Computational substrates of social value in interpersonal collaboration. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(21), 81708180. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4775-14.2015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fareri, D. S., Smith, D. V., & Delgado, M. R. (2020). The influence of relationship closeness on default-mode network connectivity during social interactions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(3), 261271. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa031CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FeldmanHall, O., Dunsmoor, J. E., Tompary, A., Hunter, L. E., Todorov, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2018). Stimulus generalization as a mechanism for learning to trust. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(7), E1690E1697. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1715227115Google Scholar
FeldmanHall, O., Raio, C. M., Kubota, J. T., Seiler, M. G., & Phelps, E. A. (2015). The effects of social context and acute stress on decision making under uncertainty. Psychological Science, 26(12), 19181926. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615605807Google Scholar
Ferrari, C., Lega, C., Vernice, M., et al. (2014). The dorsomedial prefrontal cortex plays a causal role in integrating social impressions from faces and verbal descriptions. Cerebral Cortex, 26(1), 156165. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu186Google Scholar
Fett, A. K. J., Gromann, P. M., Giampietro, V., Shergill, S. S., & Krabbendam, L. (2014). Default distrust? An fMRI investigation of the neural development of trust and cooperation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(4), 395402. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss144Google Scholar
Fiske, S. T., & Dupree, C. (2014). Gaining trust as well as respect in communicating to motivated audiences about science topics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(Suppl. 4), 1359313597. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1317505111Google Scholar
Fouragnan, E., Chierchia, G., Greiner, S., Neveu, R., Avesani, P., & Coricelli, G. (2013). Reputational priors magnify striatal responses to violations of trust. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(8), 36023611. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3086-12.2013Google Scholar
Freeman, J. B., Stolier, R. M., Ingbretsen, Z. A., & Hehman, E. A. (2014). Amygdala responsivity to high-level social information from unseen faces. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(32), 1057310581. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5063-13.2014Google Scholar
Friston, K. J. (2011). Functional and effective connectivity: A review. Brain Connectivity, 1(1), 1336. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2011.0008Google Scholar
Fujino, J., Tei, S., Itahashi, T., et al. (2020). Role of the right temporoparietal junction in intergroup bias in trust decisions. Human Brain Mapping, 41(6), 16771688. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24903Google Scholar
Gheorghiu, A. I., Callan, M. J., & Skylark, W. J. (2017). Facial appearance affects science communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(23), 59705975. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1620542114Google Scholar
Gluckman, M., & Johnson, S. P. (2013). Attentional capture by social stimuli in young infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 527. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00527Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 14641480. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464Google Scholar
Hackel, L. M., Doll, B. B., & Amodio, D. M. (2015). Instrumental learning of traits versus rewards: Dissociable neural correlates and effects on choice. Nature Neuroscience, 18(9), 12331235. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4080Google Scholar
Hackel, L. M., Wills, J. A., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2020). Shifting prosocial intuitions: Neurocognitive evidence for a value-based account of group-based cooperation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(4), 371381. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa055Google Scholar
Heatherton, T. F. (2011). Neuroscience of self and self-regulation. Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 363390. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131616CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hetherington, M. J. (1999). The effect of political trust on the presidential vote, 1968–96. American Political Science Review, 93(2), 311326. https://doi.org/10.2307/2585398Google Scholar
Hughes, B. L., Ambady, N., & Zaki, J. (2017). Trusting outgroup, but not ingroup members, requires control: Neural and behavioral evidence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(3), 372381. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw139Google Scholar
Hughes, B. L., & Zaki, J. (2015). The neuroscience of motivated cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(2), 6264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.006Google Scholar
Hughes, B. L., Zaki, J., & Ambady, N. (2017). Motivation alters impression formation and related neural systems. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(1), 4960. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw147Google Scholar
Huijsmans, I., Ma, I., Micheli, L., Civai, C., Stallen, M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2019). A scarcity mindset alters neural processing underlying consumer decision making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(24), 1169911704. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1818572116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ide, J. S., Nedic, S., Wong, K. F., et al. (2018). Oxytocin attenuates trust as a subset of more general reinforcement learning, with altered reward circuit functional connectivity in males. NeuroImage, 174, 3543. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.035Google Scholar
Jachimowicz, J. M., Chafik, S., Munrat, S., Prabhu, J. C., & Weber, E. U. (2017). Community trust reduces myopic decisions of low-income individuals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(21), 54015406. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1617395114Google Scholar
Jaeger, B., Todorov, A. T., Evans, A. M., & Van Beest, I. (2020). Can we reduce facial biases? Persistent effects of facial trustworthiness on sentencing decisions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 90, Article 104004. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104004Google Scholar
Jirsaraie, R. J., Ranby, K. W., & Albeck, D. S. (2019). Early life stress moderates the relationship between age and prosocial behaviors. Child Abuse & Neglect, 94, Article 104029. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104029Google Scholar
Keres, A., & Chartier, C. R. (2016). The biasing effects of visual background on perceived facial trustworthiness. Psi Chi Journal of Psychological Research, 21(3), 170175.Google Scholar
King-Casas, B. (2005). Getting to know you: Reputation and trust in a two-person economic exchange. Science, 308(5718), 7883. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1108062Google Scholar
King-Casas, B., Tomlin, D., Anen, C., Camerer, C. F., Quartz, S. R., & Montague, P. R. (2005). Getting to know you: Reputation and trust in a two-person economic exchange. Science, 308(5718), 7883. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1108062Google Scholar
Kliemann, D., Young, L., Scholz, J., & Saxe, R. (2008). The influence of prior record on moral judgment. Neuropsychologia, 46(12), 29492957. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.06.010Google Scholar
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435(7042), 673676. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03701Google Scholar
Krosch, A. R., & Amodio, D. M. (2014). Economic scarcity alters the perception of race. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(25), 90799084. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1404448111Google Scholar
Krosch, A. R., & Amodio, D. M. (2019). Scarcity disrupts the neural encoding of Black faces: A socioperceptual pathway to discrimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(5), 859875. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000168Google Scholar
Krumhuber, E., Manstead, A. S. R., Cosker, D., Marshall, D., Rosin, P. L., & Kappas, A. (2007). Facial dynamics as indicators of trustworthiness and cooperative behavior. Emotion, 7(4), 730735. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.730Google Scholar
Kubota, J. T., Li, J., Bar-David, E., Banaji, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2013). The price of racial bias: Intergroup negotiations in the ultimatum game. Psychological Science, 24(12), 24982504. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613496435Google Scholar
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480498. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480Google Scholar
Lambert, B., Declerck, C. H., Emonds, G., & Boone, C. (2017). Trust as commodity: Social value orientation affects the neural substrates of learning to cooperate. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(4), 609617. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw170Google Scholar
Li, D., Meng, L., & Ma, Q. (2017). Who deserves my trust? Cue-elicited feedback negativity tracks reputation learning in repeated social interactions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, Article 307. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00307Google Scholar
Li, H., Song, Y., & Xie, X. (2019). Altruistic or selfish? Responses when safety is threatened depend on childhood socioeconomic status. European Journal of Social Psychology, 50(5), 10011016. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2651Google Scholar
Li, J., Delgado, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2011). How instructed knowledge modulates the neural systems of reward learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(1), 5560. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1014938108Google Scholar
Luethi, M., Meier, B., & Sandi, C. (2009). Stress effects on working memory, explicit memory, and implicit memory for neutral and emotional stimuli in healthy men. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(5). https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.08.005.2008Google Scholar
Margittai, Z., Strombach, T., Van Wingerden, M., Joëls, M., Schwabe, L., & Kalenscher, T. (2015). A friend in need: Time-dependent effects of stress on social discounting in men. Hormones and Behavior, 73, 7582. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.05.019Google Scholar
Marzi, T., Righi, S., Ottonello, S., Cincotta, M., & Viggiano, M. P. (2014). Trust at first sight: Evidence from ERPs. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 6372. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss102Google Scholar
Mather, M., & Lighthall, N. R. (2012). Risk and reward are processed differently in decisions made under stress. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 3641. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429452Google Scholar
Mende-Siedlecki, P., Said, C. P., & Todorov, A. (2013). The social evaluation of faces: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(3), 285299. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr090Google Scholar
Mende-Siedlecki, P., Verosky, S. C., Turk-Browne, N. B., & Todorov, A. (2013). Robust selectivity for faces in the human amygdala in the absence of expressions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(12), 20862106. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00469Google Scholar
Morelli, S. A., Leong, Y. C., Carlson, R. W., Kullar, M., & Zaki, J. (2018). Neural detection of socially valued community members. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(32), 81498154. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1712811115Google Scholar
Montague, P. R., Berns, G. S., Cohen, J. D., et al. (2002). Hyperscanning: Simultaneous fMRI during linked social interactions. NeuroImage, 16(4), 11591164. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1150Google Scholar
O’Doherty, J., Dayan, P., Schultz, J., Deichmann, R., Friston, K., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science, 304(5669), 452454. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1094285Google Scholar
O’Doherty, J. P., Lee, S. W., & McNamee, D. (2015). The structure of reinforcement-learning mechanisms in the human brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 1, 94100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.10.004Google Scholar
Oh, D., Shafir, E., & Todorov, A. (2020). Economic status cues from clothes affect perceived competence from faces. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(3), 287293. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0782-4Google Scholar
Olcaysoy Okten, I., Magerman, A., & Forbes, C. E. (2020). Behavioral and neural indices of trust formation in cross-race and same-race interactions. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 13(2), 100125. https://doi.org/10.1037/npe0000127Google Scholar
Olivola, C. Y., & Todorov, A. (2010). Elected in 100 milliseconds: Appearance-based trait inferences and voting. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 34(2), 83110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-009-0082-1Google Scholar
Oosterhof, N. N., & Todorov, A. (2009). Shared perceptual basis of emotional expressions and trustworthiness impressions from faces. Emotion, 9(1), 128133. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014520Google Scholar
Park, B., Fareri, D., Delgado, M., & Young, L. (2020). The role of right temporoparietal junction in processing social prediction error across relationship contexts. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa072Google Scholar
Park, B., & Young, L. (2020). An association between biased impression updating and relationship facilitation: A behavioral and fMRI investigation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 87, Article 103916. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103916Google Scholar
Phan, K. L., Sripada, C. S., Angstadt, M., & McCabe, K. (2010). Reputation for reciprocity engages the brain reward center. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(29), 1309913104. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1008137107Google Scholar
Plessow, F., Fischer, R., Kirschbaum, C., & Goschke, T. (2011). Inflexibly focused under stress: Acute psychosocial stress increases shielding of action goals at the expense of reduced cognitive flexibility with increasing time lag to the stressor. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 32183227. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00024Google Scholar
Plessow, F., Kiesel, A., & Kirschbaum, C. (2012). The stressed prefrontal cortex and goal-directed behaviour: Acute psychosocial stress impairs the flexible implementation of task goals. Experimental Brain Research, 216(3), 397408. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2943-1Google Scholar
Porcelli, A. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2017). Stress and decision making: Effects on valuation, learning, and risk-taking. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 14, 3339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.11.015Google Scholar
Porcelli, A. J., Lewis, A. H., & Delgado, M. R. (2012). Acute stress influences neural circuits of reward processing. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6(157). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00157Google Scholar
Radell, M. L., Sanchez, R., Weinflash, N., & Myers, C. E. (2016). The personality trait of behavioral inhibition modulates perceptions of moral character and performance during the trust game: Behavioral results and computational modeling. PeerJ, 4, Article e1631. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1631Google Scholar
Raio, C. M., Orederu, T. A., Palazzolo, L., Shurick, A. A., & Phelps, E. A. (2013). Cognitive emotion regulation fails the stress test. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(37), 1513915144. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1305706110Google Scholar
Raposa, E. B., Laws, H. B., & Ansell, E. B. (2015). Prosocial behavior mitigates the negative effects of stress in everyday life. Clinical Psychological Science, 4(4), 691698. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702615611073Google Scholar
Rescorla, R., & Wagner, A. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F. (Eds)., Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 6499). Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Rezlescu, C., Duchaine, B., Olivola, C. Y., & Chater, N. (2012). Unfakeable facial configurations affect strategic choices in trust games with or without information about past behavior. PLoS ONE, 7(3), Article e34293. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034293Google Scholar
Roozendaal, B., McReynolds, J. R., Van der Zee, E. A., Lee, S., McGaugh, J. L., & McIntyre, C. K. (2009). Glucocorticoid effects on memory consolidation depend on functional interactions between the medial prefrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(45), 1429914308. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3626-09.2009Google Scholar
Rule, N. O., Freeman, J. B., Moran, J. M., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Adams, R. B., & Ambady, N. (2010). Voting behavior is reflected in amygdala response across cultures. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2–3), 349355. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp046Google Scholar
Rule, N. O., Krendl, A. C., Ivcevic, Z., & Ambady, N. (2013). Accuracy and consensus in judgments of trustworthiness from faces: Behavioral and neural correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(3), 409426. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031050CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Said, C. P., Baron, S. G., & Todorov, A. (2009). Nonlinear amygdala response to face trustworthiness: Contributions of high and low spatial frequency information. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 519528. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21041Google Scholar
Salam, A. P., Rainford, E., Van Vugt, M., & Ronay, R. (2017). Acute stress reduces perceived trustworthiness of male racial outgroup faces. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 3(4), 282292. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-017-0065-0Google Scholar
Santos, S., Almeida, I., Oliveiros, B., & Castelo-Branco, M. (2016). The role of the amygdala in facial trustworthiness processing: A systematic review and meta-analyses of fMRI Studies. PLoS ONE, 11(11), Article e0167276. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0167276Google Scholar
Schilke, O., Reimann, M., & Cook, K. S. (2013). Effect of relationship experience on trust recovery following a breach. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(38), 1523615241. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1314857110Google Scholar
Schoofs, D., Wolf, O. T., & Smeets, T. (2009). Cold pressor stress impairs performance on working memory tasks requiring executive functions in healthy young men. Behavioral Neuroscience, 123(5), 10661075. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016980Google Scholar
Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275(5306), 15931599. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.275.5306.1593Google Scholar
Schweda, A., Faber, N. S., Crockett, M. J., & Kalenscher, T. (2019). The effects of psychosocial stress on intergroup resource allocation. Scientific Reports, 9(1), Article 18620. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-54954-wGoogle Scholar
Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(9), 23492356. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5587-06.2007Google Scholar
Shteingart, H., & Loewenstein, Y. (2014). Reinforcement learning and human behavior. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 25, 9398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.12.004Google Scholar
Sofer, C., Dotsch, R., Oikawa, M., Oikawa, H., Wigboldus, D. H. J., & Todorov, A. (2017). For your local eyes only: Culture-specific face typicality influences perceptions of trustworthiness. Perception, 46(8), 914928. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006617691786Google Scholar
Stanley, D. A. (2016). Getting to know you: General and specific neural computations for learning about people. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(4), 525536. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv145Google Scholar
Stanley, D. A., Sokol-Hessner, P., Banaji, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2011). Implicit race attitudes predict trustworthiness judgments and economic trust decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(19), 77107715. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1014345108Google Scholar
Stanley, D. A., Sokol-Hessner, P., Fareri, D. S., et al. (2012). Race and reputation: Perceived racial group trustworthiness influences the neural correlates of trust decisions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1589), 744753. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0300Google Scholar
Steinbeis, N., Engert, V., Linz, R., & Singer, T. (2015). The effects of stress and affiliation on social decision making: Investigating the tend-and-befriend pattern. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 62, 138148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.003Google Scholar
Sutton, R. S., & Barto, A. G. (1998). Reinforcement learning: An introduction. The MIT Press. https://muse.jhu.edu/book/60836Google Scholar
Suzuki, A., Ito, Y., Kiyama, S., et al. (2016). Involvement of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in learning others’ bad reputations and indelible distrust. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, Article 28. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00028Google Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In Worchel, S. and Austin, W. G. (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 724). Hall Publishers.Google Scholar
Tashjian, S. M., Guassi Moreira, J. F., & Galván, A. (2019). Multivoxel pattern analysis reveals a neural phenotype for trust bias in adolescents. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(11), 17261741. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01448Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E. (2006). Tend and befriend: Biobehavioral bases of affiliation under stress. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 273277. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00451.xGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. E., & Master, S. L. (2011). Social responses to stress: The tend-and-befriend model. In Contrada, R. J. & Baum, A. (Eds.), The handbook of stress science: Biology, psychology, and health (pp. 101109). Springer.Google Scholar
Theeuwes, J., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2006). Faces capture attention: Evidence from inhibition of return. Visual Cognition, 13(6), 657665. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280500410949Google Scholar
Todorov, A. (2012). The role of the amygdala in face perception and evaluation. Motivation and Emotion, 36(1), 1626. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9238-5Google Scholar
Todorov, A., Baron, S. G., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2008). Evaluating face trustworthiness: A model based approach. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3(2), 119127. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsn009Google Scholar
Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., & Hall, C. C. (2005). Inferences of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science, 308(5728), 16231626. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1110589Google Scholar
Todorov, A., Mende-Siedlecki, P., & Dotsch, R. (2013). Social judgments from faces. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(3), 373380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.010Google Scholar
Todorov, A., Olivola, C. Y., Dotsch, R., & Mende-Siedlecki, P. (2015). Social attributions from faces: Determinants, consequences, accuracy, and functional significance. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 519545. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143831Google Scholar
Todorov, A., Pakrashi, M., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2009). Evaluating faces on trustworthiness after minimal time exposure. Social Cognition, 27(6), 813833. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.813Google Scholar
Utevsky, A. V., Smith, D. V., Young, J. S., & Huettel, S. A. (2017). Large-scale network coupling with the fusiform cortex facilitates future social motivation. ENeuro, 4(5). https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0084-17.2017Google Scholar
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2012). Differential susceptibility experiments: Going beyond correlational evidence: Comment on beyond mental health, differential susceptibility articles. Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 769774. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027536Google Scholar
Van’t Wout, M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2008). Friend or foe: The effect of implicit trustworthiness judgments in social decision making. Cognition, 108(3), 796803. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.002Google Scholar
Vanyukov, P. M., Hallquist, M. N., Delgado, M., Szanto, K., & Dombrovski, A. Y. (2019). Neurocomputational mechanisms of adaptive learning in social exchanges. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19(4), 985997. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-019-00697-0Google Scholar
Vieira, J., Schellhaas, S., Enström, E., & Olsson, A. (2020). Help or flight? Increased threat imminence promotes defensive helping in humans. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/bckn3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Dawans, B., Ditzen, B., Trueg, A., Fischbacher, U., & Heinrichs, M. (2019). Effects of acute stress on social behavior in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 99, 137144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.08.031Google Scholar
von Dawans, B., Fischbacher, U., Kirschbaum, C., Fehr, E., & Heinrichs, M. (2012). The social dimension of stress reactivity: Acute stress increases prosocial behavior in humans. Psychological Science, 23(6), 651660. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611431576Google Scholar
Wardle, M. C., Fitzgerald, D. A., Angstadt, M., Sripada, C. S., McCabe, K., & Luan Phan, K. (2013). The caudate signals bad reputation during trust decisions. PLoS ONE, 8(6), Article e68884. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068884Google Scholar
Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2000). Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 748762. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.748Google Scholar
Wilson, J. P., & Rule, N. O. (2015). Facial trustworthiness predicts extreme criminal-sentencing outcomes. Psychological Science, 26(8), 13251331. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615590992Google Scholar
Winston, J. S., Strange, B. A., O’Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nature Neuroscience, 5(3), 277283. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn816Google Scholar
Wu, C.-T., Fan, Y.-T., Du, Y.-R., et al. (2018). How do acquired political identities influence our neural processing toward others within the context of a trust game? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, Article 23. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00023Google Scholar
Yan, X., Yong, X., Huang, W., & Ma, Y. (2018). Placebo treatment facilitates social trust and approach behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(22), 57325737. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1800779115Google Scholar
Zaki, J., Kallman, S., Wimmer, G. E., Ochsner, K., & Shohamy, D. (2016). Social cognition as reinforcement learning: Feedback modulates emotion inference. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(9), 12701282. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00978Google Scholar
Zarolia, P., Weisbuch, M., & McRae, K. (2017). Influence of indirect information on interpersonal trust despite direct information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(1), 3957. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000074Google Scholar

References

Adelstein, J. S., Shehzad, Z., Mennes, M., et al. (2011). Personality is reflected in the brain’s intrinsic functional architecture. PLoS ONE, 6(11), Article e27633. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633Google Scholar
Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O’Connor, E., & Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 32(3), 537551. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00491-3Google Scholar
Aimone, J. A., & Houser, D. (2013). Harnessing the benefits of betrayal aversion. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 89, 18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2013.02.001Google Scholar
Alves, P. N., Foulon, C., Karolis, V., et al. (2019). An improved neuroanatomical model of the default-mode network reconciles previous neuroimaging and neuropathological findings. Communication Biology, 2, Article 370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s42003-019-0611-3Google Scholar
Ashraf, N., Bohnet, I., & Piankov, N. (2006). Decomposing trust and trustworthiness. Experimental Economics, 9(3), 193208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10683-006-9122-4Google Scholar
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(2), 150166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868306294907Google Scholar
Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., & de Vries, R. E. (2014). The HEXACO honesty-humility, agreeableness, and emotionality factors: A review of research and theory. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18(2), 139152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868314523838Google Scholar
Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., Perugini, M., et al. (2004). A six-factor structure of personality-descriptive adjectives: Solutions from psycholexical studies in seven languages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(2), 356366. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.356Google Scholar
Becker, J., Brackbill, D., & Centola, D. (2017). Network dynamics of social influence in the wisdom of crowds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(26), E5070E5076. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1615978114Google Scholar
Behrens, T. E., Hunt, L. T., Woolrich, M. W., & Rushworth, M. F. (2008). Associative learning of social value. Nature, 456(7219), 245249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature07538Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Chernyak, S. V., Goodyear, K., Eickhoff, S. B., & Krueger, F. (2017). Neural signatures of trust in reciprocity: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 38(3), 12331248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23451Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Hahn, T., Deshpande, G., & Krueger, F. (2019). Functional connectivity of specific resting-state networks predicts trust and reciprocity in the trust game. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19(1), 165176. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-00654-3Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Molter, F., & Park, S. Q. (2019). Neural representations of honesty predict future trust behavior. Nature Communications, 10(1), Article 5184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-13261-8Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Münte, T. F., & Park, S. Q. (2020). Effects of a dopamine agonist on trusting behaviors in females. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 237(6), 16711680. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-020-05488-xGoogle Scholar
Bellucci, G., Münte, T. F., & Park, S. Q. (in press). Value computations under social uncertainty and serotonin.Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., & Park, S. Q. (2020). Honesty biases trustworthiness impressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(8), 15671586. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000730Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., & Park, S. Q. (in press). Neurocomputational mechanisms of cognitive biases in impression formation.Google Scholar
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10(1), 122142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/game.1995.1027Google Scholar
Biele, G., Rieskamp, J., & Gonzalez, R. (2009). Computational models for the combination of advice and individual learning. Cognitive Science, 33(2), 206242. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01010.xGoogle Scholar
Biele, G., Rieskamp, J., Krugel, L. K., & Heekeren, H. R. (2011). The neural basis of following advice. PLoS Biology, 9(6), Article e1001089. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001089Google Scholar
Bohnet, I., & Zeckhauser, R. (2004). Trust, risk and betrayal. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 55(4), 467484. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2003.11.004Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.011Google Scholar
Burnham, T., McCabe, K., & Smith, V. L. (2000). Friend-or-foe intentionality priming in an extensive form trust game. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 43(1), 5773. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0167-2681(00)00108-6Google Scholar
Burt, R. S., & Knez, M. (1995). Kinds of third-party effects on trust. Rationality and Society, 7(3), 255292.Google Scholar
Camerer, C. F. (2003). Behavioural studies of strategic thinking in games. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(5), 225231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00094-9Google Scholar
Chang, L. J., Doll, B. B., Van’t Wout, M., Frank, M. J., & Sanfey, A. G. (2010). Seeing is believing: Trustworthiness as a dynamic belief. Cognitive Psychology, 61(2), 87105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.03.001Google Scholar
Chaudhuri, A., & Gangadharan, L. (2007). An experimental analysis of trust and trustworthiness. Southern Economic Journal, 73(4), 959985. https://doi.org/10.2307/20111937Google Scholar
Chaudhuri, A., Sopher, B., & Strand, P. (2002). Cooperation in social dilemmas, trust and reciprocity. Journal of Economic Psychology, 23(2), 231249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0167-4870(02)00065-xGoogle Scholar
Cheong, J. H., Jolly, E., Sul, S., & Chang, L. J. (2017). Computational models in social neuroscience. In Moustafa, A. A. (Ed.), Computational models of brain and behavior (1st ed., pp. 229244). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Google Scholar
Coleman, J. (1990). Foundations of social theory. The Belknap Press of Harvard University.Google Scholar
Csukás, C., Fracalanza, P., Kovács, T., & Willinger, M. (2008). The determinants of trusting and reciprocal behaviour: Evidence from an intercultural experiment. Journal of Economic Development, 33(1), 7195. http://dx.doi.org/10.35866/caujed.2008.33.1.004Google Scholar
Das, T. K., & Teng, B.-S. (1998). Between trust and control: Developing confidence in partner cooperation in alliances. Academy of Management Review, 23(3), 491512. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amr.1998.926623Google Scholar
Das, T. K., & Teng, B.-S. (2001). Trust, control, and risk in strategic alliances: An integrated framework. Organization Studies, 22(2), 251283. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0170840601222004Google Scholar
Delgado, M. R., Frank, R. H., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 16111618. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1575Google Scholar
Deutsch, M. (1958). Trust and suspicion. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 2(4), 265279.Google Scholar
Diaconescu, A. O., Mathys, C., Weber, L. A. E., Kasper, L., Mauer, J., & Stephan, K. E. (2017). Hierarchical prediction errors in midbrain and septum during social learning. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(4), 618634. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw171Google Scholar
Dosenbach, N. U., Fair, D. A., Miezin, F. M., et al. (2007). Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(26), 1107311078. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0704320104Google Scholar
Dosenbach, N. U., Nardos, B., Cohen, A. L., et al. (2010). Prediction of individual brain maturity using fMRI. Science, 329(5997), 13581361. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1194144Google Scholar
Dreher, J. C., Kohn, P., & Berman, K. F. (2006). Neural coding of distinct statistical properties of reward information in humans. Cerebral Cortex, 16(4), 561573. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj004Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Anderson, J. E., Schlosser, T., Ehlebracht, D., & Fetchenhauer, D. (2014). Trust at zero acquaintance: More a matter of respect than expectation of reward. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(1), 122141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036673Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Fetchenhauer, D., & Schlösser, T. (2019). Why people trust: Solved puzzles and open mysteries. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(4), 366371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721419838255Google Scholar
Ekerdt, C. E. M., Kuhn, C., Anwander, A., Brauer, J., & Friederici, A. D. (2020). Word learning reveals white matter plasticity in preschool children. Brain Structure and Function, 225(2), 607619. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-020-02024-7Google Scholar
Everett, J. A., Pizarro, D. A., & Crockett, M. J. (2016). Inference of trustworthiness from intuitive moral judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(6), 772787. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000165Google Scholar
Fairley, K., Sanfey, A. G., Vyrastekova, J., & Weitzel, U. (2016). Trust and risk revisited. Journal of Economic Psychology, 57, 7485. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2016.10.001Google Scholar
Fareri, D. S., Chang, L. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2012). Effects of direct social experience on trust decisions and neural reward circuitry. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6, Article 148. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00148Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425(6960), 785791. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature02043Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Social norms and human cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 185190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.007Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817868. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/003355399556151Google Scholar
Fetchenhauer, D., & Dunning, D. (2009). Do people trust too much or too little? Journal of Economic Psychology, 30(3), 263276. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2008.04.006Google Scholar
Finkl, T., Hahne, A., Friederici, A. D., Gerber, J., Murbe, D., & Anwander, A. (2020). Language without speech: Segregating distinct circuits in the human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 30(2), 812823. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz128Google Scholar
Fletcher, P. C., Happe, F., Frith, U., et al. (1995). Other minds in the brain: A functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition, 57(2), 109128. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(95)00692-RGoogle Scholar
Fouragnan, E., Chierchia, G., Greiner, S., Neveu, R., Avesani, P., & Coricelli, G. (2013). Reputational priors magnify striatal responses to violations of trust. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(8), 36023611. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/Jneurosci.3086-12.2013Google Scholar
Frost, T., Stimpson, D. V., & Maughan, M. R. (1978). Some correlates of trust. Journal of Psychology, 99(1st Half), 103108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1978.9921447Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1907). Vox populi. Nature, 75(1949), 450451. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/075450a0Google Scholar
Gershman, S. J., & Daw, N. D. (2017). Reinforcement learning and episodic memory in humans and animals: An integrative framework. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 101128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033625Google Scholar
Gordon, R., & Spears, K. (2012). You don’t act like you trust me: Dissociations between behavioural and explicit measures of source credibility judgement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(1), 121134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.591534Google Scholar
Gottfried, J. A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Human orbitofrontal cortex mediates extinction learning while accessing conditioned representations of value. Nature Neuroscience, 7(10), 11441152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1314Google Scholar
Grèzes, J., Fonlupt, P., Bertenthal, B., Delon-Martin, C., Segebarth, C., & Decety, J. (2001). Does perception of biological motion rely on specific brain regions? NeuroImage, 13(5), 775785. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0740Google Scholar
Gromann, P. M., Heslenfeld, D. J., Fett, A. K., Joyce, D. W., Shergill, S. S., & Krabbendam, L. (2013). Trust versus paranoia: Abnormal response to social reward in psychotic illness. Brain, 136(Pt 6), 19681975. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt076Google Scholar
Gromann, P. M., Shergill, S. S., de Haan, L., et al. (2014). Reduced brain reward response during cooperation in first-degree relatives of patients with psychosis: An fMRI study. Psychological Medicine, 44(16), 34453454. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714000737Google Scholar
Hahn, T., Notebaert, K., Anderl, C., Reicherts, P., et al. (2015). Reliance on functional resting-state network for stable task control predicts behavioral tendency for cooperation. NeuroImage, 118, 231236. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.093Google Scholar
Hahn, T., Notebaert, K., Anderl, C., Teckentrup, V., Kassecker, A., & Windmann, S. (2015). How to trust a perfect stranger: Predicting initial trust behavior from resting-state brain-electrical connectivity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(6), 809813. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu122Google Scholar
Hampton, A. N., Bossaerts, P., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2008). Neural correlates of mentalizing-related computations during strategic interactions in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(18), 67416746. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0711099105Google Scholar
Hertz, U., Palminteri, S., Brunetti, S., Olesen, C., Frith, C. D., & Bahrami, B. (2017). Neural computations underpinning the strategic management of influence in advice giving. Nature Communications, 8(1), Article 2191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-02314-5Google Scholar
Hillebrandt, H., Sebastian, C., & Blakemore, S. J. (2011). Experimentally induced social inclusion influences behavior on trust games. Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(1), 2733. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2010.515020Google Scholar
Ho, T. H., & Weigelt, K. (2005). Trust building among strangers. Management Science, 51(4), 519530. http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.1040.0350Google Scholar
Hollerman, J. R., & Schultz, W. (1998). Dopamine neurons report an error in the temporal prediction of reward during learning. Nature Neuroscience, 1(4), 304309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/1124Google Scholar
Howard, J. D., & Kahnt, T. (2018). Identity prediction errors in the human midbrain update reward-identity expectations in the orbitofrontal cortex. Nature Communications, 9(1), Article 1611. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-04055-5Google Scholar
Hula, A., Montague, P. R., & Dayan, P. (2015). Monte Carlo planning method estimates planning horizons during interactive social exchange. PLoS Computational Biology, 11(6), Article e1004254. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004254Google Scholar
Hula, A., Vilares, I., Lohrenz, T., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (2018). A model of risk and mental state shifts during social interaction. PLoS Computational Biology, 14(2), Article e1005935. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005935Google Scholar
Ingvar, D. H. (1974). Patterns of brain activity revealed by measurements of regional cerebral blood flow. Paper presented at the Alfred Benzon Symposium VIII, Copenhagen.Google Scholar
Joiner, J., Piva, M., Turrin, C., & Chang, S. W. C. (2017). Social learning through prediction error in the brain. NPJ Science of Learning, 2, Article 8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41539-017-0009-2Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263291.Google Scholar
Khalvati, K., Park, S. A., Mirbagheri, S., et al. (2019). Modeling other minds: Bayesian inference explains human choices in group decision making. Science Advances, 5(11), Article eaax8783. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aax8783Google Scholar
King-Casas, B., Sharp, C., Lomax-Bream, L., Lohrenz, T., Fonagy, P., & Montague, P. R. (2008). The rupture and repair of cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Science, 321(5890), 806810. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1156902Google Scholar
King-Casas, B., Tomlin, D., Anen, C., Camerer, C. F., Quartz, S. R., & Montague, P. R. (2005). Getting to know you: Reputation and trust in a two-person economic exchange. Science, 308(5718), 7883. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1108062Google Scholar
Koster-Hale, J., Richardson, H., Velez, N., Asaba, M., Young, L., & Saxe, R. (2017). Mentalizing regions represent distributed, continuous, and abstract dimensions of others’ beliefs. NeuroImage, 161, 918. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.026Google Scholar
Krueger, F., Grafman, J., & McCabe, K. (2008). Neural correlates of economic game playing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1511), 38593874. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0165Google Scholar
Krueger, F., McCabe, K., Moll, J., et al. (2007). Neural correlates of trust. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(50), 2008420089. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0710103104Google Scholar
Kunisato, Y., Okamoto, Y., Okada, G., et al. (2011). Personality traits and the amplitude of spontaneous low-frequency oscillations during resting state. Neuroscience Letters, 492(2), 109113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.01.067Google Scholar
Lamba, A., Frank, M. J., & FeldmanHall, O. (2020). Anxiety impedes adaptive social learning under uncertainty. Psychological Science, 31(5), 592603. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797620910993Google Scholar
Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the HEXACO personality inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(2), 329358. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_8Google Scholar
Lewis, J. D., & Weigert, A. (1985). Trust as a social reality. Social Forces, 63(4), 967985. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2578601Google Scholar
Li, Y., Vanni-Mercier, G., Isnard, J., Mauguiere, F., & Dreher, J. C. (2016). The neural dynamics of reward value and risk coding in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Brain, 139(Pt 4), 12951309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awv409Google Scholar
Ligneul, R., Obeso, I., Ruff, C. C., & Dreher, J. C. (2016). Dynamical representation of dominance relationships in the human rostromedial prefrontal cortex. Current Biology, 26(23), 31073115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.09.015Google Scholar
Lis, S., Baer, N., Franzen, N., et al. (2016). Social interaction behavior in ADHD in adults in a virtual trust game. Journal of Attention Disorders, 20(4), 335345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1087054713482581Google Scholar
Lockwood, P. L., Apps, M. A., Valton, V., Viding, E., & Roiser, J. P. (2016). Neurocomputational mechanisms of prosocial learning and links to empathy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(35), 97639768. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1603198113Google Scholar
Lu, X., Li, T., Xia, Z., et al. (2019). Connectome-based model predicts individual differences in propensity to trust. Human Brain Mapping, 40(6), 19421954. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24503Google Scholar
Luhmann, N. (1979). Trust: A mechanism for the reduction of social complexity. In Luhmann, N. (Ed.), Trust and power (pp. 4103). Wiley.Google Scholar
Ma, I., Sanfey, A. G., & Ma, W. J. (2020). The social cost of gathering information for trust decisions. Scientific Reports, 10(1), Article 14073. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598–020-69766-6Google Scholar
Mahmoodi, A., Bahrami, B., & Mehring, C. (2018). Reciprocity of social influence. Nature Communications, 9(1), Article 2474. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-04925-yGoogle Scholar
Malhotra, D. (2004). Trust and reciprocity decisions: The differing perspectives of trustors and trusted parties. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 94(2), 6173. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.03.001Google Scholar
Malhotra, D., & Murnighan, J. K. (2002). The effects of contracts on interpersonal trust. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47(3), 534559. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3094850Google Scholar
Maurer, C., Chambon, V., Bourgeois-Gironde, S., Leboyer, M., & Zalla, T. (2018). The influence of prior reputation and reciprocity on dynamic trust-building in adults with and without autism spectrum disorder. Cognition, 172, 110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.11.007Google Scholar
Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative model of organizational trust. The Academy of Management Review, 20(3), 709734.Google Scholar
McCabe, K. A., Rigdon, M. L., & Smith, V. L. (2003). Positive reciprocity and intentions in trust games. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 52(2), 267275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0167-2681(03)00003-9Google Scholar
McGinnies, E., & Ward, C. D. (1980). Better liked than right: Trustworthiness and expertise as factors in credibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6(3), 467472. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014616728063023Google Scholar
Mellers, B., Ungar, L., Baron, J., et al. (2014). Psychological strategies for winning a geopolitical forecasting tournament. Psychological Science, 25(5), 11061115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797614524255Google Scholar
Meshi, D., Biele, G., Korn, C. W., & Heekeren, H. R. (2012). How expert advice influences decision making. PLoS ONE, 7(11), Article e49748. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049748Google Scholar
Metereau, E., & Dreher, J. C. (2015). The medial orbitofrontal cortex encodes a general unsigned value signal during anticipation of both appetitive and aversive events. Cortex, 63, 4254. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.08.012Google Scholar
Montague, P. R., Dayan, P., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1996). A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 16(5), 19361947. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.16-05-01936.1996Google Scholar
Na, S., Chung, D., Hula, A., et al. (2019). Humans use forward thinking to exert social control. bioRxiv. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/737353Google Scholar
O’Doherty, J., Dayan, P., Schultz, J., Deichmann, R., Friston, K., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science, 304(5669), 452454. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1094285Google Scholar
O’Doherty, J., Winston, J., Critchley, H., Perrett, D., Burt, D. M., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Beauty in a smile: The role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 147155. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00145-8Google Scholar
Park, S. A., Sestito, M., Boorman, E. D., & Dreher, J. C. (2019). Neural computations underlying strategic social decision making in groups. Nature Communications, 10(1), Article 5287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-12937-5Google Scholar
Pegors, T. K., Kable, J. W., Chatterjee, A., & Epstein, R. A. (2015). Common and unique representations in pFC for face and place attractiveness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(5), 959973. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00777Google Scholar
Phan, K. L., Sripada, C. S., Angstadt, M., & McCabe, K. (2010). Reputation for reciprocity engages the brain reward center. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(29), 1309913104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1008137107Google Scholar
Pillutla, M. M., Malhotra, D., & Keith Murnighan, J. (2003). Attributions of trust and the calculus of reciprocity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(5), 448455. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0022-1031(03)00015-5Google Scholar
Platt, M. L., & Huettel, S. A. (2008). Risky business: The neuroeconomics of decision making under uncertainty. Nature Neuroscience, 11(4), 398403. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn2062Google Scholar
Preuschoff, K., Bossaerts, P., & Quartz, S. R. (2006). Neural differentiation of expected reward and risk in human subcortical structures. Neuron, 51(3), 381390. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.024Google Scholar
Price, C. J. (2012). A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. NeuroImage, 62(2), 816847. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.062Google Scholar
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(2), 676682. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.98.2.676Google Scholar
Reichelt, J., Sievert, J., & Jacob, F. (2013). How credibility affects eWOM reading: The influences of expertise, trustworthiness, and similarity on utilitarian and social functions. Journal of Marketing Communications, 20(1–2), 6581. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13527266.2013.797758Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. Current Research and Theory, 64–99, Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, M. D., Finn, E. S., Scheinost, D., et al. (2016). A neuromarker of sustained attention from whole-brain functional connectivity. Nature Neuroscience, 19(1), 165171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.4179Google Scholar
Rothbart, M., & Park, B. (1986). On the confirmability and disconfirmability of trait concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(1), 131142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.1.131Google Scholar
Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. (1998). Not so different after all: A cross-discipline view of trust. Academy of Management Review, 23(3), 393404. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amr.1998.926617Google Scholar
Rudebeck, P. H., & Murray, E. A. (2014). The orbitofrontal oracle: Cortical mechanisms for the prediction and evaluation of specific behavioral outcomes. Neuron, 84(6), 11431156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.049Google Scholar
Rudebeck, P. H., Saunders, R. C., Prescott, A. T., Chau, L. S., & Murray, E. A. (2013). Prefrontal mechanisms of behavioral flexibility, emotion regulation and value updating. Nature Neuroscience, 16(8), 11401145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.3440Google Scholar
Sánchez-Pagés, S., & Vorsatz, M. (2007). An experimental study of truth-telling in a sender–receiver game. Games and Economic Behavior, 61(1), 86112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.geb.2006.10.014Google Scholar
Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people: The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind.” NeuroImage, 19(4), 18351842. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00230-1Google Scholar
Schultz, W. (2000). Multiple reward signals in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1(3), 199207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/35044563Google Scholar
Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275(5306), 15931599. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.275.5306.1593Google Scholar
Siegel, J. Z., Estrada, S., Crockett, M. J., & Baskin-Sommers, A. (2019). Exposure to violence affects the development of moral impressions and trust behavior in incarcerated males. Nature Communications, 10(1), Article 1942. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-09962-9Google Scholar
Siegel, J. Z., Mathys, C., Rutledge, R. B., & Crockett, M. J. (2018). Beliefs about bad people are volatile. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(10), 750756. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0425-1Google Scholar
Sjöberg, L. (2009). Are all crowds equally wise? A comparison of political election forecasts by experts and the public. Journal of Forecasting, 28(1), 118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/for.1083Google Scholar
Slovic, P. (1993). Perceived risk, trust, and democracy. Risk Analysis, 13(6), 675682. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1993.tb01329.xGoogle Scholar
Snijders, C., & Keren, G. (2001). Do you trust? Whom do you trust? When do you trust? Advances in Group Processes, 18, 129160. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0882-6145(01)18006-9Google Scholar
Sperber, D. A. N., Clément, F., Heintz, C., et al. (2010). Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language, 25(4), 359393. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01394.xGoogle Scholar
Sripada, C. S., Angstadt, M., Banks, S., Nathan, P. J., Liberzon, I., & Phan, K. L. (2009). Functional neuroimaging of mentalizing during the trust game in social anxiety disorder. Neuroreport, 20(11), 984989. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832d0a67Google Scholar
Stirrat, M., & Perrett, D. I. (2010). Valid facial cues to cooperation and trust: Male facial width and trustworthiness. Psychological Science, 21(3), 349354. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797610362647Google Scholar
Strickland, L. H. (1958). Surveillance and trust. Journal of Personality, 26(2), 200215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1958.tb01580.xGoogle Scholar
Thielmann, I., & Hilbig, B. E. (2015). The traits one can trust: Dissecting reciprocity and kindness as determinants of trustworthy behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(11), 15231536. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167215600530Google Scholar
Todorov, A., Pakrashi, M., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2009). Evaluating faces on trustworthiness after minimal time exposure. Social Cognition, 27(6), 813833. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.813Google Scholar
Toelch, U., Bach, D. R., & Dolan, R. J. (2014). The neural underpinnings of an optimal exploitation of social information under uncertainty. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(11), 17461753. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst173Google Scholar
Tsuchida, A., Doll, B. B., & Fellows, L. K. (2010). Beyond reversal: A critical role for human orbitofrontal cortex in flexible learning from probabilistic feedback. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(50), 1686816875. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1958-10.2010Google Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5(4), 297323. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/Bf00122574Google Scholar
Van Overwalle, F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 30(3), 829858. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20547Google Scholar
Van Overwalle, F., & Baetens, K. (2009). Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 48(3), 564584. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.009Google Scholar
Vanni-Mercier, G., Mauguiere, F., Isnard, J., & Dreher, J. C. (2009). The hippocampus codes the uncertainty of cue-outcome associations: An intracranial electrophysiological study in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(16), 52875294. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5298-08.2009Google Scholar
Wardle, M. C., Fitzgerald, D. A., Angstadt, M., Sripada, C. S., McCabe, K., & Phan, K. L. (2013). The caudate signals bad reputation during trust decisions. PLoS ONE, 8(6), Article e68884. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068884Google Scholar
Weiner, J. L., & Mowen, J. C. (1986). Source credibility: On the independent effects of trust and expertise. Advances in Consumer Research, 13, 306310.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. K., & Eckel, C. C. (2006). Judging a book by its cover: Beauty and expectations in the trust game. Political Research Quarterly, 59(2), 189202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/106591290605900202Google Scholar
Winston, J. S., O’Doherty, J., Kilner, J. M., Perrett, D. I., & Dolan, R. J. (2007). Brain systems for assessing facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 195206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.009Google Scholar
Wu, H., Liu, X., Hagan, C. C., & Mobbs, D. (2020). Mentalizing during social interaction: A four component model. Cortex, 126, 242252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2019.12.031Google Scholar
Xiang, T., Ray, D., Lohrenz, T., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (2012). Computational phenotyping of two-person interactions reveals differential neural response to depth-of-thought. PLoS Computational Biology, 8(12), Article e1002841. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002841Google Scholar
Yaniv, I. (2006). The benefit of additional opinions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 7578. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00278.xGoogle Scholar
Yaniv, I., & Kleinberger, E. (2000). Advice taking in decision making: Egocentric discounting and reputation formation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 83(2), 260281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/obhd.2000.2909Google Scholar
Zatorre, R. J. (2013). Predispositions and plasticity in music and speech learning: Neural correlates and implications. Science, 342(6158), 585589. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1238414Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×