Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-swr86 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-20T06:58:15.910Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - The Intersection of Language and Culture Among Mexican-Heritage Children 3 to 7 Years Old

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Allyssa McCabe
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Lowell
Alison L. Bailey
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Gigliana Melzi
Affiliation:
New York University
Get access

Summary

Key Words: cultural communities, narrative development, language practices, Mexican, bilingual

ABSTRACT

The primary goal of this chapter is to describe the development of narrative interactions from highly scaffolded to relatively independent narrative productions among a group of low-income Mexican-heritage children growing up in a major city in the southwestern United States. The results from this study simultaneously emphasize the cultural nature of narrative and language development coupled with the evident heterogeneity in cultural practices and developmental trajectories within a particular ethnic and linguistic group. Cultural practices are defined by the language practices enacted and endorsed by families. Variations in children's narrative development are discussed within the context of language practices, providing a platform to highlight the importance of taking a within-culture and longitudinal approach to understanding normative development among minority groups. By looking at patterns of narrative and language development within a cultural context, it is evident that these Mexican-heritage children experience a similar overall developmental trajectory as their European North American (ENA) peers and that there are important variations in narrative structure and adult scaffolding within a group often considered homogeneous. Thinking about cultural language practices appears to be necessary yet not sufficient in our understanding of an individual's development of narrative and language. The chapter ends with a discussion of the implications both for future research and applications with children and families in educational settings.

Type
Chapter
Information
Spanish-Language Narration and Literacy
Culture, Cognition, and Emotion
, pp. 146 - 174
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Azmitia, M., Cooper, C. R., García, E. E., & Dunbar, N. N. (1996). The ecology of family guidance in low-income Mexican American and European American families. Social Development, 5(1), 1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayley, R., & Schecter, S. R. (2003). Introduction: Toward a dynamic model of language socialization. In Bayley, R. & Schecter, S. R. (Eds.), Language socialization in bilingual and multilingual societies. Great Britain: Cromwell Press.Google Scholar
Bedore, L. (2004). Bilingual language development and disorders in Spanish–English speakers. In Goldstein, B. A. (Ed.), Bilingual language development and disorders in Spanish-English speakers (pp. 163–185). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Bishop, D., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Language-impaired 4-year-olds: Distinguishing transient from persistent impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 156–173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bland-Stewart, L. M., & Fitzgerald, Z. M. (2001). Use of Brown's 14 grammatical morphemes by bilingual Hispanic preschoolers: A pilot study. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 22(4), 171–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas at Little Rock.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 37–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Contreras, J. M., Kerns, K., & Neal-Barnett, A. M. (Eds.) (2002). Latino children and families in the United States: Current research and future directions. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Eisenberg, A. (1985). Learning to describe past experiences in conversation. Discourse processes, 8, 177–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallimore, R., & Goldenberg, C. (1993). Activity settings of early literacy: Home and school factors in children's emergent literacy. In Forman, E., Minick, N., & Stone, C.A. (Eds.), Contexts for learning: Sociocultural dynamics in children's development (pp. 315–335). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
García, E., & Cuéllar, D. (2006). Who are these linguistically and culturally diverse students?Teachers College Record, 108(11), 2220–2246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García-Coll, C., Contran, L., Jenkins, R., McAdoo, H. P., Crinic, K., Wasik, B. H., & García, H. V. (1996). An integrative model for the study of developmental competencies in minority children. Child Development, 67, 1891–1941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garret, P. B., & Baquedano-López, P. (2002). Language socialization: Reproduction and continuity, transformation and change. Annual Review of Anthropology, 31, 339–361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gleason, J. B., & Melzi, G. (1997). The mutual construction of narrative by mothers and children: Cross-cultural observations. Journal of Narrative and Life History. Special Issue: Oral Versions of Personal Experiences: Three Decades of Narrative Analysis, 7(1–4), 217–222.Google Scholar
Goodwin, M. H. (1990). Tactical uses of stories: Participation frameworks within girls' and boys' disputes. In Tannen, D. (Ed.) Gender and Conversational Interaction. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., & Hienrichs-Ramos, L. (1993). Referential cohesion in the narratives of Spanish-speaking children: A developmental study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 559–568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F. 2002. Narratives in two languages. Assessing performance of bilingual children. Linguistics and Education, 13, 175–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., & Iglesias, A. (1992). Causal coherence in the oral narratives of Spanish-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 363–372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harwood, R., Scholmeric, A., & Schulze, P. A. (2000). Homogeneity and heterogeneity in cultural belief systems. In Harkness, S., Raeff, C., & Super, C. M. (Eds.), New directions for child and adolescent development: Variability in the social construction of the child (pp. 41–57). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publications.Google Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1982). What no bedtime story means: Narrative skills at home and school. Language in Society, 11, 49–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words. Cambridge: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Howes, C., Wishard Guerra, A. G., & Zucker, E. (2007). Cultural communities and parenting practices in Mexican-heritage families. Parenting: Science and Practice, 7, 1–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howes, C., Wishard Guerra, A. G., & Zucker, E. (2008). Migrating from Mexico and sharing pretend with peers in the United States. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 54, 256–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imbens-Bailey, A. L., & Snow, C. E. (1997). Making meaning in parent–child interaction: A pragmatic approach. In McCabe, A. & Mandell, C., The problem of meaning: Behavioral and cognitive perspectives (pp. 261–295). A volume of Advances in Psychology, Amsterdam: North-Holland Elsevier Science B.V.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city: Studies in the Black English vernacular. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
McCabe, A. (1997). Developmental and cross-cultural aspects of children's narration. In Bamberg, Michael (Ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (Eds.). (1991). Developing Narrative Structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Melzi, G. (2000). Cultural variations in the construction of personal narratives: Central American and European American mothers' elicitation styles. Discourse Processes, 30, 2, 153–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minami, M., & McCabe, A. (1995). Rice balls and bear hunts: Japanese and North American family narrative patterns. Journal of Child Language, 22(2), 423–445.Google Scholar
Muñoz, M. L., Gillam, R. B., Peña, E. D., & Gulley-Faehnle, A. (2003). Measures of language development in fictional narratives of Latino children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 34, 332–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Center for Education (2003). The condition of education for Hispanic Americans.
Ochs, E., & Capps, L. (2001). The living narrative: Creating lives in everyday storytelling. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. (1995) The impact of language socialization on grammatical development. In Fletcher, P. and MacWhinney, B. (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 73–94). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child's narrative. New York, NY: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raikes, H., Pan, B. A., Luze, G., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Brooks-Gunn, J., Constantine, J., Tarullo, L., Raikes, A., & Rodriguez, E. (2006). Mother–child book-reading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Development, 77(4), 924–953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, L., Garnier, H., Gallimore, R., & Goldenberg, C. (2000). A longitudinal analysis of the ecocultural antecedents of emergent Spanish literacy and subsequent English reading achievement of Spanish-speaking students. American Educational Research Journal, 37(3), 633–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Gönçü, A., & Mosier, C. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58 (7, Serial No. 236).
Rosenblatt, S., Garza-Mourino, R., & Howes, C. (2004). How did you end up here? Collaborative ways of listening to Mexican immigrant mothers as they contemplate their lives there/then and here/now: The story of the Cultural Change Interview (CCI). Los Angeles: University of California, Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Schecter, S. R., & Bayley, B. (2002). Language as cultural practices: Mexicanos el en Norte. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Schieffelin, B., & Ochs, E. (1986). Language socialization. Annual Review of Anthropology, 15, 163–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva, M. J., & McCabe, A. (1996). Vignettes of the continuous and family ties: Some Latino American traditions. In McCabe, A. (Ed.), Chameleon readers: Teaching children to appreciate all kinds of good stories. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1983). Literacy and language: Relationships during the preschool years. Harvard Educational Review (53), 165–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperry, L., & Sperry, D. (1996). Early development of narrative skills. Cognitive Development, 11, 443–465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Census, U.S. Bureau (2001). The HispanicPopulation: Census 2000 Brief.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×