Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T04:41:20.026Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Assessment of mood

from Psychology, health and illness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2014

Michele M. Tugade
Affiliation:
Vassar College
Tamlin Conner
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut Health Center
Lisa Feldman Barrett
Affiliation:
Boston College
Susan Ayers
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Andrew Baum
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Chris McManus
Affiliation:
St Mary's Hospital Medical School
Stanton Newman
Affiliation:
University College and Middlesex School of Medicine
Kenneth Wallston
Affiliation:
Vanderbilt University School of Nursing
John Weinman
Affiliation:
United Medical and Dental Schools of Guy's and St Thomas's
Robert West
Affiliation:
St George's Hospital Medical School, University of London
Get access

Summary

Theoretical and methodological advances in psychology, physiology and medicine have led to rigorous examinations of the role of affect and emotion in health. In this chapter, we review the role of negative and positive emotions in health research and then discuss some of the most prominent measures currently used to measure mood in this research. We conclude with specific recommendations for the measurement of mood and emotion in the context of studies of physical health. Across different samples and studies in health psychology, there is little variation in mood assessment procedures. As a consequence, we focus our discussions primarily on the cardiovascular system, with a shorter discussion of relations between cancer and mood.

Negative emotions and health

Much of the research that examines the relation between mood and health addresses the impact of negative or unpleasant affect. Although the experience of negative affect is generally adaptive in preparing the body for fight-or-flight, it can have adverse consequences when the body is continually taxed. In particular, researchers have focused on how particular experiences of negative affect (e.g. anger, anxiety and depression) have emerged as important risk factors in health (see Gallo & Matthews, 2003; Kubzansky & Kawachi, 2000).

Anger

Several studies have reported on the negative health consequences of anger on cardiovascular responses (e.g. Kawachi et al., 1996), particularly in relation to incidence of coronary heart disease (CHD). Hostility appeared to be a greater risk factor than smoking, high blood pressure and high cholesterol (Chaput et al., 2002).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, N. B. (2003). Emotional longevity. New York: Viking.
Appel, M. A., Holroyd, K. A. & Gorkin, L. (1983). Anger and the etiology and progression of physical illness. In Temoshok, L., Dyke, L. & Zegans, L. S. (Eds.). Emotions in health and illness: theoretical and research foundations. Orlando, FL: Grune und Stratton.
Aspinwall, L. G. & Taylor, S. E. (1997). A stitch in time: self-regulation and proactive coping. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 417–36.Google Scholar
Bachorowski, J.-A. & Owren, M. J. (1995). Vocal expression of emotion: acoustic properties of speech are associated with emotional intensity and context. Psychological Science, 6, 219–24.Google Scholar
Bachorowski, J.-A. (1999). Vocal expression and perception of emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 53–7.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (1997). The relationships among momentary emotion experiences, personality descriptions, and retrospective ratings of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1100–10.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (1998). Discrete emotions or dimensions? The role of valence focus and arousal focus. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 579–99.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2004). Feelings or words? Understanding the content in self-report ratings of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 266–81.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2005). Feeling is perceiving: core effect and conceptualization in the experience of emotion. In Barrett, L. F., Niedenthal, P. & Winkielman, P. (Eds.). Emotions: conscious and unconscious. (pp. 255–84). New York: Guilford.
Barrett, L. F. & Barrett, D.J. (2001). Computerized experience-sampling: how technology facilitates the study of conscious experience. Social Science Computer Review, 19, 175–85. (Invited contribution).Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. & Niedenthal, P. M. (2004). Valence focus and the perception of facial affect. Emotion, 4, 266–74.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Pietromonaco, P. R. & Eyssell, K. M. (1998). Are women the “more emotional sex?” Evidence from emotional experiences in social context. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 555–78.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. & Russell, J. A. (1998). Independence and bipolarity in the structure of current affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 967–84.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Gross, J., Conner, T. & Benvenuto, M. (2001). Emotion differentiation and regulation. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 713–24.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Quigley, K., Bliss-Moreau, E. & Aronson, K. R. (2004). Arousal focus and interoceptive sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 684–97.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T. (1996). Beck depression inventory. New York: Harcourt.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A. & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck depression inventory – Second edition manual. USA: Harcourt Assessment/The Psychological Corporation.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A. & Brown, G. K. (2000). Beck depression inventory – fast screen for medical patients. USA: Harcourt Assessment/The Psychological Corporation.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 4, 561–71.Google Scholar
Bolger, N., Davis, A. & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 579–616.Google Scholar
Boyle, G. J. (1986). Higher-order factors in the differential emotions scale (DES-III). Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 305–10.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Bernston, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K.M. & Ito, T.A. (2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. In Lewis, R. & Haviland-Jones, J. M. (Eds.). The Handbook of Emotion (2nd edn.). (pp. 173–191). New York: Guilford
Carroll, J. M., Yik, M. S. M., Russell, J. A. & Barrett, L.F. (1999). On the psychometric principles of affect. Review of General Psychology, 3, 14–22.Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U. & Nesselroade, J. R. (2000). Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 644–55.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S. & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Chaput, L. A., Adams, S. H., Simon, J. A.et al. (2002). Hostility predicts recurrent events among postmenopausal women with coronary heart disease. American Journal of Epidemiology, 156, 1092–99.Google Scholar
Christie, I. C. & Friedman, B. H. (2004). Autonomic specificity of discrete emotion and dimensions of affective space: A multivariate approach. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 51, 143–53.Google Scholar
Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., Turner, R., Alper, C. M. & Skoner, D. P. (2003). Sociability and susceptibility to the common cold. Psychological Science, 14(5), 389–95.Google Scholar
Conner, T., Feldman Barrett, L., Bliss-Moreau, E., Lebo, K. & Kaschub, C. (2003). A practical guide to experience – sampling procedures. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 53–78.Google Scholar
Croyle, R. T. & Rowland, J. H. (2003). Mood disorders and cancer: a national cancer institute perspective. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 191–4.Google Scholar
Davis, M. C., Matthews, K. A. & McGrath, C. (2000). Hostile attitudes predict elevated vascular resistance to interpersonal stress in both men and women. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 17–25.Google Scholar
Derogatis, L. R. (1996). Derogatis affects balance scale (DABS): Preliminary scoring, procedures & administration manual. Baltimore, MD, Clinical Psychometric Research.
Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S. & Covi, L. (1973). SCL–90, an outpatient psychiatric rating scale – preliminary report. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 9, 13–28.Google Scholar
Diamond, E. L. (1982). The role of anger and hostility in essential hypertension and coronary heart disease. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 410–33.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W. & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions, Science, 221, 1208–10.Google Scholar
Eng, P. M., Fitzmaurice, G., Kubzansky, L. D., Rimm, E. B. & Kawachi, I. (2003). Anger expression and risk for stroke and coronary heart disease among male health professionals. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 100–10.Google Scholar
Engebretson, T. O., Matthews, K. A. & Scheier, M. F. (1989). Relations between anger expression and cardiovascular reactivity: Reconciling inconsistent findings through a matching hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 513–21.Google Scholar
Faber, S. D. & Burns, J. W. (1996). Anger management style, degree of expressed anger, and gender influence cardiovascular recovery from interpersonal harassment. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 19, 31–53.Google Scholar
Feldman, L. A. (1993). Distinguishing depression from anxiety in self-report: evidence from confirmatory factor analysis on nonclinical and clinical samples. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 631–8.Google Scholar
Feldman, L. A. (1995a). Variations in the circumplex structure of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 806–17.Google Scholar
Feldman, L. A. (1995b). Valence focus and arousal focus: individual differences in the structure of affective experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 153–66.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions?Review of general psychology: special issue: new directions in research on emotion, 2, 300–19.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (2000). Extracting meaning from past affective experiences: the importance of peaks, ends, and specific emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 14(4), 577–606.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist: Special Issue, 56, 218–26.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. & Levenson, R. W. (1998). Positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 191–220.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L., Mancuso, R. A., Branigan, C. & Tugade, M. M. (2000). The undoing effect of positive emotions. Motivation and Emotion, 24, 237–58.Google Scholar
Gallo, L. C. & Matthews, K. A. (2003). Understanding the association between socioeconomic status and health: do negative emotions play a role?Psychological Bulletin, 129, 10–51.Google Scholar
Gerin, W. & Pickering, T. G. (1995). Association between delayed recovery of blood pressure after acute mental stress and parental history of hypertension. Journal of Hypertension, 13, 603–10.Google Scholar
Giese-Davis, J., Sephton, S. E., Abercrombie, H., Duran, R. E. F. & Spiegel, D. (2004). Repression and high anxiety are associated with aberrant diurnal cortisol rhythms in women with metastatic breast cancer. Health Psychology, 23, 645–50.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J. & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 970–86.Google Scholar
Guadagnoli, E. & Mor, V. (1989). Measuring cancer patients' affect: revision and psychometric properties of the profile of mood states (POMS). Psychological Assessment, 1, 150–54.Google Scholar
Hathaway, S. R. & McKinley, J. C. (1989). Minnesota multiphasic personality inventory-2 (MMPI-2): manual for administration and scoring. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Haynes, S. G., Levine, S., Scotch, N., Feinleb, M. & Kannel, W. B. (1978). The relationship of psychological factors to coronary heart disease in the Framingham study: methods and risk factors. American Journal of Epidemiology, 107, 362–83.Google Scholar
Hedges, S., Jandorf, L. & Stone, A. (1985). Meaning of daily mood assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 428–34.Google Scholar
James, W. (1884/1969). What is an emotion? In William James: Collected essays and reviews (pp. 244–80). New York: Russell and Russell.
Jamieson, J. L. & Lavoie, N. P. (1987). Type A behavior, aerobic power and cardiovascular recovery from a psychosocial stressor. Health Psychology, 6, 361–71.Google Scholar
Jenson, M. R. (1987). Psychobiological factors predicting the course of breast cancer. Journal of Personality, 55, 317–42.Google Scholar
Kappas, A., Hess, U. & Scherer, K. R. (1991). Voice and emotion. In Rime, B. & Feldman, R. (Eds.). Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior (pp. 200–38). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kawachi, I., Sparrow, D., Spiro, A., Vokonas, P. & Weiss, S. T. (1996). A prospective study of anger and coronary heart disease: the normative aging study. Circulation, 94(9), 2090–5.Google Scholar
Kubzansky, L. D. & Arthur, C. M. (2004). Anxiety, heart disease, and mortality. In Anderson, N. (Ed.). Emotional longevity (pp. 55–59). New York: Viking.
Kubzansky, L. D. & Kawachi, I. (2000). Going to the heart of the matter: Do negative emotions cause coronary heart disease?Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 48, 323–37.Google Scholar
Kubzansky, L. D., Berkman, L. F., Glass, T. A. & Seeman, T. E. (1998). Is educational attainment associated with shared determinants of health in the elderly? Findings from the MacArthur studies of Successful aging. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60, 578–85.Google Scholar
Kubzansky, L. D. & Kawachi, I. (2002). Affective states and health. In Berkman, L. F. & Kawachi, I. (Eds.). Social epidemiology (pp. 213–41). New York: Oxford University Press.
Lai, J.Y. & Linden, W. (1992). Gender, anger expression style, and opportunity for anger release determine cardiovascular reaction to and recovery from anger provocation, Psychosomatic Medicine, 54, 297–310.Google Scholar
Lane, R. D. & Schwartz, G. E. (1987). Levels of emotional awareness: A cognitive–developmental theory and its application to psychopathology. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144(2), 133–43.Google Scholar
Lane, R. D., Quinlan, D. M., Schwartz, G. E., Walker, P. A. & Zeitlin, S. B. (1990). The Levels of emotional awareness scale: a cognitive-developmental measure of emotion. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55(1–2), 124–34.Google Scholar
Lang, P. J, Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M. & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: affective, facial, visceral and behavioural reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261–73.Google Scholar
Larsen, R. J. (1992). Neuroticism and selective encoding and recall of symptoms: evidence from a combined concurrent–retrospective study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(3), 480–8.Google Scholar
Larsen, R. J. & Cutler, S. E. (1996). The complexity of individual emotional lives: a within-subject analysis of affect structure. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 15(2), 206–30.Google Scholar
Larsen, R. & Diener, E. (1992). Promises and problems with the circumplex model of emotion. In Clarke, M. S. (Ed.). Emotion (pp. 25–59). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Voluntary facial action generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous system activity. Psychophysiology, 27, 363–84.Google Scholar
MacDougall, J. M., Dembroski, T. M., Dimsdale, J. E. & Hackett, T. P. (1985). Components of Type-A, hostility and anger-in: further relationships to angiographic findings. Health Psychology, 4, 137–52.Google Scholar
Markovitz, J. H., Matthews, K. A., Wing, R. R., Kuller, L. H. & Meilahn, E. N. (1991). Psychological, biological, and health behavior predictors of blood pressure change in middle-aged women. Journal of Hypertension, 9, 399–406.Google Scholar
Marteau, T. M. & Bekker, H. (1992). The development of a six-item short-form of the state scale of the spielberger state–trait anxiety inventory (STAI). British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31(3), 301–6.Google Scholar
Mayer, J. D. & Gaschke, Y. N. (1988). The experience and meta-experience of mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(1), 102–11.Google Scholar
McKenna, M. C., Zevon, M. A., Corn, B. & Rounds, J. (1999). Psychosocial factors and the development of cancer: A meta-analysis, Health Psychology, 18, 520–1.Google Scholar
McNair, D., Lorr, M. & Droppleman, L. F. (1971/1981). EITS manual for the profile of mood states. San Diego, CA: educational and industrial testing service.
Messinger, D. S. (2002). Positive and negative: infant facial expressions and emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 1–6.Google Scholar
Nowlis, V. (1965). Research with the mood adjective checklist. In Tomkins, S. S. & Izard, C. E. (Eds.). Affect, cognition, and personality, (pp. 352–89). New York: Springer.
Parkinson, B., Briner, R. B., Reynolds, S. & Totterdell, P. (1995). Time frames for mood: relations between momentary and generalized ratings of affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(4), 331–9.Google Scholar
Paterniti, M., Zureik, M., Ducimetiere, P., Feve, J. M. & Alperovitch, A. (2001). Sustained anxiety and 4-year progression of carotid atherosclerosis. Atherosclerosis, Thrombosis and Vascular Biology, 21, 136–41.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W. (1982). The psychology of physical symptoms. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Pennix, B. W. J. H., Guralnik, J. M., Pahor, M.et al. (1998). Chronically depressed mood and cancer risk in older persons. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 90, 1888–93.Google Scholar
Quigley, K. S., Barrett, L. F. & Weinstein, S. (2002). Cardiovascular patterns associated with threat and challenge appraisals: a within-subjects analysis. Psychophysiology, 39, 292–302.Google Scholar
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385–401.Google Scholar
Redelmeier, D. A., Katz, J. & Kahneman, D. (2003). Memories of colonoscopy: a randomized trial. Pain, 104(1–2), 187–94.Google Scholar
Reis, H. T. & Gable, S. L. (2000). Event sampling and other methods for studying daily experience. In Reis, H. T. & Judd, C. M. (Eds.). Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 190–222). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Richter, P., Werner, J., Heerlien, A., Kraus, A. & Sauer, H. (1998). On the validity of the beck depression inventory: a review. Psychopathology 31(3), 160–8.Google Scholar
Robinson, M. D. & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6), 934–60.Google Scholar
Rozanski, A., Blumenthal, J. A. & Kaplan, J. (1999). Impact of psychological factors on the pathogenesis of cardiovascular disease and implications for therapy. Circulation, 99, 2192–217.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 805–19.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. & Carroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125(1), 3–30.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., Bachorowski, J. & Fernandez-Dols, J. (2003). Facial and vocal expressions of emotions. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 329–49.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., Weiss, A. & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1989) Affect Grid: a single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(3), 493–502.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R., Wallbott, H. G. & Summerfield, A. B. (1986). Experiencing emotion. A cross-cultural study. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Schneiderman, N. (1987). Psychophysiologic factors in atherogenesis and coronary artery disease. Circulation, 76, 141–7.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. & Cheney, D. L. (2003). Signalers and receivers in animal communication. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 145–73.Google Scholar
Shacham, S. (1983). A shortened version of the Profile of Mood States. Journal of Personality Assessment, 47(3), 305–6.Google Scholar
Singer, J. A. & Salovey, P. (1988). Mood and memory: evaluating the network theory of affect, Clinical Psychology Review, 8(2), 211–51.Google Scholar
Spielberger, C. (1988). State–Trait Anger Expression Inventory, (Res. edn.). Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L. & Lushene, R.D. (1970). Manual for the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Steptoe, A., Cropley, M. & Joekes, K. (2000). Task demands and the pressures of everyday life: associations between cardiovascular reactivity and work blood pressure and heart rate. Health Psychology, 19(1), 46–54.Google Scholar
Stone, A. A. & Shiffman, S. (1994). Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in behavioral medicine. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 16, 199–202.Google Scholar
Stone, A. A., Shiffman, S. S. & DeVries, M. W. (1999). Ecological momentary assessment. In Kahneman, D. & Diener, E. (Eds.). Well-being: the foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 26–39). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Taylor, S. E. & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Reed, G. M., Bower, J. E. & Gruenewald, T. L. (2000). Psychological resources, positive illusions, and health. American Psychologist, 55, 99–109.Google Scholar
Thomas, D. L. & Diener, E. (1990). Memory accuracy in the recall of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(2), 291–7.Google Scholar
Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kelsey, J. & Leitten, C. (1993). Subjective, physiological, and behavioural effects of threat and challenge appraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 65, 248–60.Google Scholar
Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kibler, J. & Ernst, J. M. (1997). Cognitive and physiological antecedents of threat and challenge appraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 63–72.Google Scholar
Tugade, M. M. & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 320–33.Google Scholar
Wallbott, H. G. & Scherer, K. R. (1986). Stress specificities: differential effects of coping style, gender, and type of stressor on autonomic arousal, facial expression, and subjective feeling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 147–56.Google Scholar
Watson, D. & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: the disposition to experience aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin, 96(3), 465–90.Google Scholar
Watson, D. & Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-X: Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule – Expanded form. Cedar Rapids: University of Iowa.
Watson, D. & Clark, L. A. (1997). Measurement and mismeasurement of mood: recurrent and emergent issues. Journal of Personality Assessment, 68(2), 267–96.Google Scholar
Watson, D. (1988). The vicissitudes of mood measurement: effects of varying descriptors, time frames, and response formats on measures of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(1), 128–41.Google Scholar
Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J. & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general activation systems of affect: structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5), 820–38.Google Scholar
Watson, D., Clark, L. & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of a brief measure of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–70.Google Scholar
Watson, D. & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–35.Google Scholar
Yik, M. S. M., Russell, J. A. & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Integrating four structures of current mood into a circumplex: integration and beyond. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 600–19.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, B. & Lubin, B. (1985). Manual of multiple affect adjective check list revised. San Diego: edITS.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×