Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-2lccl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T09:13:45.813Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part VII - General Issues and Theoretical Frameworks

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 May 2018

K. Anders Ericsson
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Robert R. Hoffman
Affiliation:
Florida Institute for Human and Machine Cognition
Aaron Kozbelt
Affiliation:
Brooklyn College, City University of New York
A. Mark Williams
Affiliation:
University of Utah
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369406.Google Scholar
Baker, J., Côté, J., & Abernethy, B. (2003). Learning from the experts: Practice activities of expert decision-makers in sport. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74, 342347.Google Scholar
Baláš, J., Pecha, O., Martin, A. J., & Cochrane, D. (2012). Hand–arm strength and endurance as predictors of climbing performance. European Journal of Sport Science, 12, 1625.Google Scholar
Bédard, J., & Chi, M. T. H. (1993). Expertise in auditing. Auditing, 12 (Suppl.), 125.Google Scholar
Bloom, B. S. (1985a). Generalizations about talent development. In Bloom, B. S. (ed.), Developing talent in young people (pp. 507549). New York: Ballantine Books.Google Scholar
Bloom, B. S. (ed.) (1985b). Developing talent in young people. New York: Ballantine Books.Google Scholar
Book, W. F. (1925). Learning to typewrite. New York: Gregg Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Bryan, W. L., & Harter, N. (1899). Studies on the telegraphic language: The acquisition of a hierarchy of habits. Psychological Review, 6, 345375.Google Scholar
Campitelli, G., & Gobet, F. (2008). The role of practice in chess: A longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 446458.Google Scholar
Charness, N., Tuffiash, M. I., Krampe, R., Reingold, E., & Vasyukova, E. (2005). The role of deliberate practice in chess expertise. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 151165.Google Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In Bower, G. H. (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 16, pp. 158). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Choudhry, N. K., Fletcher, R. H., & Soumerai, S. B. (2005). Systematic review: The relationship between clinical experience and quality of health care. Annals of Internal Medicine, 142, 260273.Google Scholar
Davis, D. (2009). How to help professionals maintain and improve their knowledge and skills: Triangulating best practices in medicine. In Ericsson, K. A. (ed.), The development of professional expertise: Toward measurement of expert performance and design of optimal learning environments (pp. 180202). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
de Bruin, A. B. H., Smits, N., Rikers, R. M. J. P., & Schmidt, H. G. (2008). Deliberate practice predicts performance over time in adolescent chess players and drop-outs: A linear mixed models analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 99, 473497.Google Scholar
de Groot, A. (1978). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague: Mouton. (Original work published 1946)Google Scholar
Deslauriers, L., Schelew, E., & Wieman, C. (2011). Improved learning in a large-enrollment physics class. Science, 332, 862864.Google Scholar
Duffy, L. J., Baluch, B., & Ericsson, K. A. (2004). Dart performance as a function of facets of practice amongst professional and amateur men and women players. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 35, 232245.Google Scholar
Dvorak, A., Merrick, N. L., Dealey, W. L., & Ford, G. C. (1936). Typewriting behavior. New York: American Book Company.Google Scholar
Elo, A. (1978). The rating of chess players, past and present. London: Batsford.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (1996). The acquisition of expert performance: An introduction to some of the issues. In Ericsson, K. A. (ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports and games (pp. 150). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (1998). The scientific study of expert levels of performance: General implications for optimal learning and creativity. High Ability Studies, 9, 75100.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2002). Attaining excellence through deliberate practice: Insights from the study of expert performance. In Ferrari, M. (ed.), The pursuit of excellence in education (pp. 2155). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2003). The search for general abilities and basic capacities: Theoretical implications from the modifiability and complexity of mechanisms mediating expert performance. In Sternberg, R. J. and Grigorenko, E. L. (eds.), Perspectives on the psychology of abilities, competencies, and expertise (pp. 93125). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2006). The influence of experience and deliberate practice on the development of superior expert performance. In Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Hoffman, R. R., & Feltovich, P. J. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 685706). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2007a). An expert-performance perspective on medical expertise: Study superior clinical performance rather than experienced clinicians! Medical Education, 41, 11241130.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2007b). Deliberate practice and the modifiability of body and mind: A reply to the commentaries. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 38, 109123.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2009). Enhancing the development of professional performance: Implications from the study of deliberate practice. In Ericsson, K. A. (ed.), The development of professional expertise: Toward measurement of expert performance and design of optimal learning environments (pp. 405431). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2013). Exceptional memory and expert performance: From Simon and Chase’s theory of expertise to skilled memory and beyond. In Staszewski, J. (ed.), Expertise and skill acquisition (pp. 201228). Abingdon: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2014a). Why expert performance is special and cannot be extrapolated from studies of performance in the general population: A response to criticisms. Intelligence, 45, 81103.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2014b). Creative genius: A view from the expert-performance approach. In Simonton, D. K. (ed.), The Wiley handbook of genius (pp. 321349). Chichester: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2014c). Challenges for the estimation of an upper-bound on relations between accumulated deliberate practice and the associated performance of novices and experts: Comments on Macnamara (sic), Hambrick, and Oswald’s (2014) published meta-analysis. Unpublished manuscript. https://psy.fsu.edu/faculty/ericsson/ericsson.hp.html.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2014d). Supplemental online materials for “A challenge to estimates of an upper-bound on relations between accumulated deliberate practice and the associated performance in domains of expertise: Comments on Macnamara (sic), Hambrick, and Oswald’s (2014) published meta-analysis.” Unpublished manuscript. https://psy.fsu.edu/faculty/ericsson/ericsson.hp.html.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2015). Acquisition and maintenance of medical expertise: A perspective from the expert-performance approach with deliberate practice. Academic Medicine, 90, 14711486.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2016). Summing up hours of any type of practice versus identifying optimal practice activities: Comments on Macnamara, Moreau, and Hambrick (2016). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 351354.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49, 725747.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Hoffman, R. R., & Feltovich, P. J. (eds.) (2006). The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363406.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence on maximal adaptations on task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273305.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Moxley, J. H. (2012). A critique of Howard’s argument for innate limits of chess performance or why we need an account based on acquired skill and deliberate practice. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 649653.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Roring, R. W., & Nandagopal, K. (2007). Giftedness and evidence for reproducibly superior performance: An account based on the expert-performance framework. High Ability Studies, 18, 356.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (1991). Prospects and limits in the empirical study of expertise: An introduction. In Ericsson, K. A. & Smith, J. (eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 138). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Ward, P. (2007). Capturing the naturally occurring superior performance of experts in the laboratory: Toward a science of expert and exceptional performance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 346350.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Whyte, J., & Ward, P. (2007). Expert performance in nursing: Reviewing research on expertise in nursing within the framework of the expert-performance approach. Advances in Nursing Science, 30, E58E71.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Williams, A. M. (2007). Capturing naturally-occurring superior performance in the laboratory: Translational research on expert performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13, 115123.Google Scholar
Feldman, D. H. (1994). Beyond universals in cognitive development (2nd edn.). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Fitts, P., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1869/1979). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: Julian Friedman Publishers.Google Scholar
Gobet, F., Campitelli, G., & Waters, A. J. (2002). Rise of human intelligence: Comments on Howard (1999). Intelligence, 30, 303311.Google Scholar
Grape, C., Sandgren, M., Hansson, L. O., Ericson, M., & Theorell, T. (2003). Does singing promote well-being? An empirical study of professional and amateur singers during a singing lesson. Integrative Physiological & Behavioral Science, 38, 6571.Google Scholar
Groeger, J. A., & Brady, S. J. (2004). Differential effects of formal and informal driver training. Road Safety Research Report No. 42. London: Department of Transport.Google Scholar
Guid, M., & Bratko, I. (2011). Using heuristic-search based engines for estimating human skill at chess. ICGA Journal, 34, 7181.Google Scholar
Hanushek, E. A., & Rivkin, S. J. (2010). Generalizations about using value-added measures of teacher quality. American Economics Review, 100, 267271.Google Scholar
Harris, K. R. (2008). Deliberate practice, mental representations, and skilled performance in bowling. PhD Dissertation, Florida State University. Electronic theses, treatises and dissertations, Diginole Commons (Paper No. 4245).Google Scholar
Hattie, J. A. C. (2009). Visible learning: A synthesis of over 800 meta-analyses relating to achievement. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hayes, J. R. (1981). The complete problem solver. Philadelphia, PA: Franklin Institute Press.Google Scholar
Helgerud, J., Engen, L. G., Wisloff, U., & Hoff, J. (2001). Aerobic endurance training improves soccer performance. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 33, 19251931.Google Scholar
Hoffman, R. R., Ward, P., DiBello, L., Feltovich, P. J., Fiore, S. M., & Andrews, D. (2014). Accelerated expertise: Training for high proficiency in a complex world. Boca Raton, FL: Taylor & Francis/CRC Press.Google Scholar
Howard, R. W. (2013). Practice other than playing games apparently has only a modest role in the development of chess expertise. British Journal of Psychology, 104, 3956.Google Scholar
Huijgen, B. C. H., Elferink-Gemser, M. T., Post, W., & Visscher, C. (2010). Development of dribbling in talented youth soccer players aged 12–19 years: A longitudinal study. Journal of Sports Sciences, 28, 689698.Google Scholar
Hutchinson, C. U., Sachs-Ericsson, N. J., & Ericsson, K. A. (2013). Generalizable aspects of the development of expertise in ballet across countries and cultures: A perspective from the expert-performance approach. High Ability Studies, 24, 2147.Google Scholar
Jääskeläinen, R. (2010). Are all professionals experts? Definitions of expertise and reinterpretation of research evidence in process studies. In Shreve, G. M. & Angelone, E. (eds.), Translation and cognition (pp. 213227). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Krampe, R. T., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 331359.Google Scholar
Laursen, P. B., & Jenkins, D. G. (2002). The scientific basis for high-intensity interval training: Optimizing training programs and maximizing performance in highly trained endurance athletes. Sports Medicine, 32, 5373.Google Scholar
Law, M. P., Côté, J., & Ericsson, K. A. (2007). Characteristics of expert development in rhythmic gymnastics: A retrospective study. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 5, 82103.Google Scholar
Lehmann, A. (1997). The acquisition of expertise in music: Efficiency of deliberate practice as a moderating variable in accounting for sub-expert performance. In Deliège, I. & Sloboda, J. A. (eds.), Perception and cognition of music (pp. 161187). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1993). Sight-reading ability of expert pianists in the context of piano accompanying. Psychomusicology, 12, 182195.Google Scholar
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1998). The historical development of domains of expertise: Performance standards and innovations in music. In Steptoe, A. (ed.), Genius and the mind (pp. 6794). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lövdén, M., Wenger, E., Mårtensson, J., Lindenberger, U., & Bäckman, L. (2013). Structural brain plasticity in adult learning and development. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 22962310.Google Scholar
Macnamara, B. N., Hambrick, D. Z., & Oswald, F. L. (2014). Deliberate practice and performance in music, games, sports, education, and professions: A meta-analysis. Psychological Science, 25, 16081618.Google Scholar
Macnamara, B. N., Moreau, D., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2016). The relationship between deliberate practice and performance in sports: A meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 333350.Google Scholar
McKinney, E. H., & Davis, K. J. (2003). Effects of deliberate practice on crisis decision performance. Human Factors, 45, 436444.Google Scholar
McPherson, G. E., & Renwick, J. M. (2001). A longitudinal study of self-regulation in children’s musical practice. Music Education Research, 3, 169186.Google Scholar
Montgomery, E. C., Kunik, M. F., Wilson, N., Stanley, M. A., & Weiss, B. (2010). Can paraprofessionals deliver cognitive-behavioral therapy to treat anxiety and depressive symptoms? Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 74, 4562.Google Scholar
Ostojic, S. (2013). Prediction of adult height by Tanner–Whitehouse method in young Caucasian male athletes. Quarterly Journal of Medicine, 106, 341345.Google Scholar
Phillips, E., Portus, M., Davids, K., & Renshaw, I. (2012). Performance accuracy and functional variability in elite and developing fast bowlers. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 15, 182188.Google Scholar
Platz, F., Kopiez, R., Lehmann, A. C., & Wolf, A. (2014). The influence of deliberate practice on musical achievement: A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 646.Google Scholar
Raskin, E. (1936). Comparison of scientific and literary ability: A biographical study of eminent scientists and letters of the nineteenth century. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 31, 2035.Google Scholar
Robergs, R. A., & Roberts, S. O. (1997). Exercise physiology: Exercise, performance, and clinical applications. St. Louis, MO: Mosby Year Book.Google Scholar
Saunders, P. U., Pyne, D. B., Telford, R. D., & Hawley, J. A. (2004). Factors affecting running economy in trained distance runners. Sports Medicine, 34, 465485.Google Scholar
Saxon, D., & Barkham, M. (2012). Patterns of therapist variability: Therapist effects and the contribution of patient severity and risk. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 80, 535546.Google Scholar
Schulz, R., & Curnow, C. (1988). Peak performance and age among superathletes: Track and field, swimming, baseball, tennis, and golf. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 43, 113120.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A., & Chase, W. G. (1973). Skill in chess. American Scientist, 61, 394403.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1997). Creative productivity: A predictive and explanatory model of career trajectories and landmarks. Psychological Review, 104, 6689.Google Scholar
Spengler, P. M., & Pilipis, L. A. (2015). A comprehensive meta-reanalysis of the robustness of the experience-accuracy effect in clinical judgment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 62, 360378.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1996). Costs of expertise. In Ericsson, K. A. (ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports and games (pp. 347354). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tracey, T. J., Wampold, B. E., Lichtenberg, J. W., & Goodyear, R. K. (2014). Expertise in psychotherapy: An elusive goal? American Psychologist, 69, 218229.Google Scholar
van der Maas, H. L. J., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2005). A psychometric analysis of chess expertise. American Journal of Psychology, 118, 2960.Google Scholar
Vickers, A. J., Bianco, F. J., Serio, A. M., Eastham, J. A., Schrag, D., Klein, E. A., … & Scardino, P. T. (2007). The surgical learning curve for prostate cancer control after radical prostatectomy. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 99, 11711177.Google Scholar
Waljee, J. F., & Curtin, C. (2014). Quality assessment in hand surgery, Hand Clinics, 30, 329334.Google Scholar
Wei, G. X., Zhang, Y. C., Jiang, T. Z., & Luo, J. (2011). Increased cortical thickness in sports experts: A comparison of diving players with the controls. PloS ONE, 6, e17112.Google Scholar
Williams, D., Kuipers, A., Mukai, C., & Thirsk, R. (2009). Acclimation during space flight: Effects on human physiology. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 180, 13171323.Google Scholar
Young, B. W. (1998). Deliberate practice and the acquisition of expert performance in Canadian middle-distance running. Master’s Dissertation. Masters Abstracts International, University of Ottawa (URI No. 10393/8607).Google Scholar
Young, B. W., & Salmela, J. H. (2010). Examination of practice activities related to the acquisition of elite performance in Canadian middle distance running. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 41, 7390.Google Scholar

References

Ackerman, P. L. (1988). Determinants of individual differences during skill acquisition: Cognitive abilities and information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 288318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, E. C., & Freeman, C. (2000). Communities of practice: Bridging technology and knowledge assessment. Journal of Knowledge Management, 4, 3842.Google Scholar
Amirault, R. J., & Branson, R. K. (2006). Educators and expertise: A brief history of theories and models. In Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Hoffman, R. R., & Feltovich, P. J. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 6986). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369406.Google Scholar
Andrade, H. G., & Perkins, D. N. (1998). Learnable intelligence and intelligent learning. In Sternberg, R. J. & Williams, W. M. (eds.), Intelligence, instruction, and assessment (pp. 6794). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Antonakis, J., Cianciolo, A. T., & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.) (2004a). The nature of leadership. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Antonakis, J., Cianciolo, A. T., & Sternberg, R. J. (2004b). Leadership: Past, present, and future. In Antonakis, J., Cianciolo, A. T., & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.), The nature of leadership (pp. 315). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Argyris, C. (1993). Knowledge for action. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Ark, T., Brooks, L. R., & Eva, K. W. (2006). Giving learners the best of both worlds: Do clinical teachers need to guard against teaching pattern recognition? Academic Medicine, 81, 405409.Google Scholar
Ark, T., Brooks, L. R., & Eva, K. W. (2007). The benefits of flexibility: The pedagogical value of instructions to adopt multifaceted diagnostic reasoning. Medical Education, 41, 281287.Google Scholar
Armstrong, S. J., & Li, Y. (2017). A study of Anglo expatriate managers’ learning, knowledge acquisition, and adjustment in multinational companies in China. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 16, 122.Google Scholar
Armstrong, S. J., & Mahmud, A. (2008). Experiential learning and the acquisition of managerial tacit knowledge. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 7, 189208.Google Scholar
Barrows, H. S., & Tamblyn, R. M. (1980). Problem-based learning: An approach to medical education. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Billett, S. (2006). Constituting the workplace curriculum. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 38, 3148.Google Scholar
Bobrow, D. G., & Whalen, J. (2002). Community knowledge sharing in practice: The Eureka story. Reflections: The SoL Journal, 4, 4759.Google Scholar
Bransford, J., Sherwood, R., Vye, N., & Rieser, J. (1986). Teaching thinking and problem solving: Research foundations. American Psychologist, 41, 10781089.Google Scholar
Braude, H. D. (2009). Clinical intuition versus statistics: Different modes of tacit knowledge in clinical epidemiology and evidence-based medicine. Theoretical Medicine and Bioethics, 30, 181198.Google Scholar
Brehmer, B., & Joyce, C. R. B. (1988). Human judgment: The SJT view. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Brown, J. S., & Duguid, P. (1991). Organizational learning and communities-of-practice: Toward a unified view of working, learning, and innovation. Organization Science, 2, 4057.Google Scholar
Butler, D. L., & Winne, P. H. (1995). Feedback and self-regulated learning: A theoretical synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 65, 245281.Google Scholar
Charlin, B., Roy, L., Brailovsky, C., Goulet, F., & van der Vleuten, C. (2000). The script concordance test: A tool to assess the reflective clinician. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 12, 189195.Google Scholar
Chart, H., Grigorenko, E. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (2008). Identification: The Aurora Battery. In Plucker, J. A. & Callahan, C. M. (eds.), Critical issues and practices in gifted education (pp. 281301). Waco, TX: Prufrock.Google Scholar
Chi, M. T. H. (2006). Two approaches to the study of experts’ characteristics. In Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Hoffman, R. R., & Feltovich, P. J. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 2130). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chisholm, C. D., Whenmouth, L. F., Daly, E. A., Cordell, W. H., Giles, B. K., & Brizendine, E. J. (2004). An evaluation of emergency medicine resident interaction time with faculty in different teaching venues. Academic Emergency Medicine, 11, 149155.Google Scholar
Cianciolo, A. T., Antonakis, J., & Sternberg, R. J. (2004). Practical intelligence and leadership: Using experience as a “mentor.” In Day, D. V., Zaccaro, S., & Halpin, S. (eds.), Leader development for transforming organizations: Growing leaders for tomorrow (pp. 211236). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cianciolo, A. T., & Evans, K. M. (2013). Distributed communities of practice. In Lee, J. & Kirlik, A. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive engineering (pp. 610621). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cianciolo, A. T., Grigorenko, E. L., Jarvin, L., Gil, G., Drebot, M., & Sternberg, R. J. (2006). Practical intelligence and tacit knowledge: Advancements in the measurement of developing expertise. Learning and Individual Differences, 16, 235253.Google Scholar
Cianciolo, A. T., & Sternberg, R. J. (2004). Intelligence: A brief history. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Collins, H. (2013). Three dimensions of expertise. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 12, 253273.Google Scholar
Colonia-Willner, R. (1998). Practical intelligence at work: Relationship between aging and cognitive efficiency among managers in a bank environment. Psychology and Aging, 13, 4557.Google Scholar
Crager, J., & Lemons, D. (2003). Consortium Learning Forum best-practice report: Measuring the impact of knowledge management. Houston, TX: American Productivity & Quality Center.Google Scholar
Dahl, T. (2000). Text summarisation: From human activity to computer program – the problem of tacit knowledge. Journal of Linguistics, 25, 113131.Google Scholar
Dai, D. Y., & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.) (2004). Motivation, emotion, and cognition: Integrative perspectives on intellectual functioning and development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Davidson, J. E., & Sternberg, R. J. (1984). The role of insight in intellectual giftedness. Gifted Child Quarterly, 28, 5864.Google Scholar
Denning, S. (2005). The leader’s guide to storytelling: Mastering the art and discipline of the business narrative. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1933). How we think: A restatement of the relation of reflective thinking to the educative process. Boston: Heath.Google Scholar
Dixon, N. M., Allen, N., Burgess, T., Kilner, P., & Schweitzer, S. (2005). Company command: Unleashing the power of the army profession. West Point, NY: Center for the Advancement of Leader Development and Organizational Learning.Google Scholar
DuBois, D., & Shalin, V. L. (1995). Adapting cognitive methods to real-world objectives: An application to job knowledge testing. In Nichols, P. D., Chipman, S. F., & Brennan, R. L. (eds.), Cognitively diagnostic assessment (pp. 189220). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Duncan, J., Seitz, R. J., Kolodny, J., Bor, D., Herzog, H., Ahmed, A., … & Emslie, H. (2000). A neural basis for general intelligence. Science, 289, 457460.Google Scholar
Durning, S. J., Artino, A., Boulet, J., La Rochelle, J., Van Der Vleuten, C., Arze, B., & Schuwirth, L. (2012). The feasibility, reliability, and validity of a post-encounter form for evaluating clinical reasoning. Medical Teacher, 34, 3037.Google Scholar
Elton, L. (2010). Academic writing and tacit knowledge. Teaching in Higher Education, 15, 151160.Google Scholar
Engel, P. J. H. (2008). Tacit knowledge and visual expertise in medical diagnostic reasoning: Implications for medical education. Medical Teacher, 30, e184e188.Google Scholar
Ennis, R. H. (1987). A taxonomy of critical thinking dispositions and abilities. In Baron, J. B. & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.), Teaching thinking skills: Theory and practice (pp. 926). New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2015). Acquisition and maintenance of medical expertise: A perspective from the expert-performance approach with deliberate practice. Academic Medicine, 90, 14711486.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (2013). The nature and nurture of expertise: A fourth dimension. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 12, 275288.Google Scholar
Fitts, P., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Gardner, H., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Damon, W. (2002). Good work: Where excellence and ethics meet. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Garner, W. R., Hake, H. W., & Eriksen, C. W. (1956). Operationism and the concept of perception. Psychological Review, 63, 149159.Google Scholar
Gherardi, S., Nicolini, D., & Odella, F. (1998). Toward a social understanding of how people learn in organizations. Management Learning, 29, 273297.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J., & Pick, A. D. (2000). An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.Google Scholar
Goldstein, W. M. (2006). Introduction to Brunswikian theory and method. In Kirlik, A. (ed.), Adaptive perspectives on human–technology interaction (pp. 1024). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Greeno, J. (1994). Gibson’s affordances. Psychological Review, 101, 336342.Google Scholar
Grotzer, T. A., & Perkins, D. N. (2000). Teaching intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 492515). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hammond, K. R., Hursch, C. J., & Todd, F. J. (1964). Analyzing the components of clinical inference. Psychological Review, 71, 438456.Google Scholar
Hedlund, J., Forsythe, G. B., Horvath, J. A., Williams, W. M., Snook, S., & Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Identifying and assessing tacit knowledge: Understanding the practical intelligence of military leaders. The Leadership Quarterly, 14, 117140.Google Scholar
Hedlund, J., Horvath, J. A., Forsythe, G. B., Snook, S., Williams, W. M., Bullis, R. C., … & Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Tacit knowledge in military leadership: Evidence of construct validity (Technical Report 1080). Alexandria, VA: US Army Research Institute for the Behavioral & Social Sciences.Google Scholar
Hedlund, J., Wilt, J. M., Nebel, K. R., Ashford, S. J., & Sternberg, R. J. (2006). Assessing practical intelligence in business school admissions: A supplement to the Graduate Management Admissions Test. Learning and Individual Differences, 16, 101127.Google Scholar
Henry, S. (2006). Recognizing tacit knowledge in medical epistemology. Theoretical Medicine and Bioethics, 27, 187213.Google Scholar
Heredia, A., Garcia-Guzman, J., Amescua, A., & Sanchez-Segura, M. I. (2013). Interactive knowledge asset management: Acquiring and disseminating tacit knowledge. Journal of Information Science and Engineering, 29, 133147.Google Scholar
Hill, N. M., & Schneider, W. (2006). Brain changes in the development of expertise: Neuroanatomical and neurophysiological evidence about skill-based adaptations. In Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Hoffman, R. R., & Feltovich, P. J. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 653682). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hoffman, R. R., Shadbolt, N., Burton, A. M., & Klein, G. A. (1995). Eliciting knowledge from experts: A methodological analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 62, 129158.Google Scholar
Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1966). Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallized general intelligences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 57, 253270.Google Scholar
Horn, J. L. & Masunaga, H. (2006). A merging theory of expertise and intelligence. In Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Hoffman, R. R., & Feltovich, P. J. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 587611). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hunter, J. E. (1983). A causal analysis of cognitive ability, job knowledge, job performance, and supervisor ratings. In Landy, F. J., Zedeck, S., & Cleveland, J. (eds.), Performance measurement and theory (pp. 257275). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ilgen, J. S., Eva, K. W., & Regehr, G. (2016). What’s in a label? Is diagnosis the start or the end of clinical reasoning? Journal of General Internal Medicine, 31, 435437.Google Scholar
Insch, G. S., McIntyre, N., & Dawley, D. (2008). Tacit knowledge: A refinement and empirical test of the academic tacit knowledge scale. Journal of Psychology, 142, 561580.Google Scholar
Karelitz, T. M., Jarvin, L., & Sternberg, R. J. (2010). The meaning of wisdom and its development throughout life. In Overton, W. (ed.), Handbook of lifespan human development (pp. 837881). New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kennedy, T. J. T., Regehr, G., Baker, G. R., & Lingard, L. (2008). Point-of-care assessment of medical trainee competence for independent clinical work. Academic Medicine, 83 (Suppl.), S89S92.Google Scholar
Kirlik, A. (1998). The ecological expert: Acting to create information to guide action. Proceedings of the 4th Conference on Human Interaction with Complex Systems (HICS ’98), Los Alamitos, CA.Google Scholar
Kirlik, A. (ed.) (2006). Adaptive perspectives on human–technology interaction. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kirlik, A. (2013). Judgment analysis. In Lee, J. & Kirlik, A. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive engineering (pp. 476489). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kirsh, D., & Maglio, P. (1994). On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic action. Cognitive Science, 18, 513549.Google Scholar
Klamen, D. L. (2015). Getting real: Embracing the conditions of the third-year clerkship and reimagining the curriculum to enable deliberate practice. Academic Medicine, 90, 13141317.Google Scholar
Kolb, D. A. (1984). Experiential learning: Experience as the source of learning and development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Kolb, D. A., & Fry, R. (1975). Towards an applied theory of experiential learning. In Cooper, C. L. (ed.), Theories of group processes (pp. 3358). New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Kulatunga-Moruzi, C., Brooks, L. R., & Norman, G. R. (2011). Teaching posttraining: Influencing diagnostic strategy with instructions at test. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17, 195209.Google Scholar
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Legree, P. J. (1995). Evidence for an oblique social intelligence factor established with a Likert-based testing procedure. Intelligence, 21, 247266.Google Scholar
Legree, P. J., Heffner, T. S., Psotka, J., Martin, D. E., & Medsker, G. J. (2003). Traffic crash involvement: Experiential driving knowledge and stressful contextual antecedents. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 1526.Google Scholar
Lesser, E. L., & Storck, J. (2001). Communities of practice and organizational performance. IBM Systems Journal, 40, 831841.Google Scholar
Lineberry, M., Kreiter, C. D., & Bordage, G. (2013). Threats to validity in the use and interpretation of script concordance test scores. Medical Education, 47, 11751183.Google Scholar
Lipman, M. (1993). Promoting better classroom thinking. Educational Psychology, 13, 291304.Google Scholar
Loevinger, J. (1966). Psychological tests in the conceptual framework of psychology. In Hammond, K. R. (ed.), The psychology of Egon Brunswik (pp. 107148). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Matthew, C. T., & Sternberg, R. J. (2009). Developing experience-based (tacit) knowledge through reflection. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 530540.Google Scholar
McDaniel, M. A., & Nguyen, N. T. (2001). Situational judgment tests: A review of practice and constructs assessed. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 9, 103113.Google Scholar
McDaniel, M. A., & Whetzel, D. L. (2005). Situational judgment test research: Informing the debate on practical intelligence theory. Intelligence, 33, 515525.Google Scholar
Motowidlo, S. J., Dunnette, M. D., & Carter, G. W. (1990). An alternative selection procedure: The low-fidelity simulation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 640647.Google Scholar
Nakano, D., Jr. Muniz, J., & Jr. Batista, E. D. (2013). Engaging environments: Tacit knowledge sharing on the shop floor. Journal of Knowledge Management, 17, 290306.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1976). General, academic, and artificial intelligence. In Resnick, L. B. (ed.), The nature of intelligence (pp. 135144). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nestor-Baker, N. S., & Hoy, W. K. (2001). Tacit knowledge of school superintendents: Its nature, meaning, and content. Educational Administration Quarterly, 37, 86129.Google Scholar
Nickerson, R. S. (1989). On improving thinking through instruction. Review of Research in Education, 15, 357.Google Scholar
Nonaka, I., & Takeuchi, H. (1995). The knowledge-creating company: How Japanese companies create the dynamics of innovation. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Osman, N. Y., Walling, J. L., Mitchell, V. G., & Alexander, E. K. (2015). Length of attending-student and resident-student interactions in the inpatient medicine clerkship. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 27, 130137.Google Scholar
Oxford English Dictionary (2015). www.oed.com/.Google Scholar
Panahi, S., Watson, J., & Partridge, H. (2012). Social media and tacit knowledge sharing: Developing a conceptual model. World Academy of Science, Engineering and Technology, 64, 10951102.Google Scholar
Papierno, P., Ceci, S., Makel, M., & Williams, W. (2005). The nature and nurture of talent: A bioecological perspective on the ontogeny of exceptional abilities. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 28, 312332.Google Scholar
Park, Y. S., Lineberry, M., Hyderi, A., Bordage, G., Riddle, J., & Yudkowsky, R. (2013). Validity evidence for a patient note scoring rubric based on the new patient note format of the United States Medical Licensing Examination. Academic Medicine, 88, 15521557.Google Scholar
Paul, R. W. (1987). Dialogical thinking: Critical thought essential to the acquisition of rational knowledge and passions. In Baron, J. B. & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.), Teaching thinking skills: Theory and practice (pp. 127148). New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Polanyi, M. (1958). Personal knowledge: Towards a post-critical philosophy. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Polanyi, M. (1966). The tacit dimension. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Prober, C. G., Kolars, J. C., First, L. R., & Melnick, D. E. (2016). A plea to reassess the role of the United States Medical Licensing Examination Step 1 scores in residency selection. Academic Medicine, 91, 1215.Google Scholar
Pusic, M., Pecaric, M., & Boutis, K. (2011). How much practice is enough? Using learning curves to assess the deliberate practice of radiograph interpretation. Academic Medicine, 86, 731736.Google Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 219235.Google Scholar
Reber, A. S., & Lewis, S. (1977). Implicit learning: An analysis of the form and structure of a body of tacit knowledge. Cognition, 5, 333361.Google Scholar
Ree, M. J., Caretta, T. R., & Teachout, M. S. (1995). Role of ability and prior job knowledge in complex training performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 721730.Google Scholar
Revill, J., & Jefferson, C. (2014). Tacit knowledge and the biological weapons regime. Science and Public Policy, 41, 597610.Google Scholar
Rizan, C., Elsey, C., Lemon, T., Grant, A., & Monrouxe, L. V. (2014). Feedback in action within bedside teaching encounters: A video ethnographic study. Medical Education, 48, 902920.Google Scholar
Ryan, S., & O’Connor, R. V. (2009). Development of a team measure for tacit knowledge in software development teams. Journal of Systems and Software, 82, 229240.Google Scholar
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1993). Tacit knowledge, practical intelligence, general mental ability, and job knowledge. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 89.Google Scholar
Schön, D. A. (1983). The reflective practitioner: How professionals think in action. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Schwartz, D. L., & Bransford, J. D. (1998). A time for telling. Cognition and Instruction, 16, 475522.Google Scholar
Shanteau, J. (1992). Competence in experts: The role of task characteristics. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 53, 252266.Google Scholar
Somech, A., & Bogler, R. (1999). Tacit knowledge in academia: Its effects on student learning and achievement. Journal of Psychology, 133, 605616.Google Scholar
Spengler, P. M., & Pilipis, L. A. (2015). A comprehensive meta-reanalysis of the robustness of the experience-accuracy effect in clinical judgment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 62, 360378.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1987a). Liking versus loving: A comparative evaluation of theories. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 331345.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1987b). Most vocabulary is learned from context. In McKeown, M. G. & Curtis, M. E. (eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 89105). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1988). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1994). Cognitive conceptions of expertise. International Journal of Expert Systems, 7, 112.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Successful intelligence. New York: Plume Books.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1998a). Abilities are forms of developing expertise. Educational Researcher, 27, 1120.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1998b). Cupid’s arrow: The course of love through time. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (ed.) (2002). Why smart people can be so stupid. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2003a). WICS: A model for leadership in organizations. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 2, 386401.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2003b). Wisdom, intelligence, and creativity synthesized. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2009a). Preparing for college and graduate school: What do tests like the SAT, ACT, and GRE tell us? In Good, T. L. (ed.), 21st century education: A reference handbook (Vol. 2, pp. 473480). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2009b). Wisdom. In Lopez, S. J. (ed.) Encyclopedia of positive psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 10341037) New York: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2010a). College admissions for the 21st century. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2010b). Teaching for ethical reasoning in liberal education. Liberal Education, 96, 3237.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2012a). A model for ethical reasoning. Review of General Psychology, 16, 319326.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2012b). Teaching for ethical reasoning. International Journal of Educational Psychology, 1, 3550.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2013a). Personal wisdom in the balance. In Ferrari, M. & Weststrate, N. (eds.), The scientific study of personal wisdom: From contemplative traditions to neuroscience (pp. 5374). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2013b). Teaching for wisdom. In David, S., Boniwell, I., & Ayers, A. C. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of happiness (pp. 631643). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2015). Ethical impotence. Journal of College and Character, 16, 180185.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2016). What universities can be. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Forsythe, G. B., Hedlund, J., Horvath, J. A., Wagner, R. K., Williams, W. M., … & Grigorenko, E. L. (2000). Practical intelligence in everyday life. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Horvath, J. A. (eds.) (1999). Tacit knowledge in professional practice: Researcher and practitioner perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Jarvin, L., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2009). Teaching for wisdom, intelligence, creativity, and success. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Rainbow Project Collaborators (2005). Augmenting the SAT through assessments of analytical, practical, and creative skills. In Camara, W. J. & Kimmel, E. W. (eds.), Choosing students: Higher education tools for the 21st century (pp. 159176). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Wagner, R. K., & Okagaki, L. (1993). Practical intelligence: The nature and role of tacit knowledge in work and at school. In Puckett, J. M. & Reese, H. W. (eds.), Mechanisms of everyday cognition (pp. 205223). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Steurer, J., Held, U., & Miettinen, O. S. (2013). Diagnostic probability function for acute coronary heart disease garnered from experts’ tacit knowledge. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 66, 12891295.Google Scholar
Tan, H., & Libby, R. (1997). Tacit managerial versus technical knowledge as determinants of audit expertise in the field. Journal of Accounting Research, 35, 97113.Google Scholar
Tee, M. Y., & Karney, D. (2010). Sharing and cultivating tacit knowledge in an online learning environment. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 5, 385413.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C., & Brunswik, E. (1935). The organism and the causal texture of the environment. Psychological Review, 42, 4377.Google Scholar
Venkitachalam, K., & Busch, P. (2012). Tacit knowledge: Review and possible research directions. Journal of Knowledge Management, 16, 357372.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K. (1987). Tacit knowledge in everyday intelligent behavior. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 52, 12361247.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Practical intelligence in real-world pursuits: The role of tacit knowledge. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 49, 436458.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Tacit Knowledge Inventory for Sales: Written. Unpublished test.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K, & Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Street smarts. In Clark, K. E. & Clark, M. B. (eds.), Measures of Leadership (pp. 493504). West Orange, NJ: Leadership Library of America.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Sternberg, R. J. (1991). Tacit Knowledge Inventory for managers. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wenger, E. (1998). Communities of practice: Learning, meaning, and identity. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wenger, E. (2000). Communities of practice and social learning systems. Organization, 7, 225246.Google Scholar
Wenger, E., McDermott, R., & Snyder, W. M. (2002). Cultivating communities of practice. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.Google Scholar
Wenger, E., & Snyder, W. M. (2000). Communities of practice: The organizational frontier. Harvard Business Review (January–February), 139145.Google Scholar
Wenger, E., White, N., & Smith, J. D. (2009). Digital habitats: Stewarding technology for communities. Portland, OR: CPSquare.Google Scholar
Wigton, R. S. (1996). Social judgment theory and medical judgment. Thinking & Reasoning, 2, 175190.Google Scholar
Williams, R. G., & Dunnington, G. L. (2006). Assessing the ACGME competencies with methods that improve the quality of evidence and adequacy of sampling. ACGME Bulletin (April), 3842.Google Scholar
Williams, R. G., & Klamen, D. L. (2012). Examining the diagnostic justification abilities of fourth-year medical students. Academic Medicine, 87, 10081014.Google Scholar
Williams, W., Blythe, T., White, N., Li, J., Gardner, H., & Sternberg, R. J. (2002). Practical intelligence for school: Developing metacognitive sources of achievement in adolescence. Developmental Review, 22, 162210.Google Scholar
Williams, W., Blythe, T., White, N., Li, J., Sternberg, R. J., & Gardner, H. (1996). Practical intelligence for school handbook. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Wilson, T. D., & Dunn, E. W. (2004). Self-knowledge: Its limits, value, and potential for improvement. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 493518.Google Scholar
Wittmann, W. W., & Süß, H. M. (1999). Investigating the paths between working memory, intelligence, knowledge, and complex problem-solving performances via Brunswik symmetry. In Ackerman, P. L., Kyllonen, P. C., & Roberts, R. D. (eds.), Learning and individual differences: Process, trait, and content determinants (pp. 77108). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Zemel, A., & Koschmann, T. (2014). ‘Put your fingers right in here’: Learnability and instructed experience. Discourse Studies, 16, 163183.Google Scholar
Zemel, A., Koschmann, T., & LeBaron, C. (2011). Pursuing a response: Prodding recognition and expertise within a surgical team. In Streeck, J., Goodwin, C., & LeBaron, C. (eds.), Embodied interaction: Language and body in the material world (pp. 227242). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, B. J. (2006). Development and adaptation of expertise: The role of self-regulatory processes and beliefs. In Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Hoffman, R. R., & Feltovich, P. J. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 705722). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

References

Amadieu, F., Tricot, A., & Mariné, C. (2009a). Prior knowledge in learning from a non-linear electronic document: Disorientation and coherence of the reading sequences. Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 381388.Google Scholar
Amadieu, F., van Gog, T., Paas, F., Tricot, A., & Mariné, C. (2009b). Effects of prior knowledge and concept-map structure on disorientation, cognitive load, and learning. Learning and Instruction, 19, 376386.Google Scholar
Ausubel, D. P. (1968). Educational psychology: A cognitive view. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Beilock, S. L., Bertenthal, B. I., McCoy, A. M., & Carr, T. H. (2004). Haste does not always make waste: Expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 373379.Google Scholar
Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 701725.Google Scholar
Beilock, S. L., Jellison, W. A., Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., & Carr, T. H. (2006). On the causal mechanisms of stereotype threat: Can skills that don’t rely heavily on working memory still be threatened? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 10591071.Google Scholar
Blayney, P., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2010). Interactions between the isolated–interactive elements effect and levels of learner expertise: Experimental evidence from an accountancy class. Instructional Science, 38, 277287.Google Scholar
Bokosmaty, S., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2015). Learning geometry problem solving by studying worked-examples: Effects of learner guidance and expertise. American Educational Research Journal, 52, 307333.Google Scholar
Boshuizen, H. P. A., & Schmidt, H. G. (1992). On the role of biomedical knowledge in clinical reasoning by experts, intermediates and novices. Cognitive Science, 16, 153184.Google Scholar
Brunstein, A., Betts, S., & Anderson, J. R. (2009). Practice enables successful learning under minimal guidance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 790802.Google Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 5581.Google Scholar
Clarke, T., Ayres, P., & Sweller, J. (2005). The impact of sequencing and prior knowledge on learning mathematics through spreadsheet applications. Educational Technology Research and Development, 53, 1524.Google Scholar
Cronbach, L. (1967). How can instruction be adapted to individual differences? In Gagné, R. (ed.), Learning and individual differences (pp. 2339), Columbus, OH: Merrill.Google Scholar
Cronbach, L., & Snow, R. (1977). Aptitudes and instructional methods: A handbook for research on interactions. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
de Groot, A. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague: Mouton. (Original work published 1946)Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49, 725747.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211245.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363406.Google Scholar
Fyfe, E. R., Rittle-Johnson, B., & DeCaro, M. S. (2012). The effects of feedback during exploratory mathematics problem solving: Prior knowledge matters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 10941108.Google Scholar
Geary, D. (2005). The origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Geary, D. (2007). Educating the evolved mind: Conceptual foundations for an evolutionary educational psychology. In Carlson, J. S. & Levin, J. R. (eds.), Psychological perspectives on contemporary educational issues (pp. 199). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Geary, D. (2008). An evolutionarily informed education science. Educational Psychologist, 43, 179195.Google Scholar
Geary, D. (2012). Evolutionary educational psychology. In Harris, K., Graham, S., & Urdan, T. (eds.), APA educational psychology handbook (Vol. 1, pp. 597621). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Homer, B. D., & Plass, J. L. (2010). Expertise reversal for iconic representations in science visualizations. Instructional Science, 38, 259276.Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S. (2006). Assessment of learners’ organised knowledge structures in adaptive learning environments. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 333342.Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 509539.Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S. (2014). The expertise reversal principle in multimedia learning. In Mayer, R. (ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (2nd edn.) (pp. 576597). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect. Educational Psychologist, 38, 2331.Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1998). Levels of expertise and instructional design. Human Factors, 40, 117.Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2000). Incorporating learner experience into the design of multimedia instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 126136.Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2001a). Learner experience and efficiency of instructional guidance. Educational Psychology, 21, 523.Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Tuovinen, J. & Sweller, J. (2001b). When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 579588.Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S., & Renkl, A. (2010). Expertise reversal effect and its instructional implications. Instructional Science, 38, 209215.Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S., Rikers, R., & Paas, F. (2012). Educational implications of expertise reversal effects in learning and performance of complex cognitive and sensorimotor skills. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 313337.Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2004). Measuring knowledge to optimize cognitive load factors during instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 558568.Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2005). Rapid dynamic assessment of expertise to improve the efficiency of adaptive e-learning. Educational Technology Research and Development, 53, 8393.Google Scholar
Koedinger, K., & Aleven, V. (2007). Exploring the assistance dilemma in experiments with cognitive tutors. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 239264.Google Scholar
Krause, U.-M., Stark, R., & Mandl, H. (2009). The effects of cooperative learning and feedback on e-learning in statistics. Learning and Instruction, 19, 158170.Google Scholar
Kyun, S. A., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2013). The effect of worked examples when learning to write essays in English literature. Journal of Experimental Education, 81, 385408.Google Scholar
Lee, H., Plass, J. L., & Homer, B. D. (2006). Optimizing cognitive load for learning from computer-based science simulations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 902913.Google Scholar
Leppink, J., Broers, N. J., Imbos, T., van der Vleuten, C. P. M., & Berger, M. P. F. (2012). Self-explanation in the domain of statistics: An expertise reversal effect. Higher Education, 63, 771785.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 8197.Google Scholar
Nihalani, P. K., Mayrath, M., & Robinson, D. H. (2011). When feedback harms and collaboration helps in computer simulation environments: An expertise reversal effect. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 776785.Google Scholar
Nückles, M., Hübner, S., Dümer, S., & Renkl, A. (2010). Expertise reversal effects in writing-to-learn. Instructional Science, 38, 237258.Google Scholar
Oksa, A., Kalyuga, S., & Chandler, P. (2010). Expertise reversal effect in using explanatory notes for readers of Shakespearean text. Instructional Science, 38, 217236.Google Scholar
Paas, F., Tuovinen, J. E., Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Darabi, A. A. (2005). A motivational perspective on the relation between mental effort and performance. Educational Technology Research and Development, 53, 2534.Google Scholar
Pachman, M., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2013). Levels of knowledge and deliberate practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 1, 108119.Google Scholar
Pachman, M., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2014). Effectiveness of combining worked examples and deliberate practice for high school geometry. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 685692.Google Scholar
Patel, V. L., & Arocha, J. F. (1993). Construction-integration theory and clinical reasoning. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Atlanta, GA, April.Google Scholar
Patel, V. L., Arocha, J. F., & Kaufman, D. R. (1994). Diagnostic reasoning and medical expertise. In Medin, D. (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 30, pp. 187251). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Pollock, E., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2002). Assimilating complex information. Learning and Instruction, 12, 6186.Google Scholar
Reisslein, J., Atkinson, R. K., Seeling, P., & Reisslein, M. (2006). Encountering the expertise reversal effect with a computer-based environment on electrical circuit analysis. Learning and Instruction, 16, 92103.Google Scholar
Renkl, A. (1997). Learning from worked-out examples: A study on individual differences. Cognitive Science, 21, 129.Google Scholar
Renkl, A. (2014a). Toward an instructionally oriented theory of example-based learning. Cognitive Science, 38, 137.Google Scholar
Renkl, A. (2014b). The worked examples principle in multimedia learning. In Mayer, R. E. (ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (2nd edn.) (pp. 391412). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rey, G. D., & Buchwald, F. (2011). The expertise reversal effect: Cognitive load and motivational explanations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17, 3348.Google Scholar
Rey, G. D., & Fischer, A. (2012). The expertise reversal effect concerning instructional explanations. Instructional Science, 40, 193211.Google Scholar
Rikers, R. M. J. P., Schmidt, H. G., & Boshuizen, H. P. A. (2000). Knowledge encapsulation and the intermediate effect. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 150166.Google Scholar
Roelle, J., & Berthold, K. (2013). The expertise reversal effect in prompting focused processing of instructional explanations, Instructional Science, 41, 635656.Google Scholar
Salden, R. J. C. M., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2006). Personalised adaptive task selection in air traffic control: Effects on training efficiency and transfer. Learning and Instruction, 16, 350362.Google Scholar
Schmidt, H. G., & Boshuizen, H. P. A. (1992). Encapsulation of biomedical knowledge. In Evans, D. A. & Patel, V. L. (eds.), Advanced models of cognition for medical training and practice (pp. 265282). New York: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Schmidt, H. G., & Boshuizen, H. P. A. (1993). On the origin of intermediate effects in clinical case recall. Memory & Cognition, 21, 338351.Google Scholar
Schmidt, H. G., Boshuizen, H. P. A., & Hobus, P. P. M. (1988). Transitory stages in the development of medical expertise: The “intermediate effect” in clinical case representation studies. In Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society Meeting (pp. 139145). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schnotz, W. (2010). Reanalyzing the expertise reversal effect. Instructional Science, 38, 315323.Google Scholar
Simon, H., & Gilmartin, K. (1973). A simulation of memory for chess positions. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 2946.Google Scholar
Spanjers, I. A. E., Wouters, P., Van Gog, T., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2011). An expertise reversal effect of segmentation in learning from animated worked-out examples. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 4652.Google Scholar
Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12, 257285.Google Scholar
Sweller, J. (2009). Cognitive bases of human creativity. Educational Psychology Review, 21, 1119.Google Scholar
Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Sweller, J., & Sweller, S. (2006). Natural information processing systems. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 434458.Google Scholar
Tobias, S. (1976). Achievement treatment interactions. Review of Educational Research, 46, 6174.Google Scholar
Tobias, S. (1989). Another look at research on the adaptation of instruction to student characteristics. Educational Psychologist, 24, 213227.Google Scholar
Tricot, A., & Sweller, J. (2014). Domain-specific knowledge and why teaching generic skills does not work. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 265283.Google Scholar
Van de Wiel, M. W. J., Boshuizen, H. P. A., & Schmidt, H. G. (2000). Knowledge restructuring in expertise development: Evidence from pathophysiological representations of clinical cases by students and physicians. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12, 323355.Google Scholar
Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2008). Effects of studying sequences of process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer efficiency. Learning and Instruction, 18, 211222.Google Scholar
Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1990). Strategies for programming instruction in high school: Program completion vs. program generation. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 6, 265287.Google Scholar
Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Kirschner, P. A. (2013). Ten steps to complex learning (2nd revised edn.). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Sluijsmans, D. A. (2009). Toward a synthesis of cognitive load theory, four-component instructional design, and self-directed learning. Educational Psychology Review, 21, 5566.Google Scholar
Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions. Educational Psychology Review, 17, 147177.Google Scholar
Vink, S. C., Van Tartwijk, J., Verloop, N., Gosselink, M. J., Driessen, E., & Bolk, J. H. (2016). The articulation of integration of clinical and basic sciences in concept maps: Differences between experienced and resident groups. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 21, 643657.Google Scholar
Whitehead, A. E., Taylor, J. A., & Polman, R. C. (2016). Evidence for skill level differences in the thought processes of golfers during high and low pressure situations. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01974.Google Scholar
Yeung, A. S., Jin, P., & Sweller, J. (1998). Cognitive load and learner expertise: Split attention and redundancy effects in reading with explanatory notes. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23, 121.Google Scholar
Youssef-Shalala, A., Ayres, P., Schubert, C. & Sweller, J. (2014). Using a general problem-solving strategy to promote transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 20, 215231.Google Scholar

References

Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. J. (2013). Revisiting Mednick’s model on creativity-related differences in associative hierarchies: Evidence for a common path to uncommon thought. Journal of Creative Behavior, 47, 273289.Google Scholar
Bower, G. H., & Hilgard, E. R. (1981). Theories of learning. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Buderi, R. (1996). The invention that changed the world. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Carson, S. (2014). Cognitive disinhibition, creativity, and psychopathology. In Simonton, D. (ed.), The Wiley handbook of genius (pp. 198221). Chichester: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Chan, J., Dow, S. P., & Schunn, C. (2015). Do the best design ideas (really) come from conceptually distant sources of inspiration? Design Studies, 36, 3158.Google Scholar
Chan, J., & Schunn, C. (2015). The impact of analogies on creative concept generation: Lessons from an in vivo study in engineering design. Cognitive Science, 39, 126155.Google Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 5581.Google Scholar
Chein, J. M., & Weisberg, R. W. (2014). Working memory and insight in verbal problems: Analysis of compound remote associates. Memory & Cognition, 42, 6783.Google Scholar
Chein, J. M., Weisberg, R. W., Streeter, N. L., & Kwok, S. (2010). Working memory and insight in the 9-dot problem. Memory & Cognition, 38, 883892.Google Scholar
Cranford, E. A., & Moss, J. (2012). Is insight always the same? A protocol analysis of insight in compound remote associate problems. Journal of Problem Solving, 4, 128153.Google Scholar
de Groot, A. (1978). Thought and choice in chess (2nd edn.). The Hague: Mouton (Original work published 1946)Google Scholar
Dunbar, K. (1995). How scientists really reason: Scientific reasoning in real-world laboratories. In Sternberg, R. J. & Davidson, J. (eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 365395). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2014). Creative genius: A view from the expert-performance approach. In Simonton, D. (ed.), The Wiley handbook of genius (pp. 321349). Chichester: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Hoffman, R. R., & Feltovich, P. J. (eds.) (2006). The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Pool, R. (2016). Peak: Secrets from the new science of expertise. New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1963). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Fleck, J. I., & Weisberg, R. W. (2004). The use of verbal protocols as data: An analysis of insight in the candle problem. Memory & Cognition, 32, 9901006.Google Scholar
Fleck, J. I., & Weisberg, R. W. (2013). Insight versus analysis: Evidence for diverse methods in problem solving. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 436463.Google Scholar
Friedel, R., Israel, P., & Finn, B. S. (1986). Edison’s electric light: Biography of an invention. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Friedman, R. S., Fishbach, A., Förster, J., & Werth, L. (2003). Attentional priming effects on creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 15, 277286.Google Scholar
Hayes, J. R. (1989). Cognitive processes in creativity. In Glover, J. A., Ronning, R. R., & Reynolds, C. R. (eds.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 135145). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Israel, P. (1998). Edison: A life of invention. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Jarosz, A. F., Colflesh, G. J. H., & Wiley, J. (2012). Uncorking the muse: Alcohol intoxication facilitates creative problem solving. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 487493.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1925). The mentality of apes. New York: Liveright.Google Scholar
Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2015). The eureka factor: Aha moments, creative insight, and the brain. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Kozbelt, A. (2005). Factors affecting aesthetic success and improvement in creativity: A case study of the musical genres of Mozart. Psychology of Music, 33, 235255.Google Scholar
Laurenza, D. (2006). Written text for Leonardo’s machines: Da Vinci’s inventions revealed, ed. Taddei, M. & Zanon, E.. Cincinnati, OH: David & Charles.Google Scholar
Martindale, C. (1999). Biological bases of creativity. In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 137152). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220232.Google Scholar
Ohlsson, S. (2011). Deep learning: How the mind overrides experience. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Perkins, D. N. (2000). The eureka effect: The art and logic of breakthrough thinking. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Poincaré, H. (1908). Mathematical creation. Reprinted in Ghiselin, B. (ed.), The creative process (pp. 3956). Studio City, CA: Transformation Book Circle, 2005.Google Scholar
Richter, I. (ed.) (1952). The notebooks of Leonardo da Vinci. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Seifert, C. M., Meyer, D. E., Davidson, N., Patalano, A. L., & Yaniv, I. (1995). Demystification of cognitive insight: Opportunistic assimilation and the prepared-mind perspective. In Sternberg, R. J. & Davidson, J. E. (eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 65124). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Toker, F. (2003). Fallingwater rising: Frank Lloyd Wright, E. J. Kaufmann, and America’s most extraordinary house. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Wallas, G. (1926/2014). The art of thought. Tunbridge Wells: Solis PressGoogle Scholar
Wegbreit, E., Suzuki, S., Grabowecky, M., Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2012). Visual attention modulates insight versus analytic solving of verbal problems. Journal of Problem Solving, 4, 94115.Google Scholar
Weisberg, R. W. (1986). Creativity: Genius and other myths. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Weisberg, R. W. (2006a). Modes of expertise in creative thinking: Evidence from case studies. In Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Hoffman, R. R., & Feltovich, P. J. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 761787). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Weisberg, R. W. (2006b). Creativity: Understanding innovation in problem solving, science, invention, and the arts. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Weisberg, R. W. (2009). On ‘‘out-of-the-box’’ thinking in creativity. In Markman, A. & Wood, K. (eds.), Tools for innovation (pp. 2347). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Weisberg, R. W. (2011). Frank Lloyd Wright’s Fallingwater: A case study of inside-the-box creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 296312.Google Scholar
Weisberg, R. W. (2013). On the “demystification” of insight: A critique of neuroimaging studies of insight. Creativity Research Journal, 25, 114.Google Scholar
Weisberg, R. W. (2015). Toward an integrated theory of insight in problem solving. Thinking and Reasoning, 21, 539.Google Scholar
Weisberg, R. W. (Forthcoming). Problem solving. In Ball, L. J. & Thompson, V. A. (eds.), International handbook of thinking and reasoning. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Wertheimer, M. (1982). Productive thinking (enlarged edn.). University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wiley, J., & Jarosz, A. F. (2012). Working memory capacity, attentional focus, and problem solving. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 258262.Google Scholar

References

Ackerman, P. L. (1988). Determinants of individual differences during skill acquisition: Cognitive abilities and information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 288318.Google Scholar
Amer, T., Kalender, B., Hasher, L., Trehub, S. E., & Wong, Y. (2013). Do older professional musicians have cognitive advantages? PloS ONE, 8. DOI: ARTN e71630 DOI 10.1371/journal.pone.0071630.Google Scholar
Bahrick, H. P., & Hall, L. K. (1991). Lifetime maintenance of high school mathematics content. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 2033.Google Scholar
Bailey, S. K. T., & Sims, V. K. (2014). Self-reported craft expertise predicts maintenance of spatial ability in old age. Cognitive Processing, 15(2), 227231.Google Scholar
Bengtsson, S. L., Nagy, Z., Skare, S., Forsman, L., Forssberg, H., & Ullén, F. (2005). Extensive piano practicing has regionally specific effects on white matter development. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 11481150.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & DePape, A. M. (2009). Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 565574.Google Scholar
Bilalić, M., Kiesel, A., Pohl, C., Erb, M., & Grodd, W. (2011). It takes two: Skilled recognition of objects engages lateral areas in both hemispheres. PloS ONE, 6. DOI: ARTN e16202 10.1371/journal.pone.0016202.Google Scholar
Bilalić, M., Langner, R., Erb, M., & Grodd, W. (2010). Mechanisms and neural basis of object and pattern recognition: A study with chess experts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 728742.Google Scholar
Boisgontier, M. P., Beets, I. A., Duysens, J., Nieuwboer, A., Krampe, R. T., & Swinnen, S. P. (2013). Age-related differences in attentional cost associated with postural dual tasks: Increased recruitment of generic cognitive resources in older adults. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 18241837.Google Scholar
Bosman, E. A. (1993). Age-related differences in the motoric aspects of transcription typing skill. Psychology and Aging, 8, 87102.Google Scholar
Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Locantore, J. K., & McIntosh, A. R. (2002). Aging gracefully: Compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. NeuroImage, 17, 13941402.Google Scholar
Callahan, J. S., Kiker, D. S., & Cross, T. (2003). Does method matter? A meta-analysis of the effects of training method on older learner training performance. Journal of Management, 29, 663680.Google Scholar
Campbell, K. L., Grady, C. L., Ng, C., & Hasher, L. (2012). Age differences in the frontoparietal cognitive control network: Implications for distractibility. Neuropsychologia, 50, 22122223.Google Scholar
Campitelli, G., & Gobet, F. (2011). Deliberate practice: Necessary but not sufficient. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 280285.Google Scholar
Castel, A. D. (2007). Aging and memory for numerical information: The role of specificity and expertise in associative memory. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 62, P194–P196.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth and action. New York: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Cavallini, E., Cornoldi, C., & Vecchi, T. (2009). The effects of age and professional expertise on working memory performance. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 382395.Google Scholar
Cerella, J. (1990). Aging and information processing rates in the elderly. In Birren, J. E. & Schaie, K. W. (eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (3rd edn.) (pp. 201221). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Charness, N. (1981a). Aging and skilled problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 2138.Google Scholar
Charness, N. (1981b). Search in chess: Age and skill differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 467476.Google Scholar
Charness, N. (1989). Expertise in chess and bridge. In Klahr, D. & Kotovsky, K. (eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 183208). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Charness, N., & Bosman, E. A. (1990). Expertise and aging: Life in the lab. In Hess, T. M. (ed.), Aging and cognition: Knowledge organization and utilization (pp. 343385). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
Charness, N., Kelley, C. L., Bosman, E. A., & Mottram, M. (2001). Word-processing training and retraining: Effects of adult age, experience, and interface. Psychology and Aging, 16, 110127.Google Scholar
Charness, N., Krampe, R. T., & Mayr, U. (1996). The role of practice and coaching in entrepreneurial skill domains: An international comparison of life-span chess skill acquisition. In Ericsson, K. A. (ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports and games (pp. 5180). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Charness, N., Tuffiash, M., Krampe, R., Reingold, E., & Vasyukova, E. (2005). The role of deliberate practice in chess expertise. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 151165.Google Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1981). Skilled memory. In Anderson, J. R. (ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 141189). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In Bower, G. H. (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 16, pp. 158). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Colonia-Willner, R. (1998). Practical intelligence at work: Relationship between aging and cognitive efficiency among managers in a bank environment. Psychology and Aging, 13, 4557.Google Scholar
Delon-Martin, C., Plailly, J., Fonlupt, P., Veyrac, A., & Royet, J. P. (2013). Perfumers’ expertise induces structural reorganization in olfactory brain regions. NeuroImage, 68, 5562.Google Scholar
Detterman, D. K., & Daniel, M. H. (1989). Correlations of mental tests with each other and with cognitive variables are highest for low IQ groups. Intelligence, 13, 349359.Google Scholar
Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1986). Toward life-span research on the functions and pragmatics of intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. & Wagner, R. K. (eds.), Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of competence in the everyday world (pp. 203235). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., & Taub, E. (1995). Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science, 270, 305307.Google Scholar
Elo, A. E. (1965). Age changes in master chess performances. Journal of Gerontology, 20, 289299.Google Scholar
Elo, A. E. (1986). The rating of chessplayers, past and present (2nd edn.). New York: Arco.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2014). Why expert performance is special and cannot be extrapolated from studies of performance in the general population: A response to criticisms. Intelligence, 45, 81103.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2016). Summing up hours of any type of practice versus identifying optimal practice activities: Commentary on Macnamara, Moreau, & Hambrick (2016). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 351354.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49, 725747.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211245.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363406.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence on maximal adaptations on task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273305.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Pool, R. (2016). Peak: Secrets from the new science of expertise. New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1979). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: Julian Friedman Publishers. (Original work published 1869)Google Scholar
Gartner, H., Minnerop, M., Pieperhoff, P., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., Altenmuller, E., & Amunts, K. (2013). Brain morphometry shows effects of long-term musical practice in middle-aged keyboard players. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. DOI: Artn 636 10.3389/Fpsyg.2013.00636.Google Scholar
Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Gray matter differences between musicians and nonmusicians. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 514517.Google Scholar
Gembris, H., & Heye, A. (2014). Growing older in a symphony orchestra: The development of the age-related self-concept and the self-estimated performance of professional musicians in a lifespan perspective. Musicae Scientiae, 18, 371391.Google Scholar
Gentner, D. G. (1987). Timing of skilled motor performance: Tests of the proportional duration model. Psychological Review, 94, 255276.Google Scholar
Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2002). Effects of domain knowledge, working memory capacity and age on cognitive performance: An investigation of the knowledge-is-power hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 339387.Google Scholar
Hanakawa, T., Honda, M., Okada, T., Fukuyama, H., & Shibasaki, H. (2003). Neural correlates underlying mental calculation in abacus experts: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. NeuroImage, 19, 296307.Google Scholar
Hanggi, J., Langer, N., Lutz, K., Birrer, K., Merillat, S., & Jancke, L. (2015). Structural brain correlates associated with professional handball playing. PloS ONE, 10. DOI: UNSP e0124222 10.1371/journal.pone.0124222.Google Scholar
Hanna-Pladdy, B., & Gajewski, B. (2012). Recent and past musical activity predicts cognitive aging variability: Direct comparison with general lifestyle activities. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 198. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00198.Google Scholar
Hoffman, R. R., Shadbolt, N. R., Burton, A. M., & Klein, G. (1995). Eliciting knowledge from experts: A methodological analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 62, 129158.Google Scholar
Horn, J. L., & Masunaga, H. (2000). New directions for research into aging and intelligence: The development of expertise. In Perfect, T. J. & Maylor, E. A. (eds.), Models of cognitive aging: Debates in psychology (pp. 125159). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1999). Innate talents: Reality or myth? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 399442.Google Scholar
Hoyer, W. J., & Ingolfsdottir, D. (2003). Age, skill, and contextual cuing in target detection. Psychology and Aging, 18, 210218.Google Scholar
Hulin, C. L., Henry, R. A., & Noon, S. L. (1990). Adding a dimension: Time as a factor in the generalizability of predictive relationships. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 328340.Google Scholar
Jastrzembski, T. S., & Charness, V. (2007). The model human processor and the older adult: Parameter estimation and validation within a mobile phone task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13, 224248.Google Scholar
Jentzsch, I., Mkrtchian, A., & Kansal, N. (2014). Improved effectiveness of performance monitoring in amateur instrumental musicians. Neuropsychologia, 52, 117124.Google Scholar
Kaufman, A. S. (2001). WAIS-III IQs, Horn’s theory, and generational changes from young adulthood to old age. Intelligence, 29, 131167.Google Scholar
Kennedy, Q., Taylor, J., Noda, A., Yesavage, J., & Lazzeroni, L. C. (2015). The STEP model: Characterizing simultaneous time effects on practice for flight simulator performance among middle-aged and older pilots. Psychology and Aging, 30, 699711.Google Scholar
Kliegl, R., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1989). Testing-the-limits and the study of adult age differences in cognitive plasticity of a mnemonic skill. Developmental Psychology, 25, 247256.Google Scholar
Kramer, A. F., & Willis, S. L. (2002). Enhancing the cognitive vitality of older adults. Current Directions in Psychological Sciences, 11, 173177.Google Scholar
Krampe, R. T. (2002). Aging, expertise and fine motor movement. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 769776.Google Scholar
Krampe, R. T., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Intelligence as adaptive resource development and resource allocation: A new look through the lenses of SOC and expertise. In Sternberg, R. J. & Grigorenko, E. L. (eds.), The psychology of abilities, competencies, and expertise (pp. 3169). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Krampe, R. T., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2001). Age-specific problems in rhythmic timing. Psychology and Aging, 16, 1230.Google Scholar
Krampe, R. T., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 331359.Google Scholar
Krampe, R. T., Kliegl, R., Mayr, U., Engbert, R., & Vorberg, D. (2000). The fast and the slow of skilled bimanual rhythm production: Parallel versus integrated timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 206233.Google Scholar
Krampe, R. T., Smolders, C., & Doumas, M. (2014). Leisure sports and postural control: Can a black belt protect your balance from aging? Psychology and Aging, 29, 95102.Google Scholar
Krampe, R. T., Wenderoth, N., Lavrysen, A., & Swinnen, S. P. (forthcoming). It’s all in the wrist: Neural expert mechanisms protect musicians’ timing skills from aging.Google Scholar
Leyk, D., Ruther, T., Wunderlich, M., Sievert, A., Essfeld, D., Witzki, A., … & Lollgen, H. (2010). Physical performance in middle age and old age: Good news for our sedentary and aging society. Deutsches Arzteblatt International, 107, 809816.Google Scholar
Li, S. C., Lindenberger, U., Hommel, B., Aschersleben, G., Prinz, W., & Baltes, P. B. (2004). Transformations in the couplings among intellectual abilities and constituent cognitive processes across the life span. Psychological Science, 15, 155163.Google Scholar
Lima, S. D., Hale, S., & Myerson, J. (1991). How general is general slowing? Evidence from the lexical domain. Psychology and Aging, 6, 416425.Google Scholar
Lindenberger, U., Kliegl, R., & Baltes, P. B. (1992). Professional expertise does not eliminate negative age differences in imagery-based memory performance during adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 7, 585593.Google Scholar
Macnamara, B. N., Moreau, D., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2016). The relationship between deliberate practice and performance in sports: A meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 333350.Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 97, 43984403.Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Valentine, E. R., Wilding, J. M., & Kapur, N. (2003). Routes to remembering: The brains behind superior memory. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 9095.Google Scholar
Masunaga, H., & Horn, J. (2001). Expertise in relation to aging changes in components of intelligence. Psychology and Aging, 16, 293311.Google Scholar
Maylor, E. A. (1994). Ageing and the retrieval of specialized and general knowledge: Performance of masterminds. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 105114.Google Scholar
McEvoy, G. M., & Cascio, W. F. (1989). Cumulative evidence of the relationship between employee age and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 1117.Google Scholar
Meinz, E. J. (2000). Experience-based attenuation of age-related differences in music cognition tasks. Psychology and Aging, 15, 297312.Google Scholar
Meister, I., Krings, T., Foltys, H., Boroojerdi, B., Muller, M., Topper, R., & Thron, A. (2005). Effects of long-term practice and task complexity in musicians and nonmusicians performing simple and complex motor tasks: Implications for cortical motor organization. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 345352.Google Scholar
Mireles, D. E., & Charness, N. (2002). Computational explorations of the influence of structured knowledge on age-related cognitive decline. Psychology and Aging, 17, 245259.Google Scholar
Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, E. G., Cepeda, N. J., & Chau, T. (2011). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychological Science, 22, 14251433.Google Scholar
Morrow, D. G., Ridolfo, H. E., Menard, W. E., Sanborn, A., Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Magnor, C., … & Bryant, D. (2003). Environmental support promotes expertise-based mitigation of age differences on pilot communication tasks. Psychology and Aging, 18, 268284.Google Scholar
Myerson, J., Hale, S., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L. W., & Smith, G. A. (1990). The information-loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Review, 97, 475487.Google Scholar
Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2008). The relationship of age to ten dimensions of job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 392423.Google Scholar
Nunes, A., & Kramer, A. F. (2009). Experience-based mitigation of age-related performance declines: Evidence from air traffic control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 1224.Google Scholar
Over, R., & Thomas, P. (1995). Age and skilled psychomotor performance: A comparison of younger and older golfers. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 41, 112.Google Scholar
Parbery-Clark, A., Anderson, S., Hittner, E., & Kraus, N. (2012). Musical experience offsets age-related delays in neural timing. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(7), e1481e1484.Google Scholar
Park, D. C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 173196.Google Scholar
Pimentel, A. E., Gentile, C. L., Tanaka, H., Seals, D. R., & Gates, P. E. (2003). Greater rate of decline in maximal aerobic capacity with age in endurance-trained than in sedentary men. Journal of Applied Physiology, 94, 24062413.Google Scholar
Plomin, R. (1990). The role of inheritance of behavior. Science, 248, 183188.Google Scholar
Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1993a). Crystal quest: A search for the basis of maintenance of practised skills into old age. In Baddeley, A. & Weiskrantz, L. (eds.), Attention: Selection, awareness, and control (pp. 188230). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1993b). Does it all go together when it goes? The Nineteenth Memorial Bartlett Lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 46, 385434.Google Scholar
Raz, N., Gunning-Dixon, F., Head, D., Rodrigue, K. M., Williamson, A., & Acker, J. D. (2004). Aging, sexual dimorphism, and hemispheric asymmetry of the cerebral cortex: Replicability of regional differences in volume. Neurobiology of Aging, 25, 377396.Google Scholar
Reuter, E. M., Voelcker-Rehage, C., Vieluf, S., Winneke, A. H., & Godde, B. (2014). Extensive occupational finger use delays age effects in tactile perception: An ERP study. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 76, 11601175.Google Scholar
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 177182.Google Scholar
Roring, R. W., & Charness, N. (2007). A multilevel model analysis of expertise in chess across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 22, 291299.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1984). Effects of age and skill in typing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 345371.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Expertise as the circumvention of human processing limitations. In Ericsson, K. A. & Smith, J. (eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 286300). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403428.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (2010). Major issues in cognitive aging. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A., Babcock, R. L., Skovronek, E., Mitchell, D. R. D., & Palmon, R. (1990). Age and experience effects in spatial visualization. Developmental Psychology, 26, 128136.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A., & Maurer, T. J. (1996). Aging, job performance, and career development. In Birren, J. E. & Schaie, K. W. (eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (4th edn.) (pp. 353364). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schellenberg, E. G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological Science, 15, 511514.Google Scholar
Schellenberg, E. G. (2011). Examining the association between music lessons and intelligence. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 283302.Google Scholar
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 262274.Google Scholar
Schooler, C., & Mulatu, M. S. (2001). The reciprocal effects of leisure time activities and intellectual functioning in older people: A longitudinal analysis. Psychology and Aging, 16, 466482.Google Scholar
Schulz, R., & Curnow, C. (1988). Peak performance and age among superathletes: Track and field, swimming, baseball, tennis and golf. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 43, 113120.Google Scholar
Seidler, R., Erdeniz, B., Koppelmans, V., Hirsiger, S., Merillat, S., & Jancke, L. (2015). Associations between age, motor function, and resting state sensorimotor network connectivity in healthy older adults. NeuroImage, 108, 4759.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2012). Creative productivity and aging. In Whitbourne, S. K. & Sliwinski, M. J. (eds.), Wiley-Blackwell handbook of adulthood and aging (pp. 477496). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Singer, T., Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Plasticity of memory for new learning in very old age: A story of major loss? Psychology and Aging, 18, 306317.Google Scholar
Sloboda, J. A. (1991). Musical expertise. In Ericsson, K. A. & Smith, J. (eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 153171). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sluming, V., Brooks, J., Howard, M., Downes, J. J., & Roberts, N. (2007). Broca’s area supports enhanced visuospatial cognition in orchestral musicians. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 37993806.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1999). Intelligence as developing expertise. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 24, 359375.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Wagner, R. K. (1986). Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of competence in the everyday world. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sturman, M. C. (2003). Searching for the inverted U-shaped relationship between time and performance: Meta-analyses of the experience/performance, tenure/performance, and age/performance relationships. Journal of Management, 29, 609640.Google Scholar
Swaen, G. M. H., Kant, I., van Amelsvoort, L. G. P. M., & Beurskens, A. J. H. M. (2002). Job mobility, its determinants, and its effects: Longitudinal data from the Maastricht cohort study. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 7, 121129.Google Scholar
Tanaka, H., & Seals, D. R. (2003). Invited review. Dynamic exercise performance in Masters athletes: Insight into the effects of primary human aging on physiological functional capacity. Journal of Applied Physiology, 95, 21522162.Google Scholar
Taylor, J. L., Kennedy, Q., Adamson, M. M., Lazzeroni, L. C., Noda, A., Murphy, G. M., & Yesavage, J. A. (2011). Influences of APOE epsilon4 and expertise on performance of older pilots. Psychology and Aging, 26, 480487.Google Scholar
Taylor, J. L., Kennedy, Q., Noda, A., & Yesavage, J. A. (2007). Pilot age and expertise predict flight simulator performance: A 3-year longitudinal study. Neurology, 68, 648654.Google Scholar
Vaci, N., Gula, B., & Bilalić, M. (2015). Is age really cruel to experts? Compensatory effects of activity. Psychology and Aging, 30, 740754.Google Scholar
Verhaeghen, P. (2014). The elements of cognitive aging: Meta-analyses of age-related differences in processing speed and their consequences. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Vieluf, S., Godde, B., Reuter, E. M., & Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2013). Effects of age and fine motor expertise on the bilateral deficit in force initiation. Experimental Brain Research, 231, 107116.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Sternberg, R. J. (1991). Tacit knowledge: Its uses in identifying, assessing, and developing managerial talent. In Jones, J., Steffy, B. & Bray, D. (eds.), Applying psychology in business: The manager’s handbook (pp. 333344). New York: Human Sciences Press.Google Scholar
Wai, J. (2014). Experts are born, then made: Combining prospective and retrospective longitudinal data shows that cognitive ability matters. Intelligence, 45, 7480.Google Scholar
Waldman, D. A., & Avolio, B. J. (1986). A meta-analysis of age-differences in job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 3338.Google Scholar
Walsh, D. A., & Hershey, D. A. (1993). Mental models and the maintenance of complex problem-solving skills in old age. In Cerella, J., Rybash, J., Hoyer, W., & Commons, M. (eds.), Adult information processing: Limits on loss (pp. 553584). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 272292.Google Scholar
Westerman, S. J., Davies, D. R., Glendon, A. I., & Stammers, R. B. (1998). Ageing and word processing competence: Compensation or compilation. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 579597.Google Scholar
Winner, E. (1982). Invented worlds: The psychology of the arts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Winner, E. (1996). The rage to master: The decisive role of talent in the visual arts. In Ericsson, K. A. (ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports and games (pp. 271302). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Woollett, K., & Maguire, E. A. (2011). Acquiring “the knowledge” of London’s layout drives structural brain changes. Current Biology, 21, 21092114.Google Scholar
Yang, L., Krampe, R. T., & Baltes, P. B. (2006). Basic forms of cognitive plasticity extended into the oldest-old: Retest learning, age, and cognitive functioning. Psychology and Aging, 21, 372378.Google Scholar
Zendel, B. R., & Alain, C. (2014). Enhanced attention-dependent activity in the auditory cortex of older musicians. Neurobiology of Aging, 35, 5563.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×