Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T05:05:22.784Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 December 2015

David McNeill
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Why We Gesture
The Surprising Role of Hand Movements in Communication
, pp. 188 - 199
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abrahamsen, A., Cavallo, M.M. and McCluer, J.A. 1985. “Is the sign advantage a robust phenomenon? From gesture to language in two modalities.” Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 31: 177–209.Google Scholar
Acredolo, L.P. and Goodwyn, S.W. 1990. “Sign language in babies: the significance of symbolic gesturing for understanding language development.” In Vasta, R., (ed.), Annals of Child Development, Vol. 7, pp. 1–42. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. 2005. “From monkey-like action recognition to human language: an evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28: 105–124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arbib, M. 2012. How the Brain Got Language: The Mirror System Hypothesis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, D., Stokoe, W.F. and Wilcox, S.E. 1995. Gesture and the Nature of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, Q.D. 2011. “Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa.” Science 332: 346–349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. and Hitch, G.J. 1974. “Working memory.” In Bower, G. (ed.), Recent Advances in Learning and Motivation, Vol. VIII, pp. 49–89. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bakhtin, M.M. 1981. The Dialogic Imagination, ed. M. Holquist, trans. C. Emerson and M. Holquist, trans. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Bannard, C., Lieven, E. and Tomasello, M. 2009. “Evaluating constructivist theory via Bayesian modeling of children's early grammatical development.” Abstract posted on the International Cognitive Linguistics Conference website, accessed 30 March 2009 (since removed). See, Bannard, C., Lieven, E. and Tomasello, M. 2009. “Modeling children's early grammatical knowledge.” PNAS 106: 17284–17289, for a published version, but lacking the pithy quote of the abstract.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E. and Dick, F. 2002. “Language, gesture and the developing brain.” In Casey, B.J. and Munakata, Y. (eds.). Special issue: converging method approach to the study of developmental science. Developmental Psychobiology 40: 293–310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L.Mills, D.Galaburda, A. and Korenberg, J. R. 1999. “Bridging cognition, the brain and molecular genetics: evidence from Williams syndrome.” Trends in Neuroscience 22: 197–207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bickerton, D. 1990. Language and Species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Blumenthal, Arthur (ed.). 1970. Language and Psychology: Historical Aspects of Psycholinguistics. New York: John Wiley and Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. and Tomasello, M. 1998. “Chimpanzee and human cultures.” Current Anthropology 39: 591–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolens, G. 2012. The Style of Gestures: Embodiment and Cognition in Literary Narrative. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Braine, M.D.S. 1963. “The ontogeny of English phrase structure: the first phase.” Language 39: 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bressem, J. 2010. Pounding the Verbal Utterance: Forms and Functions of Beats. International Society of Gesture Studies, Conference Talk, Frankfurt/Oder, Germany.Google Scholar
Bressem, J., Ladewig, S.H. and Müller, C. 2015. “Ways of expressing action in multimodal narrations—the semiotic complexity of character viewpoint depictions. In Hübl, A. and Steinbach, M. (eds.), Linguistic Foundations of Narration in Spoken and Sign Languages. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Brock, J. 2007. “Language abilities in Williams syndrome: a critical review.” Development and Psychopathology 19: 97–127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browman, C.P. and Goldstein, L. 1990.“Tiers in articulatory phonology, with some implications for casual speech.” In Kingston, J. and Beckman, M.E. (eds.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology I: Between the Grammar and Physics of Speech, pp. 341–376. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R. and Lenneberg, E. 1954. “A study in language and cognition.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 49: 454–462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, T.A. 2010. “Stranger from Siberia.” Nature 464: 838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, P. and Levinson, S. 1990. Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bühler, K. 1982. “The deictic field of language and deictic words.” In Jarvella, R. and Klein, W. (eds.), Speech, Place, and Action, pp. 9–30. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Butcher, C. and Goldin-Meadow, S. 2000. “Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: when hand and mouth come together.” In McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture, pp. 235–257. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chan, J.B. and Iacono, T. 2001. “Gesture and word production in children with Down Syndrome.” AAC Augmentative and Attentive Communication. 17: 73–87.Google Scholar
Chase, W.G. and Eriksson, K.A. 1981. “Skilled memory.” In Anderson, J.R. (ed.), Cognitive Skills and Their Acquisition, pp. 227–249. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Clark, A. 1997. Being There: Putting Brain, Body and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, A.A. 1977. “The communicative function of hand illustrators.” Journal of Communication, 27: 54–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, J. 1995. Pride and a Daily Marathon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cook, S.W., Yip, T.K. and Goldin-Meadow, S. 2010. Gesturing makes memories that last. Journal of Memory and Language 63: 465–475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corballis, M.C. 2002. From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Corballis, M.C. 2011. The Recursive Mind: The Origin of Human Language, Thought, and Civilization. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Corballis, M.C. 2014. The word according to Adam: the role of gesture in language evolution. In Seyfeddinipur, M. and Gullberg, M. (eds.), From Gesture in Conversation to Visible Action as Utterance, pp. 177–197. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. 2001. “The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 87–185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniels, P.T. and Bright, W. 1996. The World's Writing Systems. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D.T. 1991. Consciousness Explained. Boston: Little, Brown & Company.Google Scholar
de Ruiter, J. 2000. “The production of gesture and speech. In McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture, pp. 285–311. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Diffloth, G. 1994. “i: big a: small.” In Hinton, L., Nichols, J. and Ohala, J.J. (eds.), Sound Symbolism, pp. 107–114. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Diamond, A. 2002. “Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry.” In Stuss, D.T. and Knight, R.T. (eds.), Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, pp. 466–503. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Donald, M. 1991. Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dray, N. and McNeill, D. 1990. “Gestures during discourse: the contextual structuring of thought,” in Tsohatzidis, S.L. (ed.). Meanings and Prototypes: Studies in Linguistic Categorization, pp. 465–487. London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Dreyfus, H. 1994. Being-in-the-World: A Commentary on Heidegger's Being and Time, Division I. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. 1996. Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Duncan, S. 2005. “Gesture in signing: a case study in Taiwan Sign Language.” Language and Linguistics 6: 279–318.Google Scholar
Duncan, S. and Fiske, D.W. 1977. Face-To-Face Interaction: Research, Methods, and Theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dunn, M., Greenhill, S.J., Levinson, S.C. and Gray, R.D. 2011. “Evolved structure of language shows lineage-specific trends in word-order universals.” Nature, www.nature.com/journal/vaop/ncurrent/full/nature09923.html (accessed April 19, 2011).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duranti, A. and Goodwin, C. 1992. Rethinking Context: Language as an Interactive Phenomenon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Borinstein, H.B. and Thompson, R. 2005. “Bimodal bilingualism: code-blending between spoken English and American Sign Language.” In Cohen, J., McAlister, K.T., Rolstad, K. and MacSwan, J. (eds.). Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism, pp. 663–673. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Enfield, N.J. 2001. “Lip-pointing: a discussion of form and function with reference to data from Laos.” Gesture 1: 185–211.Google Scholar
Engle, R.A. 2000. Toward a Theory of Multimodal Communication: Combining Speech, Gestures, Diagrams, and Demonstrations in Instructional Explanations. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Evans, P.D, Gilbert, S.L., Mekel-Bobrow, N., Vallender, E.J., Anderson, J.R., Vaez-Azizi, L.M., Tishkoff, S.A., Hudson, R.R. and Lahn, B.T. 2005. “Microcephalin, a gene regulating brain size, continues to evolve adaptively in humans.” Science 309: 1717–1720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Federmeier, K.D. and Kutas, M. 1999. “Right words and left words: electrophysiological evidence for hemispheric differences in meaning processing.” Cognitive Brain Research 8: 373–392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fetz, E.E. 2007. “Volitional control of neural activity: Implications for brain-computer interfaces.” The Journal of Physiology 579: 571–579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fillmore, C., Kay, P. and O'Connor, C. 1988. Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions: the case of let alone. Language 64: 501–538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Firbas, J. 1971. “On the concept of communicative dynamism in the theory of functional sentence perspective.” Philologica Pragensia 8, 135–144.Google Scholar
Fodor, J.A. 1983. Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Forrester, M. 2008. “The emergence of self-repain: a case study of one child during the preschool years.” Research on Language and Social Interaction 41: 97–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forrester, M. 2014. Early Social Interaction: A Case Comparison of Developmental Pragmatics and Psychoanalytic Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freyd, J. 1983. “Shareability: the social psychology of epistemology.” Cognitive Science 7: 191–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fromkin, V.A. 1973. Speech Errors as Linguistic Evidence. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Furuyama, N. 2000. “Gestural interactions between the instructor and the learner in origami instruction.” In McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture, pp. 99–117. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Furuyama, N. and Sekine, K. 2007. “Forgetful or strategic? The mystery of the systematic avoidance of reference in the cartoon story narrative.” In Duncan, S.D, Cassell, J. and Levy, E.T. (eds.), Gesture and the Dynamic Dimension of Language, pp. 75–81. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. 2005. How the Body Shapes the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelb, A. and Goldstein, K. 1925. “Über Farbennamenamnesie.” Psychologische Forschung 6: 127–186.Google Scholar
Gentilucci, M. and Dalla Volta, R. 2007. “The motor system and the relationship between speech and gesture.” Gesture 7: 159–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. and Goldstein, D.G. 1996. “Mind as computer: birth of a metaphor.” Creativity Research 9: 131–144.Google Scholar
Gill, S. 2007. “Entrainment and musicality in the human system interface.” AI & Society. 21: 567–605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. 1985. “Iconicity, isomorphism and non-arbitrary coding in syntax.” In Haiman, J. (ed.). Iconicity in Syntax, pp. 187–219. Amsterdamand Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Glick, J. 1983. “Piaget, Vygotsky, and Werner.” In Wapner, S. and Kaplan, B. (eds.), Toward a Holistic Developmental Psychology, pp. 35–52. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. 1995. Constructions: A Construction Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. 2003a. The Resilience of Language: What Gesture Creation in Deaf Children Can Tell Us About How All Children Learn Language. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. 2003b. Hearing Gesture: How Our Hands Help Us Think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. and Butcher, C. 2003. “Pointing toward two-word speech in young children.” In Kita, S. (ed.), Pointing: Where Language, Culture, and Cognition Meet, pp. 85–107. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., McNeill, D. and Singleton, J. 1996. “Silence is liberating: removing the handcuffs on grammatical expression in the manual modality.” Psychological Review 103: 34–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H., Kelley, S.D. and Wagner, S. 2001. “Explaining math: gesturing lightens the load.” Psychological Science 12: 516–522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goren-Inbar, N., Alperson, N., Kislev, M.E., Simchoni, O., Melamed, Y., Ben-Nun, A. and Werker, E. 2004. “Evidence of hominid control of fire at Gesher Benot Ya'aqov, Israel.” Science 304: 725–727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, J.H. 1970. The Languages of Africa. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S. and Keysar, B. 1990Understanding metaphorical comparisons: beyond similarity,” Psychological Review 97: 3–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M. 2013. “So you think gestures are compensatory? Reflections based on child and adult learner data.” In Mattsson, A.F. and Norrby, C. (eds.), Language Acquisition and Use in Multilingual Contexts: Theory and Practice, pp. 39–49. Lund: Travaux de l'Institut de linguistique de Lund 52.Google Scholar
Gullberg, M., Hendricks, H. and Hickmann, M. 2008.“Learning to talk and gesture about motion in French.” First Language 28: 200–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hale, K. 1983. “Warlpiri and the grammar of non-configurational languages.” Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 1: 5–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M., Chomsky, N. and Fitch, W.T. 2002. “The language faculty: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?Science 298: 1569–1579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haviland, J. 2013. “Where did ‘where do nouns come from?’ come from?Gesture 13: 245–252.Google Scholar
Herder, J.G. 1986 [1772]. Essay on the Origin of Language. Moran, J.H. and Gode, A. (trans.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Hickmann, M., Hendricks, H. and Gullberg, M. 2011. “Developmental perspectives on the expression of motion in speech and gesture: a comparison of French and English.” Language, Interaction & Acquisition 2: 129–156.Google Scholar
Hickok, G. 2009. “Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21: 1229–1243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoetjes, M., Krahmer, E. and Swerts, M. 2015. “On what happens in gesture when communication is unsuccessful.” Speech Communication 72: 160–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, W.D. and Cantero, M. 2003. “From hand to mouth in the evolution of language: the influence of vocal behavior on lateralized hand use in manual gestures by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).” Developmental Science 6: 55–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hrdy, S.B. 2009. Mothers and Others: The Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Humboldt, W. 1999. On Language. Trans. Heath, P., ed. Losonsky, M.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hurley, S. 1998. Consciousness in Action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, P.R. and Dabholkar, A.S. 1997. “Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex.” Journal of Comparative Neurology 387: 167–178.Google ScholarPubMed
Iverson, J.M., Longobardi, E. and Caselli, M.C. 2003. “Relationship between gestures and words in children with Down's syndrome and typically developing children in the early stages of communicative development.” International Journal of Language and Communicative Disorders 38: 179–197.Google ScholarPubMed
Jakobson, R. 1960. “Concluding statement: linguistics and poetics.” In Sebeok, T. (ed.), Style in Language, pp. 350–377. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
James, W. 1890. Psychology (Vol. 1, Chap. IX). New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1979. “Micro- and macrodevelopmental changes in language acquisition and other representational systems.” Cognitive Science 3: 91–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, S., Kravitz, C. and Hopkins, M. 2004. “Neural correlates of bimodal speech and gesture comprehension.” Brain and Language 89: 253–260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendon, A. 1980. “Gesticulation and speech: two aspects of the process of utterance.” In Key, M.R. (ed.), The Relationship of Verbal and Nonverbal Communication, pp. 207–227. The Hague: Mouton and Co.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. 1988. Sign Languages of Aboriginal Australia: Cultural, Semiotic and Communicative Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. 1991. “Some considerations for a theory of language origins.” Man 26: 199–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. 2004. Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. 2008. “Some reflections of the relationship between ‘gesture’ and ‘sign.’Gesture 8: 348–366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. 2009. “Manual actions, speech and the nature of language.” In Gambarara, D. and Givigliano, A. (eds.), Origine e sviluppo del linguaggio, fra teoria e storia. Pubblicazioni della Società di Filosofia del Linguaggio, pp. 19–33. Rome: Aracne editrice s.r.l.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. 2010. Accounting for Forelimb Actions as a Component of Utterance: An Evolutionary Approach. Plenary Lecture. International Society for Gesture Studies, Frankfurt/Oder, July 25.Google Scholar
Kimbara, I. 2006. “On gestural mimicry.” Gesture, 6: 39–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kita, S. 2000. “How representational gestures help speaking.” In McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture, pp. 162–185. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kita, S. and Özyürek, A. 2003. “What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking.Journal of Memory and Language 48: 16–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopp, S., Bergmann, K. and Kahl, S. 2013. A Spreading-Activation Model of the Semantic Coordination of Speech and Gesture, 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Krause, J. et al. 2010. “The complete mitochondrial DNA genome of an unknown hominin from southern Siberia.” Nature 464: 894–897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krauss, R., Chen, Y. and Gottesman, R.F. 2000. “Lexical gestures and lexical access: a process model,” in McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture, 261–283. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kröger, B.J., Birkholtz, P., Kaufmann, E. and Neuschaefer-Rube, C. 2010. “Beyond vocal tract actions: speech prosody and co-verbal gesturing in face-to-face communication.” Cognitive Processing 11: 187–205.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. 1980. Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
LeBaron, C. and Streeck, J. 2000. “Gestures, knowledge, and the world.” In McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture, pp. 118–138. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Levitin, D.J. and Menon, V. 2003. “Musical structure is processed in ‘language’ areas of the brain: a possible role for Brodmann Area 47 in temporal coherence.” Neuroimage. 20: 2142–2152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, W.J.M. 1989. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Levelt, W.J.M. 2013. A History of Psycholinguistics: The Pre-Chomskyan Era. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Levelt, W.J.M., Richardson, G and La Heij, W. 1985. “Pointing and voicing in deictic expressions.” Journal of Memory and Language 24: 133–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W.J.M., Roelofs, A. and Meyer, A.S. 1999. “A theory of lexical access in speech production.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 1–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levinson, S. and Holler, J. 2014. “The origin of human multi-modal communication.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 369, 20130302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levinson, S. and Torreira, F. 2015. “Timing in turn-taking and its implication for processing models of language.” Frontiers in Psychology 6: Article 731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, E.T. 2007. “The construction of temporally coherent narrative by an autistic adolescent: co-construction of speech, enactment and gesture.” In Duncan, S.D., Cassell, J. and Levy, E.T (eds.), Gesture and the Dynamic Dimension of Language, pp. 285–301. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Levy, E.T. 2008. The Mediation of Coherent Discourse by Kinesthetic Reenactment: A Case Study of an Autistic Adolescent, Part II. Manuscript. Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut at Stamford.Google Scholar
Levy, E.T. 2009–2010. “The mediation of coherent discourse by kinesthetic reenactment: a case study of an autistic adolescent, part II.” Imagination, Cognition and Personality 29: 41–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, E.T. 2011. A New Study of the Co-Emergence of Speech and Gestures: Towards an Embodied Account of Early Narrative Development. Poster presented at the 2011 Language Fest, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT.
Levy, E.T. and Fowler, C.A. 2005. How Autistic Children May Use Narrative Discourse to Scaffold Coherent Interpretations of Events: A Case Study. Manuscript. Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut at Stamford.Google Scholar
Levy, E.T. and McNeill, D. 2015. The Narrative Development of Young Children: Explorations in the Embodiment of Cohesion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, J. and Trevarthen, C. 1976: “Metacontrol of hemispheric function in human split-brain patients.” Journal of Experimental Psychology (Human Performance & Perception) 2: 299–312Google ScholarPubMed
Liebal, K., Bressem, J. and Müller, C. 2010. Recurrent Forms and Contexts: Families of Gestures in Non-Human Primates. Conference of the International Society of Gesture Studies, Panel 13: Towards a grammar of gesture: Evolution, brain and linguistic structures, Frankfurt/Oder, Germany.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. 2002. “On the nature and evolution of the neural bases of human language.” Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 45: 36–62.Google Scholar
Lieven, E., Salomo, D. and Tomasello, M. 2009. “Two-year-old children's production of multiword utterances: a usage-based analysis.” Cognitive Linguistics 20: 461–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez-Ozieblo, R. 2013. Exchange on Why We Gesture. Apply to author at Huddersfield University (UK), School of Linguistics and Modern Languages.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P.F. 2008. The Origin of Speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mampe, B., Friederici, A.D., Christophe, A. and Wermke, K. 2009. “Newborns’ cry melody is shaped by their native language.” Current Biology 19: 1–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W.D. 1987. “Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition.” Cognition 25(1): 71–102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayberry, R. and Jaques, J. 2000. “Gesture production during stuttered speech: insights into the nature of gesture-speech integration.” In McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture, pp. 199–214. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McCullough, K.-E. 2003. Gaze, Gesture, and the Construction of Meaning. Unpublished manuscript. University of Chicago Gesture-Speech Lab.Google Scholar
McCullough, K.-E. 2005. Using Gestures During Speaking: Self-Generating Indexical Fields. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Department of Linguistics, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. 1985. “So you think gestures are non-verbal?Psychological Review. 92: 350–371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. 1992. Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal about Thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. 2000. “Catchments and contexts: Non-modular factors in speech and gesture production.” In McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture, pp. 312–328. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. 2003. “Aspects of aspect.” Gesture 3: 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. 2005. Gesture and Thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. 2008. “Unexpected metaphors.” In Cienki, A. and Müller, C. (eds.), Metaphor and Gesture, pp. 155–170. AmsterdamandPhliadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. 2009. “Imagery for speaking.” In Guo, J., Lieven, E., Budwig, N., Ervin-Tripp, S., Nakamura, K. and Özçaliskan, S. (eds.), Crosslinguistic Approaches to the Psychology of Language: Research in the Tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin, pp. 517–530. London: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. 2012. How Language Began: Gesture and Speech in Human Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. 2014a. “The emblem as metaphor.” In Seyfeddinipur, M. and Gullberg, M. (eds.), From Gesture in Conversation to Visible Action as Utterance, pp. 75–104. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. 2014b. “Gesture–speech unity: phylogenesis, ontogenesis, and microgenesis.” Language, Interaction & Acquisition 5: 137–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. and Duncan, S.D. 2000. “Growth points in thinking for speaking,” in McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture, pp. 141–161. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. and Duncan, S.D. 2010. “Gestures and growth points in three language disorders,” in Guendouzi, J., Loncke, F. and Williams, M.J. (eds.), The Handbook of Psycholinguistic and Cognitive Processes: Perspectives in Communication Disorders, pp. 663–685. New York: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. and Levy, E. 1982. “Conceptual representations in language activity and gesture.” In Jarvella, R.J. and Klein, W. (eds.), Speech, Place, and Action, pp. 271–296. Oxford: Wiley.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. and Pedelty, L. 1995. “Right brain and gesture.” In Emmorey, K. and Reilly, J.S (eds.), Language, Gesture, and Space, pp. 63–85. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McNeill, D., Duncan, S., Franklin, A., Goss, J., Kimbaba, I., Parrill, F., Chen, L., Harper, M.Quek, F., Rose, T. and Tuttle, R. 2010. “Mind-merging.” In Morsella, E. (ed.), Expressing Oneself/Expressing One's Self: Communication, Language, Cognition, and Identity, pp. 143–164. London: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
McNeill, R.B. 2010. “Cum tacent, clamant: the pragmatics of silence in Catullus.” Classical Philology 105: 69–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mead, G.H. 1974. Mind, Self, and Society from the Standpoint of a Social Behaviorist. Ed. and intro. Morris, C.W.. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. 2007 [1962]. Phenomenology of Perception. Trans. Smith, C.. London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mithen, S. 2006. The Singing Neanderthals: The Origins of Music, Language, Mind, and Body. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Morris, D., Collett, P., Marsh, P. and O'shaughnessy, M. 1979. Gestures: Their Origins and Distribution. New York: Stein & Day.Google Scholar
Montaigne, M. 1958. The Complete Essays of Montaigne. Trans. Frame, D.. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Müller, C. 2008. Metaphors—Dead and Alive, Sleeping and Waking: A Dynamic View. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, C. 2014. “Gesture as ‘deliberate expressive movement.’” In Seyfeddinipur, M. and Gullberg, M. (eds.), From Gesture in Conversation to Visible Action as Utterance, pp. 127–151. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Müller, F.M. 1861. The Theoretical Stage, and the Origin of Language. Lecture 9 from Lectures on the Science of Language. Reprinted in R. Harris (ed.). 1996.The Origin of Language.Bristol: Thoemmes Press, pp. 7–41.Google Scholar
Morton, J., Marcus, S. and Frankis, C. 1976. “Perceptual centers.” Psychological Review 83: 105–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nobe, S. 1996. Representational Gestures, Cognitive Rhythms, and Acoustic Aspects of Speech: A Network/Threshold Model of Gesture Production. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Department of Psychology, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Oberauer, K. 2005. “Access to information in working memory: exploring the focus of attention.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 31: 714–728.Google Scholar
Özçaliskan, Ş. and Goldin-Meadow, S. 2009. “When gesture-speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change.” Language and Cognitive Processes 24: 190–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Özyürek, A. 2001a. “What do speech-gesture mismatches reveal about language specific processing? A comparison of Turkish and English.” In Cavé, C., Guaïtella, I. and Santi, S. (eds.), Oralité et Gestualité: Interactions et Comportments Multimodaux Dans La Communnication. Paris: L'Harattan.Google Scholar
Özyürek, A. 2001b. What Do Speech-Gesture Mismatches Reveal About Speech and Gesture Integration? A Comparison of Turkish and English. Proceedings of the 27th Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Park-Doob, M.A. 2010. Gesturing Through Time: Holds and Intermodal Timing in the Stream of Speech. Unpublished PhD dissertation. Department of Linguistics, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Peña, M., Maki, A., Kovacić, D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Koizumi, H., Bouquet, F. and Mehler, J. 2003. “Sounds and silence: an optical topography study of language recognition at birth.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 100: 11702–11705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pika, S. and Bugnyar, T. 2011. “The use of referential gestures in ravens (Corvus corax) in the wild.” Nature Communications, 29 November.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. 1994. The Language Instinct. New York: Harper Perennial.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollick, A.S. 2006. Gestures and Multimodal Signaling in Bonobos and Chimpanzees. Unpublished PhD dissertation. Department of Psychology, Emory University.Google Scholar
Quaeghebeur, L. 2010. A Philosophy of Everyday Face-to-Face Conversation. Antwerp: University of Antwerp.Google Scholar
Quaeghebeur, L. 2012. “The ‘all-at-onceness’ of embodied, face-to-face interaction.” Journal of Cognitive Semiotics 4: 167–188.Google Scholar
Quaeghebeur, L. and Reynaert, P. 2010. Does the need for linguistic expression constitute a problem to be solved?Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 9: 15–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quaeghebeur, L., Duncan, S., Gallgher, S., Cole, J. and McNeill, D. 2014. “Aproprioception, gesture, and cognitive being.” In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S.H., McNeill, D and Bressem, J. (eds.), Body—language—communication: an international handbook on multimodality in human interaction, Vol. 2, pp. 2026–2048. Berlin: De Gruyter-Mouton.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. and Blakeslee, S. 1998. Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Reddy, M. 1979. “The conduit metaphor—a case of frame conflict in our language about language.” In Ortony, A. (ed.), Metaphor and Thought, pp. 284–324. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rhodes, R.A. and Lawler, J.M. 1981. “Athematic metaphors.” In Proceedings of the Chicago Linguistic Society, pp. 318–342. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Rieber, R.W. and Carton, A.S. (eds.). 1987. The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky. Volume 1: Problems of General Psychology. Including the Volume “Thinking and Speech.” Intro. and trans. Minick, N. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. and Arbib, M. 1998. “Language within our grasp.” Trends in Neurosciences 21: 188–194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozzi, F.V.R. and de Castro, J.M.B. 2004. “Surprisingly rapid growth in Neanderthals.” Nature 428: 936–939.Google Scholar
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E.A. and Jefferson, G. 1974. “A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation.” Language. 50: 696–735CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sahin, N.T., Pinker, S., Cash, S.S., Schomer, D. and Halgren, E. 2009. “Sequential processing of lexical, grammatical and phonological information within Broca's Area.” Science 326: 445–449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapir, E. 1921. Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.Google Scholar
Sapir, E. 1929. “A study in phonetic symbolism.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 12: 225–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saussure, F. 1959. Course in General Linguistics. Ed. Bally, C. and Sechehaye, A., trans. Baskin, W.. New York: The Philosophical Library.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E.A. 1984.“On some gestures’ relation to talk,” in Atkinson, J.M. and Heritage, J. (eds.), Structures of Social Action, pp. 266–298. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Science Daily. 2010. “New evidence of culture in wild chimpanzees.” Science Daily, January 4. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2009/10/091022122321.htm (accessed September 9, 2014).
Sekine, K. 2009a. “Changes in frame of reference use across the preschool years: a longitudinal study of the gestures and speech produced during route descriptions.” Language and Cognitive Processes 24: 218–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sekine, K. 2009b. Creating Context: A Function of Gesture. Seminar at the University of Chicago, January 21.Google Scholar
Senghas, A. 2003. “Intergenerational influence and ontogenetic development in the emergence of spatial grammar in Nicaraguan Sign Language.” Cognitive Development 18: 511–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silverstein, M. 1997. “The improvisational performance of culture in real time discursive practice.” In Sawyer, R.K. (ed.), Creativity in Performance, pp. 265–312. Greenwich, CT: Ablex.Google Scholar
Silverstein, M. 2003. “Indexical order and the dialectics of sociolinguistic life.” Language & Communication 23: 193–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, H.A. 1996. The Sciences of the Artificial, 3rd edn. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D.I. 1987. “Thinking for speaking.” In Aske, J. et al. (eds.), Proceedings of the Thirteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistic Society, pp. 435–445. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Slobin, D.I. 1996. “From ‘thought and language’ to ‘thinking for speaking.’” In Gumperz, J.J. and Levinson, S.C (eds.), Rethinking Linguistic Relativity, pp. 70–96. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sowell, E.R., Thompson, P.M. Holmes, C.Jernigan, T.L. and Toga, A.W. 1999. “Invivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions.” Nature Neuroscience 2: 859–861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stefanini, S., Caselli, M.C. and Volterra, V. 2007. “Spoken and gestural production in a naming task by young children with Down syndrome.” Brain and Language 101: 208–221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Streeck, J. 2009. Gesturecraft: The Manu-Facturing of Meaning. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweet, H. 1971 [1900]. “The history of language.” In Henderson, E. (ed.). The Indispensable Foundation: A Selection from the Writings of Henry Sweet, pp. 1–24. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. 2000. Toward a Cognitive Semantics. Vol. 2: Typology and Process in Concept Structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thomason, S. 1997. “On mechanisms of interference.” In Eliasson, S. and Jahr, E.H., (eds.). Language and Its Ecology: Essays in Memory of Einar Haugen, pp. 181–207. Berlin: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Thomason, S. 2011. Does Language Contact Simplify Grammars? (No.) Talk given at the University of Chicago, April 12.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 1999. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2008. Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2014. A Natural History of Human Thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trudgill, P. 2011. Investigations in Sociohistorical Linguistics: Stories of Colonization and Contact. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tuite, K. 1993. “The production of gesture.” Semiotica 93 :83–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Petten, C., Coulson, S., Rubin, S., Plante, E. and Parks, M. 1999. Time course of word identification and semantic integration in spoken language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 25: 394–417.Google ScholarPubMed
Vendler, Z.. 1967. Linguistics in Philosophy. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Volterra, V., Caselli, M.C., Capirici, O. and Pizzuto, E. 2005. “Gesture and the emergence and development of language.” In Tomasello, M. and Slobin, D.I. (eds.), Beyond Nature-Nurture: Essays in Honor of Elizabeth Bates, pp. 3–40. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L.S. 1987. Thought and Language. Ed. and trans. Hanfmann, E. and Vakar, G., revised and ed. Kozulin, A.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wachsmuth, I. and Sowa, T. (eds.). (2002). Gesture and Sign Language in Human–Computer Interaction. Berlin, Heidelberg and New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, H. and Kaplan, B. 1963. Symbol Formation. New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. [Reprinted in 1984 by Erlbaum.]Google Scholar
Whorf, B.L. 1956. Language, Thought, and Reality. Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Ed. Carroll, J.B.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Weinreich, U. 1953. Languages in Contact. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H. and Perner, J. 1983. “Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception.” Cognition 13:103–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woll, B. 2005/2006.“Do mouths sign? Do hands speak?” In Botha, R. and Swart, H.de (eds.), Restricted Linguistic Systems as Windows on Language Evolution. Utrecht: Netherlands Graduate School of Linguistics Occasional Series, Utrecht University, http://lotos.library.uu.nl/publish/articles/000287/bookpart.pdf (accessed May 2, 2011).Google Scholar
WSJ. 2009. “Magic flute: primal find sings of music's mystery,” WSJ, July 3–5, p. A9.
Wu, Y.C. and Coulson, S. 2010. “Gestures modulate speech processing early in utterances.NeuroReport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research 21: 522–526.Google ScholarPubMed
Wynn, T. and Coolidge, F. 2008. “Why not cognition?Current Anthropology 49: 895–897.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, T. and Coolidge, F. 2011. How to Think Like a Neandertal. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zbikowski, L.M. 2011. “Musical gesture and musical grammar: a cognitive approach.” In Gritten, A. and King, E. (eds.), New Perspectives on Music and Gesture, pp. 83–98. Farnham: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Zinchenko, V.P. 1985. “Vygotsky's ideas about units for the analysis of mind.” In Wertsch, J. (ed.), Culture Communication, and Cognition: Vygotskian Perspectives, pp. 94–118. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • David McNeill, University of Chicago
  • Book: Why We Gesture
  • Online publication: 05 December 2015
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316480526.011
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • David McNeill, University of Chicago
  • Book: Why We Gesture
  • Online publication: 05 December 2015
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316480526.011
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • David McNeill, University of Chicago
  • Book: Why We Gesture
  • Online publication: 05 December 2015
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316480526.011
Available formats
×