Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T04:34:59.386Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Intergroup Emotions Theory: Prejudice and Differentiated Emotional Reactions toward Outgroups

from Part I - General Theoretical Perspectives

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 November 2016

Angela T. Maitner
Affiliation:
American University of Sharjah
Eliot R. Smith
Affiliation:
Indiana University
Diane M. MacKie
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Barbara
Chris G. Sibley
Affiliation:
University of Auckland
Fiona Kate Barlow
Affiliation:
University of Queensland
Get access

Summary

As a man I pity you, but as an official I must show you no mercy; as a politician I regard him as an ally, but as a moralist I loathe him.

William James (1890/1983, p. 43)

Emotions, William James tells us, are embedded in identity. How I feel about you depends not only on who you are, but also on who I am. In an influential chapter merging theories of social identity and emotion felt toward other groups, Smith (1993) similarly argued that considering intergroup attitudes as a combination of appraisals, emotions, and action tendencies based in the perceiver's social identity could better explain shifting intergroup evaluations across contexts and could better predict differentiated intergroup outcomes than traditional models of prejudice.

In the more than 20 years since the publication of that work, intergroup emotions theory (Mackie, Devos, & Smith, 2000; Mackie, Maitner, & Smith, 2009; Mackie & Smith, 2002, 2015; Smith & Mackie, 2006, 2008; Smith, Seger, & Mackie, 2007) has grown and developed into a comprehensive theory of intergroup relations that provides a complementary perspective to traditional attitude models of prejudice. In this chapter, we rearticulate the central tenets of the theory and then focus on how social categorization and group identification – who I am – interact with context to influence individuals’ appraisals, emotions, and behavioral intentions. We then focus on how three discrete emotional reactions elicit different and specific negative intergroup responses, before focusing on implications of intergroup emotions theory for improving intergroup relations. We finish with a discussion of current directions.

Intergroup Emotions Theory

Intergroup emotions theory (IET) suggests that when a particular social identity is activated, individuals will interpret events in terms of their implications for that ingroup, rather than for the individual personally. The more individuals identify with their currently activated social identity, the more extremely they will appraise group-relevant events. Group-based appraisals then elicit specific emotions and action tendencies. Because they are rooted in concerns for the group, we refer to such emotions as group based (see Niedenthal & Brauer, 2012). Intergroup relations are dynamic and ongoing, and thus individuals are expected to continuously reappraise changing situations, and their emotions to wax and wane, or amplify and change, along with changes in the intergroup context.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Boiger, M., Güngӧr, D., Karasawa, M., & Mesquita, B. (2014). Defending honour, keeping face: Interpersonal affordances of anger and shame in Turkey and Japan. Cognition and Emotion, 28, 1255–1269. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.881324Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this “we”? Levels of collective identity and self representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 83–93. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.71.1.83Google Scholar
Buckels, E. E., & Trapnell, P. D. (2013). Disgust facilitates outgroup dehumanization. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 16, 771–780. doi: 10.1177/1368430212471738Google Scholar
Čahajić-Clancy, S., Effron, D., Halperin, E., Liberman, V., & Ross, L. (2011). Affirmation, acknowledgment of ingroup responsibility, group-based guilt, and support for reparative measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 256–270. doi: 10.1037/a0023936Google Scholar
Cheung-Blunden, V., & Blunden, B. (2008). The emotional construal of war: Anger, fear and other negative emotions. Peace and Conflict, 14, 123–149. doi:10.1080/10781910802017289Google Scholar
Crisp, R. J., Heuston, S., Farr, M. J., & Turner, R. (2007). Seeing red or feeling blue: Differentiated intergroup emotions and in-group identification in soccer fans. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 10, 9–26. doi:10.1177/1368430207071337Google Scholar
Crocker, J., Voelkl, K., Testa, M., & Major, B. (1991). Social stigma: The affective consequences of attributional ambiguity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 218–228. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.218Google Scholar
Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2007). The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 631–648. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.631Google Scholar
Dasgupta, N., DeSteno, D., Williams, L. A., & Hunsinger, M. (2009). Fanning the flames of prejudice: The influence of specific incidental emotions on implicit prejudice. Emotion, 9, 585–591. doi: 10.1037/a0015961Google Scholar
deVos, B., van Zomeren, M., Gordijn, E. H., & Postmes, T. (2013). The communication of “pure” group-based anger reduces tendencies toward intergroup conflict because it increases out-group empathy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 1043–1052. doi: 10.1177/0146167213489140Google Scholar
Dijker, A. J. (1987). Emotional reactions to ethnic minorities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 305–325. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420170306Google Scholar
Dion, K. L., & Earn, B. M. (1975). The phenomenology of being a target of prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 944–950. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.32.5.944Google Scholar
Doosje, B., Branscombe, N. R., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1998). Guilty by association: When one's group has a negative history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 872–886. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.4.872Google Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (1999). Reducing prejudice: Combating intergroup biases. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 101–105. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00024Google Scholar
Dumont, M., Yzerbyt, V., Wigboldus, D., & Gordijn, E. H. (2003). Social categorization and fear reactions to the September 11th terrorist attacks. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1509–1520. doi: 10.1177/0146167203256923Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212–228. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.2.212Google Scholar
Gaertner, S. L., Mann, J., Murrell, A., & Dovidio, J. F. (1989). Reducing intergroup bias: The benefits of recategorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 239–249. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.2.239Google Scholar
Gaertner, S. L. Rust, M. C., Dovidio, J. R., Bachman, B. A., & Anastasio, P. A. (1994). The contact hypothesis: The role of a common ingroup identity on reducing intergroup bias. Small Group Research, 25, 224–249. doi: 10.1177/1046496494252005Google Scholar
Giner-Sorolla, R., Castano, E., Espinosa, P., & Brown, R. (2007). Shame expressions reduce the recipient's insult from outgroup reparations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 519–526. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2007.08.003Google Scholar
Giner-Sorolla, R., & Maitner, A. T. (2013) Angry at the unjust, scared of the powerful: Emotional responses to terrorist threat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 1069–1082. doi: 10.1177/0146167213490803Google Scholar
Goldenberg, A., Saguy, T., & Halperin, E. (2014). How group-based emotions are shaped by collective emotions: Evidence for emotional transfer and emotional burden. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 581–596. doi: 10.1037/a0037462Google Scholar
Gordijn, E. H., Wigboldus, D., & Yzerbyt, V. (2001). Emotional consequences of categorizing victims of negative outgroup behavior as ingroup or outgroup. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 4, 317–326. doi: 10.1177/1368430201004004002Google Scholar
Gordijn, E. H., Yzerbyt, V., Wigboldus, D., & Dumont, M. (2006). Emotional reactions to harmful intergroup behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 15–30. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.296Google Scholar
Gurin, P. (1985). Women's gender consciousness. Public Opinion Quarterly, 49, 143–163. doi: 10.1086/268911Google Scholar
Halperin, E. (2008). Group-based hatred in intractable conflict in Israel. The Journal of Conflict Resolution, 52, 713–736. doi: 10.1177/0022002708314665Google Scholar
Halperin, E. (2014). Emotion, emotion regulation, and conflict resolution. Emotion Review, 6, 68–76. doi: 10.1177/1754073913491844Google Scholar
Halperin, E., Crisp, R. J., Husnu, S., Trzesniewski, K. H., Dweck, C., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Promoting intergroup contact by changing beliefs: Group malleability, intergroup anxiety, and contact motivation. Emotion, 12, 1192–1195. doi: 10.1037/a0028620Google Scholar
Halperin, E., & Gross, J. (2011a). Emotion regulation in violent conflict: Reappraisal, hope, and support for humanitarian aid to the opponent in wartime. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 1–9. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.536081Google Scholar
Halperin, E. & Gross, J. J. (2011b). Intergroup anger in intractable conflict: Long-term sentiments predict anger responses during the Gaza War. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 14, 477–488. doi: 10.1177/1368430210377459Google Scholar
Halperin, E., Porat, R., Tamir, M., & Gross, J. J. (2013). Can emotion regulation change political attitudes in intractable conflict? From the laboratory to the field. Psychological Science, 24, 106–111. doi: 10.1177/0956797612452572Google Scholar
Halperin, E., Proat, R., & Wohl, M. J. A. (2013). Extinction threat and reciprocal threat reduction: Collective angst predicts willingness to compromise in intractable intergroup conflicts. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 16, 797–813. doi: 10.1177/1368430213485994Google Scholar
Halperin, E., Russell, A. G., Dweck, C. S., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Anger, hatred, and the quest for peace: Anger can be constructive in the absence of hatred. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 55, 274–291. doi: 10.1177/0022002710383670Google Scholar
Harmon-Jones, E., Sigelman, J. D., Bohlig, A., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2003). Anger, coping, and frontal cortical activity: The effect of coping potential on anger-induced left frontal activity. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 1–24. doi: 10.1080/02699930302278Google Scholar
Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychological Science, 17, 847–853. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01793.xGoogle Scholar
Hastorf, A. H., & Cantril, H. (1954). They saw a game: a case study. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 49, 129–134. doi: 10.1037/h0057880Google Scholar
Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., & Bloom, P. (2012). Disgusting smells cause decreased liking of gay men. Emotion, 12, 23–27. doi: 10.1037/a0023984Google Scholar
James, W. (1983). Principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Original work published 1890.
Jetten, J., & Wohl, M. J. A. (2012). The past as a determinant of the present: Historical continuity, collective angst, and opposition to immigration. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 442–450. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.865Google Scholar
Johnson, B. M., & Glasford, D. E. (2014). A threat-emotion profile approach to explaining active versus passive harm in intergroup relations. Social Psychology, 45, 399–407. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000199Google Scholar
Kamans, E., Otten, S., & Gordijn, E. H. (2011). Power and threat in intergroup conflict. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 14, 293. doi:10.1177/1368430210372525Google Scholar
Kamans, E., van Zomeren, M., Gordijn, E. H., & Postmes, T. (2014). Communicating the right emotion makes violence seem less wrong: Power-congruent emotions lead outsiders to legitimize violence of powerless and powerful groups in intractable conflict. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 17, 286–305. doi: 10.1177/1368430213502562Google Scholar
Kessler, T., & Hollbach, S. (2005). Group-based emotion as determinants of ingroup identification. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 677–685. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.01.001Google Scholar
Kuppens, T., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2012). Group-based emotions: The impact of social identity on appraisals, emotions, and behaviors. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 34, 20–33. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2011.637474Google Scholar
Kuppens, T., Yzerbyt, V. Y., Dandache, S., Fischer, A. H., & van der Schalk, J. (2013). Social identity salience shapes group-based emotions through group-based appraisals. Cognition and Emotion 27, 1359–1377. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.785387Google Scholar
Kuppens, T., Pollet, T. V., Teixeira, C. P., Demoulin, S., Roberts, S. C., & Little, A. C. (2012). Emotions in context: Anger causes ethnic bias but not gender bias in men but not women. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 432–441. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1848Google Scholar
Leonard, D. J., Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (2011). Emotional responses to intergroup apology mediate intergroup forgiveness and retribution. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 1198–1206. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.05.002Google Scholar
Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischhoff, B. (2003). Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A national field experiment. Psychological Science, 14, 144–150. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01433Google Scholar
Leung, A. K.-Y., & Cohen, D. (2011). Within- and between-culture variation: Individual differences and the cultural logics of honor, face, and dignity cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 507–526. doi: 10.1037/a0022151Google Scholar
Levin, S., Pratto, F., Matthews, M., Sidanius, J., & Kteily, N. (2012). A dual process approach to understanding prejudice toward Americans in Lebanon: An extension to intergroup threat perceptions and emotions. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 16, 139–158. doi: 10.1177/1368430212443866Google Scholar
Livingstone, A. G., Spears, R., Manstead, A. S. R., & Bruder, M. (2009). Illegitimacy and identity threat in (inter)action: Predicting intergroup orientations among minority group members. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 755–775. doi: 10.1348/014466608X398591Google Scholar
Livingstone, A. G., Spears, R., Manstead, A. S. R., Bruder, M., & Shepherd, L. (2011). We feel, therefore we are: Emotion as a basis for self-categorization and social action. Emotion, 11, 754–767. doi: 10.1037/a0023223Google Scholar
Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions: Explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 602–616. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.602Google Scholar
Mackie, D. M., Maitner, A. T., & Smith, E. R. (2009). Intergroup emotion theory. In Nelson, T. D. (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 285–307). New York: Psychology Press.
Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (2002). Intergroup emotions and the social self: Prejudice reconceptualized as differentiated reactions to outgroups. In Forgas, J. P. & Williams, K. D. (Eds.), The social self: Cognitive, interpersonal, and intergroup perspectives (pp. 309–326). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (2015). Intergroup emotions. In Mikulincer, M. & Shaver, P. R. (Editors-in-chief), APA handbook of personality and social psychology: Vol. 2. Group Processes. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Maitner, A. T. (2007). Perceiving the world through group-colored glasses: Effects of self-categorization and group identification on attention and information processing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa Barbara.
Maitner, A., Mackie, D. M., Claypool, H. M., & Crisp, R. J. (2010). Identity salience moderates processing of group-relevant information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 441–444. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.11.010Google Scholar
Maitner, A. T., Mackie, D. M., Pauketat, J. V. T., & Smith, E. R. (2015). The impact of culture and identity on emotional reactions to insults. Unpublished manuscript.
Maitner, A. T., Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (2006). Evidence for the regulatory function of intergroup emotion: Implementing and impeding intergroup behavioral intentions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 720–728. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.08.001Google Scholar
Maitner, A. T., Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (2007). Antecedents and consequences of satisfaction and guilt following in-group aggression. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 10, 223–237. doi:10.1177/1368430207075154Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991) Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224Google Scholar
McCoy, S. K., & Major, B. (2003). Group identification moderates emotional responses to perceived prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1005–1017. doi: 10.1177/0146167203253466Google Scholar
Migdal, M. J., Hewstone, M., & Mullen, B. (1998). The effects of crossed categorization on intergroup evaluations: A meta-analysis. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 303–324. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1998.tb01174.xGoogle Scholar
Moons, W. G., Leonard, D. J., Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (2009). I feel our pain: Antecedents and consequences of emotional self-stereotyping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 760–769. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.04.016Google Scholar
Niedenthal, P. M., & Brauer, M. (2012). Social functionality of human emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 259–285. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131605Google Scholar
Ray, D. G., Mackie, D. M., Rydell, R. J., & Smith, E. R. (2008). Changing categorization of self can change emotions about out-groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1210–1213. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.03.014Google Scholar
Ray, D. G., Mackie, D. M., Smith, E. R., & Terman, A. W. (2012). Discrete emotions elucidate the effects of cross-categorization on prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 55–69. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.07.011Google Scholar
Rodriguez Mosquera, P. M., Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (2002). The role of honour concerns in emotional reactions to offences. Cognition & Emotion, 16, 143–163. doi: 10.1080/02699930143000167Google Scholar
Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotion: A structural theory. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 11–36.Google Scholar
Roseman, I. J., Antoniou, A. A., & Jose, P. E. (1996). Appraisal determinants of emotions: Constructing a more accurate and comprehensive theory. Cognition & Emotion, 10, 241–277. doi: 10.1080/026999396380240Google Scholar
Sadler, M. S., Lineberger, M., Correll, J., & Park, B. (2005). Emotions, attributions, and policy endorsement in response to the September 11th terrorist attacks. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 249–258. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp2703_6Google Scholar
Schmader, T. (2002). Gender identification moderates stereotype threat effects on women's math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 194–201. doi: 10.1006/jesp.2001.1500Google Scholar
Skitka, L. J., Bauman, C. W., Aramovich, N. P., & Morgan, G. C. (2006). Confrontational and preventative policy responses to terrorism: Anger wants a fight and fear wants “them” to go away. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28, 375–384. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp2804_11Google Scholar
Skitka, L. J., Bauman, C. W., & Mullen, E. (2004). Political tolerance and coming to psychological closure following the September 11, 2001 terrorist attacks: An integrative approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 743–756. doi: 10.1177/0146167204263968Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 233–269. doi: 10.1080/02699939308409189Google Scholar
Smith, E. R. (1993). Social identity and social emotions: Toward new conceptualizations of prejudice. In Mackie, D. M. & Hamilton, D. L. (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 297–315). San Diego: Academic Press.
Smith, E. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2006). It's about time: Intergroup emotions as time-dependent phenomena. In Capozza, D. & Brown, R. (Eds.), Social identities: Motivational, emotional, and cultural influences. New York: Psychology Press.
Smith, E. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2008). Intergroup emotions. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J., & Barrett, L. Feldman (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (ed., pp. 428–439). New York: Guilford Press.
Smith, E. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2015). Dynamics of group-based emotions: Insights from Intergroup Emotion Theory. Emotion Review, 7, 349–354. doi: 10.1177/1754073915590614Google Scholar
Smith, E. R., Seger, C. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2007). Can emotions be truly group-level? Evidence regarding four conceptual criteria. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 431–446. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.431Google Scholar
Smith, H. J., & Spears, R. (1996). Ability and outcome evaluations as a function of personal and collective (dis)advantage: A group escape from individual bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 690–704. doi: 10.1177/0146167296227004Google Scholar
Spanovic, M., Lickel, B., Denson, T. F., & Petrovic, N. (2010). Fear and anger as predictors of motivation for intergroup aggression: Evidence from Serbia and Republika Srpska. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13, 725–739. doi:10.1177/1368430210374483Google Scholar
Stephan, W. G. (2014). Intergroup anxiety: Theory, research, and practice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18, 239–255. doi: 10.1177/1088868314530518Google Scholar
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Tajfel, H., & Billig, M. (1974) Familiarity and categorization in intergroup behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 159–170. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(74)90064-XGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 149–178.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In Austin, W. & Worchel, S. (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago: Nelson Hall.
Tam, T., Hewstone, M., Cairns, E., Tausch, N., Maio, G., & Kenworthy, J. (2007). The impact of intergroup emotions on forgiveness in Northern Ireland. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 10, 119–136. doi:10.1177/1368430207071345Google Scholar
Tapias, M. P., Glaser, J., Keltner, D., Vasquez, K., & Wickens, T. (2007). Emotion and prejudice: Specific emotions toward outgroups. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 10, 27–39. doi: 10.1177/1368430207071338Google Scholar
Tausch, N., Becker, J. C., Spears, R., Christ, O., Saab, R., Singh, P., & Siddiqui, R. N. (2011). Explaining radical group behavior: Developing emotion and efficacy routes to normative and nonnormative collective action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 129–148. doi:10.1037/a0022728Google Scholar
Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Van Tongeren, D. R., Burnette, J. L., O'Boyle, E., Worthington, E. L. Jr., & Forsyth, D. R. (2014). A meta-analysis of intergroup forgiveness. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 9, 81–95. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2013.844268Google Scholar
van Zomeren, M., Spears, R., Fischer, A. H., & Leach, C. W. (2004). Put your money where your mouth is! Explaining collective action tendencies through group-based anger and group efficacy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 649–664. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.649Google Scholar
Vartanian, L. R., Thomas, M. A., & Vanman, E. J. (2013). Disgust, contempt, and anger and the stereotypes of obese people. Eat Weight Discord, 18, 377–382. doi: 10.1007/s40519-013-0067-2Google Scholar
Verkuyten, M., Drabbles, M., & van den Nieuwenhuijzen, K. (1999). Self-categorization and emotional reactions to ethnic minorities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 605–619.Google Scholar
Wenzel, M., & Mummendey, A. (1996). Positive-negative asymmetry of social discrimination: A normative analysis of differential evaluations of in-group and out-group on positive and negative attributes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 493–507. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1996.tb01110.xGoogle Scholar
Wohl, M. J. A., & Branscombe, N. R. (2009). Group threat, collective angst, and ingroup forgiveness for the war in Iraq. Political Psychology, 30, 193–217. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00688.xGoogle Scholar
Wohl, M. J. A., Branscombe, N. R., & Reysen, S. (2010) Perceiving your group's future to be in jeopardy: Extinction threat induces collective angst and the desire to strengthen the ingroup. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 898–910. doi: 10.1177/0146167210372505Google Scholar
Wohl, M. J. A., Hornsey, M. J., & Bennett, S. H. (2012). Why group apologies succeed and fail: Intergroup forgiveness and the role of primary and secondary emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 306–322. doi:10.1037/a0024838Google Scholar
Yzerbyt, V., Dumont, M., Wigboldus, D., & Gordijn, E. (2003). I feel for us: The impact of categorization and identification on emotions and action tendencies. British Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 533–549. doi: 10.1348/014466603322595266Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×