Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-767nl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-12T00:06:19.388Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - The Evolutionary Biology and Sociology of Social Order

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 December 2015

Jonathan H. Turner
Affiliation:
University of California, Riverside USA
Edward J. Lawler
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Shane R. Thye
Affiliation:
University of South Carolina
Jeongkoo Yoon
Affiliation:
EWHA Women's University, Seoul
Get access

Summary

Abstract

By examining the evolutionary biology of hominins and then humans, it is possible to construct more robust explanations for how dynamics at the micro level of social organization affect the integration of the micro, meso, and macro realms that constitute social reality. In this chapter, the evolution of hominins and eventually humans is reviewed with an eye to understanding how the pre-adaptations and behavioral propensities of hominins could be selected upon to create a more emotional animal, Homo sapiens, that was not locked into the group and kin structures of most mammals, with the result that humans have been able to build up solidarities at the group level that do not lock Homo sapiens into the micro social universe. Rather, the same emotional dynamic that generate micro-level solidarities can, under specified conditions, cause integration of the micro with the macro social universe composed of institutional domains, stratification systems, and societies. Evolutionary sociology is not just an esoteric program of theory and research but, rather, an approach that can shed new light on old theoretical problems.

Sociology emerged as a discipline to explain the dramatic changes associated with industrialization and urbanization. These new social orders appeared to be on the edge of chaos for many of those living through the tumultuous transition from agrarian to industrial social formations. Indeed, the first generations of sociologists in the nineteenth century posited a wide variety of “pathologies” inhering in the structure and culture of modern societies. In analyzing these transformative transitions, most early theorists employed an evolutionary approach, outlining the forces that had driven societies from simple to ever-larger and more complex patterns of social organization; and all had implicit conceptions of how these formations violated the fundamental nature of humans, thereby generating personal pathologies from alienation through egoism to marginality. Indeed, these are still the central problematics of much critical sociology and sociology more generally, but now we can approach questions of human nature and the relation of this nature to patterns of social organization with more sophisticated analytical tools that reduce speculation and downplay the often highly evaluative overtones of much sociological analysis of the human condition and society.

Type
Chapter
Information
Order on the Edge of Chaos
Social Psychology and the Problem of Social Order
, pp. 18 - 42
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baizer, J. S., Baker, J. F., Haas, K., and Lima, R.. 2007. “Neurochemical Organization of the Nucleus Paramedinaus Dorsalis in the Human.” Brain Research 1176: 45–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blau, P. M. 1977. Inequality and Heterogeneity. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Blau, P. M. 1994. Structural Context of Opportunities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boehm, C. 2012. Moral Origins: The Evolution of Virtue, Altruism, and Shame. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bronson, S. F., and de Waal, F. B. M.. 2003. “Fair Refusal by Capuchin Monkeys.Nature128–40.Google Scholar
Brosnan, Sarah F., Schiff, Hillary C., and de Waal, Frans B. M.. 2005. “Tolerance for Inequity May Increase with Social Closeness in Chimpanzees.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 272: 253–58.Google ScholarPubMed
Brosnan, Sarah F., Freeman, Cassie, and de Waal, Frans B. M.. 2006. “Capuchin Monkey's (Cebus apella) Reactions to Inequity in an Unrestricted Barpull Situation.” American Journal of Primatology 68: 713–724.Google Scholar
Burghardt, Gordon M. 2005. The Genesis of Animal Play: Testing the Limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Collins, Randall. 2004. Interaction Ritual Chains. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooley, Charles Horton. 1902. Human Nature and the Social Order. New York: Scribners.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. 1989. “The Logic of Social Exchange: Has Natural Selection Shaped How Humans Reason?Cognition 31: 187–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
deWaal, Frans B. M. 1989. “Food Sharing and Reciprocal Obligations among Chimpanzees.Journal of Human Evolution 18: 433–59.Google Scholar
deWaal, Frans B. M. 1991. “The Chimpanzee's Sense of Social Regularity and Its Relation to the Human Sense of Justice.American Behavioral Scientist 34: 335–49.Google Scholar
deWaal, Frans B. M. 1996. Good Natured: The Origins of Right and Wrong in Humans and Other Animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
deWaal, Frans B. M. 2009. The Age of Empathy: Nature's Lessons for a Kinder Society. New York: Three Rivers Press.Google Scholar
de Waal, Frans B. M., and Brosnon, Sarah F.. 2006. “Simple and Complex Reciprocity in Primates.” Pp. 85–106 in Cooperation in Primates and Humans: Mechanisms and Evolution, edited by Kappeler, P. and Schaik, C. P.van. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
de Waal, Frans B. M., and Brosnon, Sarah F.Evolution, edited by Kappeler, P. and van Schaik, C. P. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Durkheim, Emile. 1912 [1965]. The Elementary Forms of the Religious Life.New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. 1984. Expression and the Nature of Emotion.” Pp. 319–343 in Approaches to Emotion, edited by K. Scherer and P. Ekman. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Emde, Robert N. (1962). “Level of Meaning for Infant Emotions: A Biosocial View.” Pp. 1–37 in Development of Cognition, Affect and Social Relations, edited by Collins, W. A. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fisher, R. A. 1930. The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallup, G. G., Jr. 1970. “Chimpanzees: Self-Recognition.”Science 167: 88–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geschwind, N. 1979. Self-Recognition in Chimpanzees and Man: A Developmental and Comparative Perspective. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. 1982. “Self-Awareness and the Emergence of Mind in Primates.” American Journal of Primatology 2: 237–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G. and Csibra, G.. 2006. “Sylvia's Recipe: The Role of Imitation and Pedagogy.” Pp. 229–55 in The Transmission of Cultural Knowledge, edited by Enfield, N. J. and Levinson, S. C.. Oxford, UK: Berg Press.Google Scholar
Geschwind, N. 1965a. “Disconnection Syndromes in Animals and Man, Part I.Brain 88: 237–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geschwind, N. 1965b. “Disconnection Syndromes in Animals and Man, Part II.Brain 88: 585–644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geschwind, Norman, and Damasio, Antonio, 1984. “The Neural Basis of Language.Annual Review of Neuroscience 7: 127–47.Google Scholar
Goffman, Erving. 1961. Encounters: Two Studies in the Sociology of Interaction. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill.Google Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J., and Tomasello, M.. 2001. “Do Chimpanzees Know What Conspecifics Know?Animal Behavior 61: 139–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, B., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. 2006. “Chimpanzees Deceive a Human Competitor by Hiding.Cognition 101: 495–514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horowitz, A. C. 2003. “Do Chimps Ape? Or Apes Human? Imitation and Intension inn Humans (Homo sapiens) and Other Animals.Journal of Comparative Psychology 117: 325–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Itakura, S. 1996. “An Exploratory Study of Gaze-Monitoring in Non-Human Primates.Japanese Psychological Research 38: 174–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemper, Theodore D. 1987. “How Many Emotions Are There? Wedding the Social and the Autonomic Components.American Journal of Sociology 93: 263–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawler, Edward J. 2001. “An Affect Theory of Social Exchange.American Journal of Sociology 107: 321–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawler, Edward J., Thye, S., and Yoon, J.. 2009. Social Commitments in a Depersonalized World. New York: Russell Sage.Google Scholar
Luhmann, Niklas 1982. The Differentiation of Society. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Machalek, R. 1992. “Why Are Large Societies Rare?Advances in Human Ecology 1: 33–64.Google Scholar
Maryanski, Alexandra. 1986. “African Ape Social Structure: A Comparative Analysis.” Ph.D. diss., University of California.
Maryanski, Alexandra 1987. “African Ape Social Structure: Is There Strength in Weak Ties?Social Networks 9: 191–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maryanski, Alexandra 1992. “The Last Ancestor: An Ecological-Network Model on the Origins of Human Sociality.Advances in Human Ecology 2: 1–32.Google Scholar
Maryanski, Alexandra 1993. “The Elementary Forms of the First Proto-Human Society: An Ecological/Social Network Approach.Advances in Human Evolution 2: 215–41.Google Scholar
Maryanski, Alexandra 1996. “Was Speech an Evolutionary Afterthought?” In Communicating Meaning: The Evolution and Development of Language, edited by Velichikovsky, B. and Rumbaugh, D.. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Maryanski, A and Turner, J. H.. 1992. The Social Cage: Human Nature and The Evolution of Society. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Mead, George Herbert. 1934. Mind, Self, and Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Menzel, E. W. 1971. “Communication about the Environment in a Group of Young Chimpanzees.Folia Primatologica 15: 220–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, John and Watts, David. 2001. “Why Do Chimpanzees Hunt and Share Meet?Animal Behaviour 61: 915–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okamoto, S.Tomonaga, M., Ishii, K., Kawai, N., Tanaka, M., and Matsuzawa, T. 2002. “An Infant Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Follows Human Gaze.Animal Cognition 5: 107–14.Google ScholarPubMed
Osgood, Charles E. (1966). “Dimensionality of the Semantic Space for Communication via Facial Expressions.” Scandanavian Journal of Psychology, 7, 1–30.Google ScholarPubMed
Parsons, T. 1963a. “On the Concept of Political Power.” Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 107: 232–262.Google Scholar
Parsons, T. 1963b. “On the Concept of Influence.” Public Opinion Quarterly 27: 37–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plutchik, Robert 1980. Emotion: A Psychoevolutionary Synthesis. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. 2000. Folk Physics for Apes: The Chimpanzee's Theory of How the World Works. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. and Eddy, T. J.. 1997. “Specificity of Gaze-Following in Young Chimpanzees.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology 15: 213–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolattti, G. L. Fadiga, Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V.. 2002. “From Mirror Neurons to Imitation: Facts and Speculations.” Pp. 247–66 in The Imitative Mind: Devolopment, Evolution and Brain Bases, edited by Prinz, W. and Meltzoff, A. N.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rumbaugh, Duane, and Savage-Rumbaugh, E. Sue. 1990. “Chimpanzees: Competencies for Language and Numbers.” In Comparative Perception, vol. 2, edited by Stebbins, William and Berkley, Mark. New York: Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, Sue, and Lewin, Roger. 1994. Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Murphy, J., Seveik, J., Brakke, K., Williams, S. L., and Rumbaugh, D.. 1993. “Language Comprehension in the Ape and Child.” Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58. Chicago: University of Chicato Press.Google Scholar
Sherwood, Chet C., Holloway, R. L., Semendeferi, K., and Hoff, P. R.. 2005. “Is Prefrontal White Matter Enlargement a Human Evolutionary Specialization?”Nature Neuroscience 8: 537–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, Baldwin, and Gillen, Francis. 1899. The Nature Tribes of Central Australia (New York: Macmillan and Co.).Google Scholar
Stephan, H. 1983. “Evolutionary Trends in Limbic Structures.”Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review 7: 367–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, H., and Andy, O. J.. 1969. “Quantitative Comparative Neuroanatomy of Primates: An Attempt at Phylogenetic Interpretation.Annals of the New York Academy of Science 167: 370–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, H., and Andy, O. J. 1977. “Quantitative Comparison of the Amygdala in Insectivores and Primates.” Acta Antomica 98: 130–53.Google ScholarPubMed
Subiaul, F. 2007. “The Imitation Faculty in Monkeys: Evaluating Its Features, Distribution, and Evolution.Journal of Anthropological Science 85: 35–62.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, Hare, B., and Fogleman, T.. 2001. “The Ontegeny of Gaze Folling in Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, and Rhesus Macaques, Macaca mulatta.” Animal Behavior 61: 335–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomonaga, Michael. 1999. “Attending to the Others’ Attention in Macaques’ Joint Attention or Not?”Primate Research 15: 425.Google Scholar
Turner, Jonathan H. 2000. On the Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry into the Evolution of Human Affect. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Turner, Jonathan H. 2002. Face to Face: Toward a Theory of Interpersonal Behavior. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Turner, Jonathan H. 2007. Human Emotions: A Sociological Theory. Oxford, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Turner, Jonathan H. 2008. Human Emotions: A Sociological Theory. Oxford, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Turner, Jonathan H. 2010. Theoretical Principles of Sociology, Volume 2 on Microdynamics. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Turner, Jonathan H. and Maryanski, Alexandra. 2012. “The Biology and Neurology of Group Processes.” Advances in Group Processes 29: 1–37.
Turner, Jonathan H. and Maryanski, Alexandra 2015. “Evolutionary Sociology: A Cross-species Strategy for Discovering Human Nature.” Pp. 546–71 in Handbook of Evolution and Society: Toward an Evolutionary Social Science, edited by Turner, J. H., Machalek, R., and Maryanski, A. R.. Boulder, CO: Paradigm Press.Google Scholar
Turner, Jonathan H., and Stets, Jan E.. 2005. The Sociology of Emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×