Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-q6k6v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-10T15:25:04.663Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Churchland on Connectionism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Aarre Laakso
Affiliation:
Indiana University
Garrison W. Cottrell
Affiliation:
University of California
Brian L. Keeley
Affiliation:
Pitzer College, Claremont
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

Paul Churchland cemented his appointment as Ambassador of Connectionism to Philosophy with the 1986 publication of his paper “Some reductive strategies in cognitive neurobiology.” However, as Churchland tells the story in the preface to his collection of papers, A Neurocomputational Perspective, his relationship with connectionism began three years earlier, when he became acquainted with the model of the cerebellum put forward by Andras Pellionisz and Rodolfo Llinas (1979). The work of Pellionisz and Llinas foreshadows many of the arguments that Churchland makes. They argue that functions of the brain are represented in multidimensional spaces, that neural networks should therefore be treated as “geometrical objects” (323), and that “the internal language of the brain is vectorial” (330). The Pellionisz and Llinas paper also includes an argument for the superiority of neural network organization over von Neumann computer organization on the grounds that the network is more reliable and resistant to damage, a theme to which Churchland often returns.

Over the years, Churchland has applied connectionism to several areas of philosophy, notably: philosophy of mind, epistemology, philosophy of science, and ethics. Churchland's arguments in these areas have a common structure. First, he shows that the predominant positions in the field are (a) based on an assumption that the fundamental objects of study are propositions and logical inferences, and (b) have significant internal difficulties largely attributable to that assumption. Second, he presents a re-construal of the field based on connectionism, giving a “neurocomputational perspective” on the fundamental issues in the field.

Type
Chapter
Information
Paul Churchland , pp. 113 - 153
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abeles, M., Bergman, H., Margalit, E., & Vaadia, E. (1993). “Spatiotemporal firing patterns in the frontal cortex of behaving monkeys.” Journal of Neurophysiology 70(4): 1629–38CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashby, F. G., Boynton, G., & Lee, W. W. (1994). “Categorization response time with multidimensional stimuli.” Perception & Psychophysics 55(1): 11–27CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bair, W., & Koch, C. (1996). “Temporal precision of spike trains in extrastriate cortex of the behaving macaque monkey.” Neural Computation 8(6): 1185–202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ballard, D. H. (1986). Parallel logical inference and energy minimization. Proceedings of the 5th National Conference on Artificial Intelligence (AAAI-86) (Vol. 1, pp. 203–9). Philadelphia, Morgan KaufmannGoogle Scholar
Ballard, D. H. (1999). An introduction to natural computation. Cambridge, MA, MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (1985). “Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 11(1–4): 629–54Google ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W (1991). Deriving categories to acheive goals. Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, (Vol. 27, pp. 1–64). San Diego, Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D . (1995). Language and human behavior. Seattle, University of Washington PressGoogle Scholar
Bishop, C. M. (1995). Neural networks for pattern recognition. Oxford, Clarendon PressGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1986). “Some reductive strategies in cognitive neurobiology.” Mind 95(379): 279–309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1988). Folk psychology and the explanation of human behavior. A neurocomputational perspective: The nature of mind and the structure of science (pp. 111–35). Cambridge, MA, MIT Press/Bradford BooksGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1989a). Learning and conceptual change. A neurocomputational perspective: The nature of mind and the structure of science (pp. 231–53). Cambridge, MA, MIT Press/Bradford BooksGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1989b). Moral facts and moral knowledge. A neurocomputational perspective: The nature of mind and the structure of science (pp. 297–303). Cambridge, MA, MIT Press/Bradford BooksGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1989c). A neurocomputational perspective: The nature of mind and the structure of science. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press/Bradford BooksGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1989d). On the nature of explanation: A PDP approach. A neurocomputational perspective: The nature of mind and the structure of science (pp. 197–230). Cambridge, MA, MIT Press/Bradford BooksGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1989e). Preface. A neurocomputational perspective: The nature of mind and the structure of science (pp. ⅺ–ⅹⅶ). Cambridge, MA, MIT Press/Bradford BooksGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1990). On the nature of theories: A neurocomputational perspective. Savage, C. W. (Ed.), Scientific theories (Vol. 14). Minneapolis, University of Minneapolis PressGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1995). The engine of reason, the seat of the soul: A philosophical journey into the brain. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press/Bradford BooksGoogle Scholar
Cottrell, G. (1985). Parallelism in inheritance hierarchies with exceptions. Proceedings of the Eighth International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Los Angeles, CAGoogle Scholar
Cottrell, G. W., Bartell, B., & Haupt, C. (1990). Grounding meaning in perception. Marburger, H. (Ed.), Proceedings of the German Workshop on Artificial Intelligence (GWAI) (pp. 307–21). Berlin, Springer-VerlagCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derthick, M. A. (1987). A connectionist architecture for representing and reasoning about structured knowledge. Proceedings of the Ninth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 131–42). Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
Elman, J. L. (1991). “Distributed representations, simple recurrent networks, and grammatical structure.” Machine Learning 7: 195–225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Gerstner, W. (2001). What's different with spiking neurons? Mastebroek, H. & Vos, H. (Eds.), Plausible neural networks for biological modeling (pp. 23–48). Boston: KluwerCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerstner, W ., & Kistler, W. M. (2002). Spiking neuron models: Single neurons, populations, plasticity. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilmore, G. C., Hersh, H., Caramazza, A., & Griffin, J. (1979). “Multidimensional letter similarity derived from recognition errors.” Perception and Psychophysics, 25: 425–31CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glymour, C. (2001). The mind's arrows: Bayes nets and graphical causal models in psychology. Cambridge, MA, MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Goldstone, R. L. (1994). “The role of similarity in categorization: providing a groundwork.” Cognition 52: 125–57CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstone, R. L., & Son, J. (2005). Similarity. Holyoak, K. & Morrison, R. (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press
Gorman, R. P., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1988). “Analysis of hidden units in a layered network trained to classify sonar targets.” Neural Networks 1: 75–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hahn, U., Chater, N., & Richardson, L. B. (2002). “Similarity as transformation.” Cognition, 87: 1–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertz, J., Krogh, A., & Palmer, R. G. (1991). Introduction to the theory of neural computation. New York, Addison-WesleyGoogle Scholar
Holyoak, K. J., & Gordon, P. C. (1983). “Social reference points.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44: 881–7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopfield, J. J., & Brody, C. D. (2001). “What is a moment? Transient synchrony as a collective mechanism for spatiotemporal integration.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98(3)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krumhansl, C. L. (1978). “Concerning the applicability of geometric models to similarity data: The interrelationship between similarity and spatial density.” Psychological Review 85: 450–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago, University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
Laakso, A., & Cottrell, G. W. (2000). “Content and cluster analysis: Assessing representational similarity in neural systems.” Philosophical Psychology 13(1): 77–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maass, W. (1998). On the role of time and space in neural computation. Proceedings of the Federated Conference of CLS'98 and MFCS'98 (Vol. 1450, pp. 72–83). Berlin: SpringerGoogle Scholar
Malsburg, C. von. (1995). “Binding in models of perception and brain function.” Current Opinion in Neurobiology 5: 520–6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosofsky, R. M. (1991). “Stimulus bias, asymmetric similarity, and classification.” Cognitive Psychology 23: 94–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmeria, T. J., & Nosofsky, R. M. (2001). “Central tendencies, extreme points, and prototype enhancment effects in ill-defined perceptual categorization.” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology 54A(1): 197–235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pellionisz, A., & Llinas, R. (1979). “Brain modeling by tensor network theory and computer simulation. The cerebellum: Distributed processor for predictive coordination.” Neuroscience 4: 323–48CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plate, T. A. (1995). “Holographic reduced representations.” IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks 6(3): 623CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Podgorny, P., & Garner, W. R. (1979). “Reaction time as a measure of inter-intraobject visual similarity: Letters of the alphabet.” Perception and Pyschophysics 26(1): 37–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollack, J. B. (1990). “Recursive distributed representations.” Artificial Intelligence 46(1–2): 77–105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quine, W. V. O. (1951). “Two dogmas of empiricism.” Philosophical Review 60: 20–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rao, R. P. N., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2001). “Spike-timing-dependent Hebbian placticity as temporal difference learning.” Neural Computation 13(10): 2221–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). “Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories.” Cognitive Psychology 7(4): 573–605CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, S. J., & Norvig, P. (2003). Artificial intelligence: A modern approach (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ, Prentice-HallGoogle Scholar
Schlimmer, J. S. (1987a). Concept acquisition through representational adjustment. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, IrvineGoogle Scholar
Schlimmer, J. S (1987b). Mushrooms dataset. The UCI Machine Learning Repository. (Retrieved August 11, 2004, from ftp://ftp.ics.uci.edu/pub/machine-learning-databases/mushroom)
Sejnowski, T. J., & Rosenberg, C. R. (1987). “NETtalk: Parellel networks that learn to pronounce english text.” Complex Systems 1, 145–68Google Scholar
Shastri, L., & Ajjanagadde, V. (1993). “From simple associations to systematic reasoning: A connectionist representation of rules, variables and dynamic bindings using temporal synchrony.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 417–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shon, A. P., Rao, R. P. N., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2004). “Motion detection and prediction through spike-timing dependent plasticity.” Network: Computation in Neural Systems 15: 179–98CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, W., & Gray, C. M. (1995). “Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis.” Annual Review of Neuroscience 18: 555–86CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smolensky, P. (1990). “Tensor product variable binding and the representation of symbolic structures in connectionist systems.” Artificial Intelligence 46(1–2): 159–216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tesar, B., & Smolensky, P. (1994). Synchronous-firing variable binding is spatio-temporal tensor product representation. Ram, A. & Eiselt, K. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 16th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
Thorpe, S., Fize, D., & Marlot, C. (1996). “Speed of processing in the human visual system.” Nature 381: 520–2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Touretzky, D. S. (1990). “BoltzCONS: dynamic symbol structures in a connectionist network.” Artificial Intelligence 46: 5–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Touretzky, D. S., & Hinton, G. E. (1985). Symbols among the neurons: Details of a connectionist inference architecture. Proceedings of the 9th International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence (IJCAI 85) (pp. 238–43). San Mateo, CA, Morgan KaufmannGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A. (1977). “Features of similarity.” Psychological Review 84(4): 327–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., & Gati, I. (1978). Studies of similarity. E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.). Cognition and categorization (pp. 79–98). Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., & Hutchinson, J. W. (1986). “Nearest-neighbor analysis of psychological spaces.” Psychological Review 93: 3–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×