Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-hfldf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T10:41:59.916Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

IV - The structure of ritual action

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 November 2011

Edited by
Get access

Summary

The classical study of ritual centres on the question of the connections between features of ritual action, on the one hand, and various religious representations, on the other. The connections are supposed to go in both directions. On the one hand, rituals are described as contexts in which particular religious models are ‘expressed’, displayed, made manifest in actions and statements (see e.g. Beattie 1970). Conversely, it is generally accepted that participants in ritual make use of various shared cultural models to give ‘meaning’ to their actions. Both assumptions, however, are generally couched in such vague terms that they do not seem to have any practical implications for the study of religious ritual. The first hypothesis, which could be called the ‘expressive’ understanding of ritual, seems to be mostly a projection of the anthropologists’ own methodologies. Because anthropologists consider rituals as a primary source of information about the religious representations of a given group, they spontaneously tend to consider that that is the way in which the participants too represent their rituals. This, however, is not really supported by the evidence. The hypothesis is all the more difficult to maintain, as the particular aspects of ritual contexts make communication and therefore ‘expression’ particularly awkward. As Bloch pointed out (1974 passim; 1985: 19–45), religious ritual makes constant use of behavioural modes (dance, special languages, stereotyped gestures) which, if anything, reduce the range of information that can be conveyed by their performance. In other words, rituals are remarkable in that they are particularly nonexpressive, as other authors have stressed (Rappaport 1974; Staal 1979).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×