Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-sjtt6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-28T11:32:12.606Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Models of Change and Continuity in Romantic Experiences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 December 2010

Frank D. Fincham
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Ming Cui
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Get access

Summary

The development of romantic relationships commonly begins in adolescence and continues to unfold over the life course. Over the course of time, a romantic relationship may take on new characteristics, acquire new meaning, and serve new functions (Furman & Collins, 2009). For example, the attachment and caregiving behavioral systems typically become more salient as the relationship develops (Furman & Wehner, 1994).

Of particular importance are the formative experiences and developmental processes that occur during emerging adulthood. The salience of romantic relationships increases across these years, rivaling and sometimes surpassing those with parents and eclipsing those with friends (Laursen & Williams, 1997). Many emerging adults face decisions about long-term commitment, including cohabitation and marriage.

Not only do particular romantic relationships change and develop but also changes can occur across relationships. Emerging adulthood is often a time of exploration, and many persons during this period have a series of different romantic relationships. As relationship experiences accumulate, patterned styles of interacting with romantic partners may emerge or change (Laursen & Jensen-Campbell, 1999). These developmental processes are complicated and multiple in nature. One of the key questions social scientists face is how best to capture the changes and development that occur in romantic relationships and experiences in emerging adulthood.

Over the last two decades, statisticians have developed a range of different statistical procedures to analyze change and development. Traditionally, autoregressive models were the method of choice with longitudinal data.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bollen, K. A., & Curran, P. J. (2004). Autoregressive latent trajectory (ALT) models: A synthesis of two traditions. Sociological Methods & Research, 32, 336–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burchinal, M., & Appelbaum, M. I. (1991). Estimating individual developmental functions: Methods and their assumptions. Child Development, 62, 23–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, D. A., Martin, N. C., & Steiger, J. H. (2005). Empirical and conceptual problems with longitudinal trait-state models: Introducing a trait-state-occasion model. Psychological Methods, 10, 3–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curran, P. J., & Bollen, K. A. (2001). The best of both worlds: Combining autoregressive and latent curve models. In Collins, L. M. & Sayer, A. G. (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 107–135). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, T. E., Duncan, S. C., Strycker, L. A., Li, F., & Alpert, A. (1999). An introduction to latent variable growth curve modeling. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ferrer, E., & McArdle, J. J. (2003). Alternative structural models for multivariate longitudinal data analysis. Structural Equation Modeling, 10, 493–524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furman, W., & Collins, W. A. (2008). Adolescent romantic relationships and experiences. In Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W., & Laursen, B. (Eds.), Peer interactions, relationships, and groups (pp. 341–360). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Furman, W., Low, S., & Ho, M. (2009). Romantic experience and psychosocial adjustment in middle adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 38, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furman, W., & Wehner, E. (1994). Romantic views: Toward a theory of adolescent romantic relationships. In Montemayor, R., Adams, G. R., & Gullota, T. P. (Eds.), Advances in adolescent development: Personal relationships during adolescence (Vol. 6, pp. 168–195). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Grimm, K. J. (2007). Multivariate longitudinal methods for studying developmental relationships between depression and academic achievement. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 328–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grimm, K. J. (2008). Longitudinal associations between reading and mathematics achievement. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 410–426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harter, S. (1988). The Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents. Unpublished manual, University of Denver, Denver.
Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: A review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 3–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenny, D. A., & Zautra, A. (1995). The trait-state-error model for multiwave data. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 52–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kline, R. B. (2004). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Laursen, B., & Jensen-Campbell, L. A. (1999). The nature and functions of social exchange in adolescent romantic relationships. In Furman, W., Brown, B. B., & Feiring, C. (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on adolescent romantic relationships (pp. 50–74). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laursen, B., Popp, D., Burk, W. J., Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2008). Incorporating interdependence into developmental research: Examples from the study of homophily and homogeneity. In Card, N. A., Selig, J. P., & Little, T. D. (Eds.), Modeling dyadic and interdependent data in the developmental and behavioral sciences (pp. 11–37). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Laursen, B., & Williams, V. A. (1997). Perceptions of interdependence and closeness in family and peer relationships among adolescents with and without romantic partners. In Shulman, S. & Collins, W. A. (Eds.), Romantic relationships in adolescence: Developmental perspectives: New directions for child development (Vol. 78, pp. 3–20). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
McArdle, J. J., & Hamagami, F. (2001). Latent difference score structural models for linear dynamic analyses with incomplete longitudinal data. In Collins, L. & Sayer, A. (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 139–175). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagin, D. S. (1999). Analyzing developmental trajectories: A semiparametric, group-based approach. Psychological Methods, 4, 139–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norton, R. (1983). Measuring marital quality: A critical look at the dependent variable. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 141–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popp, D., Laursen, B., Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Burk, W. J. (2008). Modeling homophily over time with an Actor-Partner Interdependence Model. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1028–1039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ram, N., & Grimm, K. (2007). Using simple and complex growth models to articulate developmental change: Matching theory to method. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 303–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×