Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-m42fx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-19T11:30:43.765Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 December 2009

Friedemann Pulvermüller
Affiliation:
Medical Research Council, Cambridge
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
The Neuroscience of Language
On Brain Circuits of Words and Serial Order
, pp. 277 - 296
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abeles, M. (1991). Corticonics – Neural circuits of the cerebral cortex. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Abeles, M., Bergman, H., Gat, I., Meilijson, I., Seidemann, E., Tishby, N., & Vaadia, E. (1995). Cortical activity flips among quasi-stationary states. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 92, 8616–20CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abeles, M., Bergman, H., Margalit, E., & Vaadia, E. (1993). Spatiotemporal firing patterns in the frontal cortex of behaving monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 70, 1629–38CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aboitiz, F., Scheibel, A. B., Fisher, R. S., & Zaidel, E. (1992). Fiber composition of the human corpus callosum. Brain Research, 598, 143–53CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahissar, E., Vaadia, E., Ahissar, M., Bergman, H., Arieli, A., & Abeles, M. (1992). Dependence of cortical plasticity on correlated activity of single neurons and on behavior context. Science, 257, 1412–15CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ajukiewicz, K. (1936). Die syntaktische Konnexität. Studia Philosophica, 1, 1–27Google Scholar
Angrilli, A., Dobel, C., Rockstroh, B., Stegagno, L., & Elbert, T. (2000). EEG brain mapping of phonological and semantic tasks in Italian and German languages. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111, 706–16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Assadollahi, R., & Pulvermüller, F. (2001). Neuromagnetic evidence for early access to cognitive representations. Neuroreport, 12, 207–13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bach, E., Brown, C., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (1986). Crossed and nested dependencies in German and Dutch: a psycholinguistic study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 249–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bak, T. H., O'Donovan, D. G., Xuereb, J. H., Boniface, S., & Hodges, J. R. (2001). Selective impairment of verb processing associated with pathological changes in Brodmann areas 44 and 45 in the motor neuron disease–dementia–aphasia syndrome. Brain, 124, 103–20CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, G. P., & Hacker, P. M. S. (1984). Language, sense and nonsense. Oxford: Basil Blackwell
Bar-Hillel, Y., Perles, M., & Shamir, E. (1961). On formal properties of simple phrase structure grammars. Zeitschrift für Phonetik, Sprachwissenschaft und Kommunikationsforschung, 14, 143–72Google Scholar
Barlow, H. (1972). Single units and cognition: a neurone doctrine for perceptual psychology. Perception, 1, 371–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlow, H. B., Hill, R. M., & Levick, W. R. (1964). Retinal ganglion cells responding selectively to direction and speed of image motion in the rabbit. Journal of Physiology, 173, 377–407CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barlow, H., & Levick, W. R. (1965). The mechanism of directionally selective units in rabbit's retina. Journal of Physiology, 178, 477–504CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bavelier, D., Corina, D. P., & Neville, H. J. (1998). Brain and language: a perspective from sign language. Neuron, 21, 275–8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benson, D. F. (1979). Neurologic correlates of anomia. In H. A. Whitaker & H. Whitaker (Eds.), Studies in neurolinguistics. Vol. 4. New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Bienenstock, E. (1996). On the dimensionality of cortical graphs. Journal of Physiology, Paris, 90(3–4), 251–6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, H., Lambon-Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). The rise and fall of frequency and imageability: noun and verb production in semantic dementia. Brain and Language, 73, 17–49CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, J. K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K., Loebell, H., & Morey, R. (1992). From conceptual roles to structural relations: bridging the syntactic cleft. Psychological Review, 99, 150-71CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braitenberg, V. (1971). On the texture of brains. Heidelberg: Springer
Braitenberg, V. (1978a). Cell assemblies in the cerebral cortex. In R. Heim & G. Palm (Eds.), Theoretical approaches to complex systems. (Lecture notes in biomathematics, vol. 21) (pp. 171–88). Berlin: Springer
Braitenberg, V. (1978b). Cortical architectonics: general and areal. In M. A. B. Brazier & H. Petsche (Eds.), Architectonics of the cerebral cortex (pp. 443–65). New York: Raven Press
Braitenberg, V. (1980). Alcune considerazione sui meccanismi cerebrali del linguaggio. In G. Braga & V. Braitenberg & C. Cipolli & E. Coseriu & S. Crespi-Reghizzi & J. Mehler & R. Titone (Eds.), L'accostamento interdisciplinare allo studio del linguaggio (pp. 96–108). Milano: Franco Angeli Editore
Braitenberg, V. (1996). Il gusto della lingua. Meccanismi cerebrali e strutture grammaticali. Merano: Alpha & Beta
Braitenberg, V., Heck, D., & Sultan, F. (1997). The detection and generation of sequences as a key to cerebellar function: experiments and theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 229–45CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braitenberg, V., & Pulvermüller, F. (1992). Entwurf einer neurologischen Theorie der Sprache. Naturwissenschaften, 79, 103–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braitenberg, V., & Schüz, A. (1998). Cortex: statistics and geometry of neuronal connectivity (2 ed.). Berlin: SpringerCrossRef
Brent, M. R., & Cartwright, T. A. (1996). Distributional regularity and phonotactic constraints are useful for segmentation. Cognition, 61(1–2), 93–125CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bressler, S. L. (1995). Large-scale cortical networks and cognition. Brain Research Review, 20, 288–304CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bressler, S. L., & Freeman, W. J. (1980). Frequency analysis of olfactory system EEG in cat, rabbit and rat. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 50, 19–24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broca, P. (1861). Remarques sur la siège de la faculté de la parole articulée, suivies d'une observation d'aphémie (perte de parole). Bulletin de la Société d'Anatomie, 36, 330–57Google Scholar
Brodmann, K. (1909). Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Groβhirnrinde. Leipzig: Barth
Brown, C. M., Hagoort, P., & Keurs, M. (1999). Electrophysiological signatures of visual lexical processing: open- and closed-class words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 261–81CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, T. H., Kairiss, E. W., & Keeman, C. L. (1996). Hebbian synapses: biophysical mechanisms and algorithms. Review in Neuroscience, 13, 475–511CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, W. S., & Lehmann, D. (1979). Verb and noun meaning of homophone words activate different cortical generators: a topographic study of evoked potential fields. Experimental Brain Research, 2, s159–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryden, M. P., Hecaen, H., & DeAgostini, M. (1983). Patterns of cerebral organization. Brain and Language, 20(2), 249–62CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buonomano, D. V. (2000). Decoding temporal information: A model based on short-term synaptic plasticity. J Neuroscience, 20, 1129–41CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buonomano, D. V., & Merzenich, M. M. (1998). Cortical plasticity: from synapses to maps. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 21, 149–86CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caplan, D. (1987). Neurolinguistics and linguistic aphasiology. An introduction. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Caplan, D. (1996). Language: structure, processing, and disorders. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Cappa, S. F., Binetti, G., Pezzini, A., Padovani, A., Rozzini, L., & Trabucchi, M. (1998). Object and action naming in Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology, 50(2), 351–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caramazza, A., & Zurif, E. B. (1976). Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in sentence comprehension: evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 3, 572–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charniak, E. (1993). Statistical language learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Cheour, M., Ceponiene, R., Lehtokoski, A., Luuk, A., Allik, J., Alho, K., & Näätänen, R. (1998). Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 351–3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton
Chomsky, N. (1963). Formal properties of grammars. In R. D. Luce & R. R. Bush & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology, Vol. 2 (pp. 323–418). New York, London: Wiley
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press
Chomsky, N. (2000). New horizons in the study of language and mind. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press
Creutzfeldt, O., Ojemann, G., & Lettich, E. (1989). Neuronal activity in the human lateral temporal lobe. I. Responses to speech. Experimental Brain Research, 77, 451–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cruse, H., Bartling, C., Cymbalyuk, G., Dean, J., & Dreifert, M. (1995). A modular artificial neural net for controlling a six-legged walking system. Biological Cybernetics, 72, 421–30CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cruse, H., & Bruwer, M. (1987). The human arm as a redundant manipulator: the control of path and joint angles. Biological Cybernetics, 57, 137–44CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, A. M., Liu, A. K., Fischl, B. R., Buckner, R. L., Belliveau, J. W., Lewine, J. D., & Halgren, E. (2000). Dynamic statistical parametric mapping: combining fMRI and MEG for high-resolution imaging of cortical activity. Neuron, 26(1), 55–67CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. R. (1989). The brain binds entities and events by multiregional activation from convergence zones. Neural Computation, 1, 123–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, H., Grabowski, T. J., Tranel, D., Hichwa, R. D., & Damasio, A. R. (1996). A neural basis for lexical retrieval. Nature, 380, 499–505CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. R., & Tranel, D. (1993). Nouns and verbs are retrieved with differently distributed neural systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 90, 4957–60CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniele, A., Giustolisi, L., Silveri, M. C., Colosimo, C., & Gainotti, G. (1994). Evidence for a possible neuroanatomical basis for lexical processing of nouns and verbs. Neuropsychologia, 32, 1325–41CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deacon, T. W. (1992). Cortical connections of the inferior arcuate sulcus cortex in the macaque brain. Brain Research!, 573(1), 8–26CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeFelipe, J., & Farinas, I. (1992). The pyramidal neuron of the cerebral cortex: morphological and chemical characteristics of the synaptic inputs. Progress in Neurobiology, 39(6), 563–607CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., & Nadal, J. P. (1987). Neural networks that learn temporal sequences by selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 84, 2727–31CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Dehaene, S. (1994). Speed and cerebral correlates of syllable discrimination in infants. Nature, 370, 292–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retreival in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283–321CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Saffran, E. M., & Gagnon, D. A. (1997). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. Psychological Review, 104(4), 801–38CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Renzi, E., & Vignolo, L. (1962). The Token Test: a sensitive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics. Brain, 85, 665–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Saussure, F. (1916). Cours de linguistique generale. Paris: Payot
Devlin, J. T., Russell, R. P., Davis, M. H., Price, C. J., Moss, H. E., Fadili, M. J., & Tyler, L. K. (2002). Is there an anatomical basis for category-specificity? Semantic memory studies in PET and fMRI. Neuropsychologia, 40, 54–75CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diesch, E., Biermann, S., & Luce, T. (1998). The magnetic mismatch field elicited by words and phonological non-words. NeuroReport, 9, 455–60CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diesmann, M., Gewaltig, M. O., & Aertsen, A. (1999). Stable propagation of synchronous spiking in cortical neural networks. Nature, 402(6761), 529–33CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dilthey, W. (1989). Einleitung in die Geisteswissenschaften [Introduction to the human sciences], Vol. 1. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press
Dobel, C., Pulvermüller, F., Härle, M., Cohen, R., Kobbel, P., Schönle, P. W., & Rockstroh, B. (2001). Syntactic and semantic processing in the healthy and aphasic human brain. Experimental Brain Research, 140, 77–85CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egelhaaf, M., Borst, A., & Reichardt, W. (1989). Computational structure of a biological motion-detection system as revealed by local detector analysis in the fly's nervous system. Journal of the Optical Society of America (A), 6, 1070–87CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, P. (1999). Grundriss der deutschen Grammatik: Der Satz, Vol. 2. Stuttgart: Verlag J. B. MetzlerCrossRef
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., & Taub, E. (1995). Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science, 270(5234), 305–7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, A. W., & Young, A. W. (1988). Human cognitive neuropsychology. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd
Elman, J. L. (1990). Finding structure in time. Cognitive Science, 14, 179–211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J. L., Bates, L., Johnson, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: a connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Engert, F., & Bonhoeffer, T. (1999). Dendritic spine changes associated with hippocampal long-term synaptic plasticity. Nature, 399(6731), 66–70CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, H. T. (1999). Other brain effects of words. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 287–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eulitz, C., Eulitz, H., Maess, B., Cohen, R., Pantev, C., & Elbert, T. (2000). Magnetic brain activity evoked and induced by visually presented words and nonverbal stimuli. Psychophysiology, 37(4), 447–55CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J., & McClelland, J. L. (1991). A computational model of semantic memory impairment: modality specificity and emergent category specificity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 339–57CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Federmeier, K. D., Segal, J. B., Lombrozo, T., & Kutas, M. (2000). Brain responses to nouns, verbs and class-ambiguous words in context. Brain, 123, 2552–66CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiez, J. A., & Petersen, S. E. (1998). Neuroimaging studies of word reading. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 95(3), 914–21CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiez, J. A., Raichle, M. E., Balota, D. A., Tallal, P., & Petersen, S. E. (1996). PET activation of posterior temporal regions during auditory word presentation and verb generation. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 1–10CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, W. J. (1975). Mass action in the nervous system. New York: Academic Press
Freud, S. (1891). Zur Auffassung der Aphasien. Leipzig, Wien: Franz Deuticke
Friederici, A., & Mecklinger, A. (1996). Syntactic parsing as revealed by brain responses: first pass and second pass parsing processes. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25, 157–76CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friederici, A. D. (1997). Neurophysiological aspects of language processing. Clinical Neuroscience, 4, 64–72Google ScholarPubMed
Friederici, A. D., Pfeifer, E., & Hahne, A. (1993). Event-related brain potentials during natural speech processing: effects of semantic, morphological and syntactic violations. Cognitive Brain Research, 1, 183–92CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fry, D. B. (1966). The development of the phonological system in the normal and deaf child. In F. Smith & G. A. Miller (Eds.), The genesis of language (pp. 187–206). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Fuster, J. M. (1995). Memory in the cerebral cortex. An empirical approach to neural networks in the human and nonhuman primate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Fuster, J. M. (1997). Network memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 20, 451–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuster, J. M. (1998a). Distributed memory for both short and long term. Neurobiology in Learning and Memory, 70(1–2), 268–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuster, J. M. (1998b). Linkage at the top. Neuron, 21(6), 1223–4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuster, J. M. (1999). Hebb's other postulate at work on words. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 288–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuster, J. M. (2000). Cortical dynamics of memory. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 35(2–3), 155–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuster, J. M., & Alexander, G. E. (1971). Neuron activity related to short-term memory. Science, 173(997), 652–4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuster, J. M., Bodner, M., & Kroger, J. K. (2000). Cross-modal and cross-temporal association in neurons of frontal cortex. Nature, 405(6784), 347–51CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuster, J. M., & Jervey, J. P. (1982). Neuronal firing in the inferiotemporal cortex of the monkey in a visual memory task. Journal of Neuroscience, 2, 361–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaifman, C. (1965). Dependency systems and phrase structure systems. Information and Control, 8, 304–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galuske, R. A., Schlote, W., Bratzke, H., & Singer, W. (2000). Interhemispheric asymmetries of the modular structure in human temporal cortex. Science, 289(5486), 1946–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrett, M. (1980). Levels of processing in sentence production. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language Production I (pp. 177–220). London: Academic Press
Gazdar, G., Klein, E., Pullum, G., & Sag, I. (1985). Generalized phrase structure grammar. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Geschwind, N. (1965a). Disconnection syndromes in animals and man (1). Brain, 88, 237–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geschwind, N. (1965b). Disconnection syndromes in animals and man (2). Brain, 88, 585–644CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geschwind, N. (1974). Selected papers on language and the brain. Dordrecht: Kluewer
Geschwind, N., & Levitsky, W. (1968). Human brain: left–right asymmetries in temporal speech region. Science, 161, 186–7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glaser, W. R., & Düngelhoff, F. J. (1984). The time course of picture–word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 640–54Google ScholarPubMed
Goodglass, H., & Quadfasel, F. A. (1954). Language laterality in left-handed aphasics. Brain, 77, 521–48CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grabowski, T. J., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1998). Premotor and prefrontal correlates of category-related lexical retrieval. Neuroimage, 7, 232-43CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunter, T. C., Friederici, A. D., & Schriefers, H. (2000). Syntactic gender and semantic expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(4), 556–68CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haegeman, L. (1991). Introduction to government and binding theory. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell
Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP-measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 439–483CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagoort, P., Indefrey, P., Brown, C., Herzog, H., Steinmetz, H., & Seitz, R. J. (1999). The neural circuitry involved in the reading of German words and pseudowords: a PET study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(4), 383–98CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D. (1999). Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis. Early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(2), 194–205CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, M., Elman, J. L., & Daugherty, K. G. (1995). Default generalisation in connectionist networks. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 601–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Z. S. (1945). Discontinuous morphemes. Language, 21, 121–7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Z. S. (1951). Structural linguistics. Chicago: Chicago University Press
Harris, Z. S. (1952). Discourse analysis. Language, 28, 1–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Z. S. (1955). From phonemes to morphemes. Language, 31, 190–222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Z. S. (1957). Co-occurrence and transformation in linguistic structure. Language, 33, 283–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Z. S. (1965). Transformational theory. Language, 41, 363–401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasbrooke, R. E., & Chiarello, C. (1998). Bihemispheric processing of redundant bilateral lexical information. Neuropsychology, 12, 78–94CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauk, O., & Pulvermüller, F. (2002). Neurophysiological distinction of action words in the frontal lobe: an ERP study using minimum current estimates
Hayes, T. L., & Lewis, D. (1993). Hemispheric differences in layer III pyramidal neurons of the anterior language area. Archives of Neurology, 50, 501–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hays, D. G. (1964). Dependency theory: a formalism and some observations. Language, 40, 511–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. A neuropsychological theory. New York: John Wiley
Hecaen, H., Agostini, M., & Monzon-Montes, A. (1981). Cerebral organization in left-handers. Brain and Language, 12(2), 261–84CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hellwig, B. (2000). A quantitative analysis of the local connectivity between pyramidal neurons in layers 2/3 of the rat visual cortex. Biological Cybernetics, 82(2), 111–21CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heringer, H. J. (1996). Deutsche Syntax dependentiell. Tübingen: Stauffenburg Verlag
Hetherington, P. A., & Shapiro, M. L. (1993). Simulating Hebb cell assemblies: the necessity for partitioned dendritic trees and a post-not-pre LTD rule. Network, 4, 135–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinton, G. E., & Shallice, T. (1991). Lesioning an attractor network: investigation of acquired dyslexia. Psychological Review, 98, 74–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubel, D. (1995). Eye, brain, and vision (2 ed.). New York: Scientific American Library
Humphreys, G. W., Evett, L. J., & Taylor, D. E. (1982). Automatic phonological priming in visual word recognition. Memory and Cognition, 10, 576–90CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, G. W., & Forde, E. M. E. (2001). Hierarchies, similarity and interactivity in object recognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 453–509Google ScholarPubMed
Indefrey, P., Hagoort, P., Herzog, H., Seitz, R. J., & Brown, C. M. (2001). Syntactic processing in left prefrontal cortex is independent of lexical meaning. Neuroimage, 14, 546–55CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackendoff, R. (1977). X-bar syntax: a study of phrase structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Jackson, J. H. (1878). On affections of speech from disease of the brain (1). Brain, 1, 304–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, B., Batal, H. A., Lynch, B., Ojemann, G., Ojemann, L. M., & Scheibel, A. B. (1993). Quantitative dendritic and spine analyses of speech cortices: a case study. Brain and Language, 44, 239–53CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joanette, Y., Goulet, P., & Hannequin, D. (1990). Right hemisphere and verbal communication. New York: Springer-Verlag
Joshi, A. K., & Levy, L. S. (1982). Phrase structure trees bear more fruit than you would have thought. American Journal of Computational Linguistics, 8, 1–11Google Scholar
Kandel, E. R. (1991). Cellular mechanisms of learning and the biological basis of individuality. In E. R. Kandel & J. H. Schwartz & T. M. Jessell (Eds.), Principles of neural science (3 ed., pp. 1009–31). New York: Elsevier
Kaplan, R. M. (1972). Augmented transition networks as psychological models of sentence comprehension. Artificial Intelligence, 3, 77–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiefer, M. (2001). Perceptual and semantic sources of category-specific effects: Event-related potentials during picture and word categorization. Memory and Cognition, 29, 100–16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiefer, M., & Spitzer, M. (2001). The limits of a distributed account of conceptual knowledge. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 469–71CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleene, S. C. (1956). Representation of events in nerve nets and finite automata. In C. E. Shannon & J. McCarthy (Eds.), Automata studies (pp. 3–41). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University PressCrossRef
Kleinfeld, D., & Sompolinsky, H. (1988). Associative neural network model for the generation of temporal patterns. Theory and application to central pattern generators. Biophysical Journal, 54, 1039–51CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kluender, R., & Kutas, M. (1993). Bridging the gap: evidence from ERPs on the processing of unbounded dependencies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 196–214CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenig, T., & Lehmann, D. (1996). Microstates in language-related brain potential maps show noun–verb differences. Brain and Language, 53, 169–82CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolk, H. H. J., van Grunsven, M. J. F., & Keyser, A. (1985). On the parallelism between production and comprehension in agrammatism. In M.-L. Kean (Ed.), Agrammatism (pp. 165–206). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Korpilahti, P., Krause, C. M., Holopainen, I., & Lang, A. H. (2001). Early and late mismatch negativity elicited by words and speech-like stimuli in children. Brain and Language, 76, 332–39CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krause, C. M., Korpilahti, P., Porn, B., Jantti, J., & Lang, H. A. (1998). Automatic auditory word perception as measured by 40 Hz EEG responses. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 107, 84–7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreiter, A. K. (2002). Functional implications of temporal structure in primate cortical information processing. Zoology: Analysis of Complex Systems, 104, 241–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kujala, T., Alho, K., & Naatanen, R. (2000). Cross-modal reorganization of human cortical functions. Trends in Neuroscience, 23(3), 115–20CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambek, J. (1958). The mathematics of sentence structure. Americal Mathematical Monthly, 65, 154–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambek, J. (1959). Contributions to a mathematical analysis of the English verb phrase. Journal of the Canadian Linguistic Association, 5, 83–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato's problem: the Latent Semantic Analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1950). In search of the engram. Symposium of the Society for Experimental Biology, 4, 454–82Google Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior. The Hixxon symposium, pp. 112–36. New York: John Wiley
Clec'H, G., Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Mehler, J., Dupoux, E., Poline, J. B., Lehericy, S., Moortele, P. F., & Bihan, D. (2000). Distinct cortical areas for names of numbers and body parts independent of language and input modality. Neuroimage, 12, 381–91CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, K. H., Chung, K., Chung, J. M., & Coggeshall, R. E. (1986). Correlation of cell body size, axon size, and signal conduction velocity for individually labelled dorsal root ganglion cells in the cat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 243(3), 335–46CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehmann, D., & Skrandies, W. (1984). Spatial analysis of evoked potentials in man: a review. Progress in Neurobiology, 23, 227–50CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, W. J. M. (1974). Formal grammars in linguistics and psycholinguistics. Vol. 1. An introduction to the theory of formal languages and automata. The Hague, Paris: Mouton
Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, W. J., Schriefers, H., Vorberg, D., Meyer, A. S., Pechmann, T., & Havinga, J. (1991). The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming. Psychological Review, 98, 122–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lichtheim, L. (1885). On aphasia. Brain, 7, 433–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1989). Babbling and early speech: continuity and individual differences. First Language, 9, 191–206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1993). The child's path to spoken language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Lutzenberger, W., Pulvermüller, F., & Birbaumer, N. (1994). Words and pseudowords elicit distinct patterns of 30-Hz activity in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 176, 115–18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcel, A. J., & Patterson, K. (1978). Word recognition and production: reciprocity in clinical and normal studies. In J. Requin (Ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. VII, pp. 209–26). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Marcus, S. (1965). Sur la Notion de la Projectivite. Zeitschrift für mathematische Logik und Grundlagen der Mathematik, 11, 181–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marie, P. (1906). Revision de la question de l'aphasie: la triosieme circonvolution frontale gauche ne joue uncun role special dans la fonction du langage. Semaine Medicale (Paris), 26, 241–7Google Scholar
Markov, A. A. (1913). Essai d'une recherche statistique sur le texte du roman “Eugene Onegin.”Bulletin de l'Academie Imperiale des Sciences, St. Petersburg, 7Google Scholar
Marshall, J. C. (1980). On the biology of language acquisition. In D. Caplan (Ed.), Biological studies of mental processes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Marslen-Wilson, W., & Tyler, L. (1997). Dissociating types of mental computation. Nature, 387, 592–4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W., & Tyler, L. K. (1975). Processing structure of sentence perception. Nature, 257(5529), 784–6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W., Tyler, L. K., Waksler, R., & Older, L. (1994). Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. Psychological Review, 101, 3–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., & Warren, P. (1994). Levels of perceptual representation and process in lexical access: words, phonemes, and features. Psychological Review, 101, 633–75CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K. (1980). The temporal structure of spoken language understanding. Cognition, 8, 1–71CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A., & Chao, L. L. (2001). Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11, 194–201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L., Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (1996). Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature, 379, 649–52CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, A., Nicoll, A., & Stratford, K. (1991). Synaptic transmission between individual pyramidal neurons of the rat visual cortex in vitro. Journal of Neuroscience, 11(1), 72–84CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375–407CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCulloch, W. S., & Pitts, W. H. (1943). A logical calculus of ideas immanent in nervous activity. Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics, 5, 115–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merzenich, M. M., Kaas, J. H., Wall, J., Nelson, R. J., Sur, M., & Felleman, D. (1983). Topographic reorganization of somatosensory cortical areas 3b and 1 in adult monkeys following restricted deafferentation. Neuroscience, 8(1), 33–55CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merzenich, M. M., Kaas, J. H., Wall, J. T., Sur, M., Nelson, R. J., & Felleman, D. J. (1983). Progression of change following median nerve section in the cortical representation of the hand in areas 3b and 1 in adult owl and squirrel monkeys. Neuroscience, 10(3), 639–65CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesulam, M. M. (1990). Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Annals of Neurology, 28, 597–613CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: eviedence of a dependence of retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miceli, G., Silveri, C., Nocentini, U., & Caramazza, A. (1988). Patterns of dissociation in comprehension and production of nouns and verbs. Aphasiology, 2, 351–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miceli, G., Silveri, M., Villa, G., & Caramazza, A. (1984). On the basis of agrammatics' difficulty in producing main verbs. Cortex, 20, 207–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A., & Chomsky, N. (1963). Finite models of language users. In R. D. Luce & R. R. Bush & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology, Vol. 2 (pp. 419–91). New York, London: Wiley
Miller, R. (1991). Cortico-hippocampal interplay and the representation of contexts in the brain. Berlin: Springer
Miller, R. (1996). Axonal conduction times and human cerebral laterality. A psychobiological theory. Amsterdam: Harwood Academic Publishers
Miller, R., & Wickens, J. R. (1991). Corticostriatal cell assemblies in selective attention and in representation of predictable and controllable events: a general statement of corticostriatal interplay and the role of striatal dopamine. Concepts in Neuroscience, 2, 65–95Google Scholar
Milner, P. M. (1957). The cell assembly: Mk. II. Psychological Review, 64, 242–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, P. M. (1974). A model for visual shape recognition. Psychological Review, 81, 521–35CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, P. M. (1996). Neural representation: some old problems revisited. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 69–77CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minsky, M. (1972). Computation: finite and infinite machines. London: Prentice-Hall
Minsky, M., & Papert, S. (1969). Perceptrons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Mohr, B., & Pulvermüller, E. (2002). Redundancy gains and costs in word processing: the effect of short stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition
Mohr, B., Pulvermüller, F., Mittelstädt, K., & Rayman, J. (1996). Multiple stimulus presentation facilitates lexical processing. Neuropsychologia, 34, 1003–13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohr, B., Pulvermüller, F., Rayman, J., & Zaidel, E. (1994). Interhemispheric cooperation during lexical processing is mediated by the corpus callosum: evidence from the split-brain. Neuroscience Letters, 181, 17–21CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohr, B., Pulvermüller, F., & Zaidel, E. (1994). Lexical decision after left, right and bilateral presentation of content words, function words and non-words: evidence for interhemispheric interaction. Neuropsychologia, 32, 105–24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molfese, D. L. (1972). Cerebral asymmetry in infants, children and adults: auditory evoked responses to speech and noise stimuli. Unpublished doctoral dissertation: Pennsylvania State University
Molfese, D. L., Burger-Judisch, L. M., Gill, L. A., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsch-Pasek, K. A. (1996). Electrophysiological correlates of noun-verb processing in adults. Brain and Language, 54, 388–413CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montoya, P., Larbig, W., Pulvermüller, F., Flor, H., & Birbaumer, N. (1996). Cortical correlates of semantic classical conditioning. Psychophysiology, 33, 644–49CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, C. J., & Price, C. J. (1999). A functional neuroimaging study of the variables that generate category-specific object processing differences. Brain, 122, 943–62CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moro, A., Tettamanti, M., Perani, D., Donati, C., Cappa, S. F., & Fazio, F. (2001). Syntax and the brain: disentangling grammar by selective anomalies. Neuroimage, 13, 110–18CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, J. (1969). The interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review, 76, 165–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, H. M., King, J. W., & Kutas, M. (1997). Event-related potentials elicited by spoken relative clauses. Cognitive Brain Research, 5, 193–203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mummery, C. J., Patterson, K., Hodges, J. R., & Price, C. J. (1998). Functional neuroanatomy of the semantic system: divisible by what? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 766–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Näätänen, R. (2001). The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent (MMNm). Psychophysiology, 38, 1–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Näätänen, R., Lehtokoski, A., Lennes, M., Cheour, M., Huotilainen, M., Iivonen, A., Valnio, A., Alku, P., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Luuk, A., Allik, J., Sinkkonen, J., & Alho, K. (1997). Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses. Nature, 385, 432–4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Näätänen, R., & Winkler, I. (1999). The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitive neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin, 12, 826–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neininger, B., & Pulvermüller, F. (2001). The right hemisphere's role in action word processing: a double case study. Neurocase, 7, 303–17CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neininger, B., & Pulvermüller, E. (2002). Word category–specific deficits after lesions in the right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, in press
Neville, H., Nicol, J. L., Barss, A., Forster, K. I., & Garrett, M. F. (1991). Syntactically based sentence processing classes: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 151–65CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neville, H. J., Bavelier, D., Corina, D., Rauschecker, J., Karni, A., Lalwani, A., Braun, A., Clark, V., Jezzard, P., & Turner, R. (1998). Cerebral organization for language in deaf and hearing subjects: biological constraints and effects of experience. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 95, 922–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neville, H. J., Mills, D. L., & Lawson, D. S. (1992). Fractionating language: different neural subsystems with different sensitive periods. Cerebral Cortex, 2, 244–58CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, A. J., Bavelier, D., Corina, D., Jezzard, P., & Neville, H. J. (2002). A critical period for right hemisphere recruitment in American Sign Language processing. Neuroscience, 5, 76–80Google ScholarPubMed
Nobre, A. C., & McCarthy, G. (1994). Language-related EPRs: scalp distributions and modulation by word type and semantic priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 233–255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noppeney, U., & Price, C. J. (2002). Retrieval of visual, auditory, and abstract semantics. Neuroimage, 15, 917–26CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norris, D., McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A. (2000). Merging information in speech recognition: feedback is never necessary. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 299–370CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osterhout, L. (1997). On the brain response to syntactic anomalies: manipulations of word position and word class reveal individual differences. Brain and Language, 59(3), 494–522CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785–806CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Page, M. P., & Norris, D. (1998). The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. Psychological Review, 105, 761–81CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palm, G. (1980). On associative memory. Biology and Cybernetics, 36(1), 19–31CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palm, G. (1981). Towards a theory of cell assemblies. Biology and Cybernetics, 39(3), 181–94CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palm, G. (1982). Neural assemblies. Berlin: Springer
Palm, G. (1990). Local learning rules and sparse coding in neural networks. In R. Eckmiller (Ed.), Advanced neural computers (pp. 145–50). Amsterdam: ElsevierCrossRef
Palm, G. (1993a). Cell assemblies, coherence, and corticohippocampal interplay. Hippocampus, 3(Spec No), 219–25Google Scholar
Palm, G. (1993b). On the internal structure of cell assemblies. In A. Aertsen (Ed.), Brain theory: spatio-temporal aspects of brain function (pp. 261–70). Amsterdam: Elsevier
Palm, G., & Sommer, F. T. (1995). Associative data storage and retrieval in neural networks. In E. Domany & J. L. van Hemmen & K. Schulten (Eds.), Models of neural networks III (pp. 79–118). New York: Springer Verlag
Pandya, D. N., & Yeterian, E. H. (1985). Architecture and connections of cortical association areas. In A. Peters & E. G. Jones (Eds.), Cerebral cortex, Vol. 4. Association and auditory cortices (pp. 3–61). London: Plenum PressCrossRef
Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2001). Semantic dementia. In J. L. McClelland (Ed.), The international encyclopaedia of the social and behavioural sciences. Section: disorders of the adult brain. New York: Pergamon PressCrossRef
Paus, T., Perry, D. W., Zatorre, R. J., Worsley, K. J., & Evans, A. C. (1996). Modulation of cerebral blood flow in the human auditory cortex during speech: role of motor-to-sensory discharges. European Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 2236–46CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penfield, W., & Rassmussen, T. (1950). The cerebral cortex of man. New York: Macmillan
Penfield, W., & Roberts, L. (1959). Speech and brain mechanisms. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press
Perani, D., Schnur, T., Tettamanti, M., Gorno-Tempini, M., Cappa, S. F., & Fazio, F. (1999). Word and picture matching: a PET study of semantic category effects. Neuropsychologia, 37, 293–306CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, S., & Fiez, J. A. (1993). The processing of single words studied with positron emission tomography. Annual Review in Neuroscience, 16, 509–30CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, S., Fox, P., Posner, M., Mintun, M., & Raichle, M. (1989). Positron emission tomography studies of the processing of single words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 153–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petitto, L. A., Zatorre, R. J., Gauna, K., Nikelski, E. J., Dostie, D., & Evans, A. C. (2000). Speech-like cerebral activity in profoundly deaf people processing signed languages: implications for the neural basis of human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 97, 13961–6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petri, C. A. (1970). Kommunikation mit Automaten. Dissertation: Universität Bonn
Pick, A. (1913). Die agrammatischen Sprachstörungen. Studien zur psychologischen Grundlegung der Aphasielehre. Berlin: SpringerCrossRef
Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. (1999). Syntactic priming in language production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 136–41CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. How the mind creates language. New York: Harper Collins Publishers
Pinker, S. (1997). Words and rules in the human brain. Nature, 387, 547–8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plaut, D. C., & Shallice, T. (1993). Deep dyslexia: a case study of connectionist neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 377–500CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poizner, H., Bellugi, U., & Klima, E. S. (1990). Biological foundations of language: Clues from sign language. Annual Review in Neuroscience, 13, 283–307CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I., & DiGirolamo, G. J. (1999). Flexible neural circuitry in word processing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 299–300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preissl, H., Pulvermüller, F., Lutzenberger, W., & Birbaumer, N. (1995). Evoked potentials distinguish nouns from verbs. Neuroscience Letters, 197, 81–3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Previc, F. H. (1991). A general theory concerning the prenatal origins of cerebral lateralization in humans. Psychological Review, 98, 299–334CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, C. J. (2000). The anatomy of language: contributions from functional neuroimaging. Journal of Anatomy, 197 Pt 3, 335–59CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, C. J., Warburton, E. A., Moore, C. J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Friston, K. J. (2001). Dynamic diaschisis: anatomically remote and context-sensitive human brain lesions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 419–29CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, C. J., Wise, R. J. S., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1996). Demonstrating the implicit processing of visually presented words and pseudowords. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 62–70CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F. (1992). Constituents of a neurological theory of language. Concepts in Neuroscience, 3, 157–200Google Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. (1993). On connecting syntax and the brain. In A. Aertsen (Ed.), Brain theory – spatio-temporal aspects of brain function (pp. 131–45). New York: Elsevier
Pulvermüller, F. (1994). Syntax und Hirnmechanismen. Perspektiven einer multidisziplinären Sprachwissenschaft. Kognitionswissenschaft, 4, 17–31Google Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. (1995). Agrammatism: behavioral description and neurobiological explanation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 165–81CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F. (1998). On the matter of rules. Past tense formation as a test-case for brain models of language. Network: Computation in Neural Systems, 9, R1–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. (1999a). Lexical access as a brain mechanism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 50–2Google Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. (1999b). Words in the brain's language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 253–336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. (2000). Syntactic circuits: how does the brain create serial order in sentences?Brain and Language, 71(1), 194–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F. (2001). Brain reflections of words and their meaning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 517–24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F. (2002). A brain perspective on language mechanisms: from discrete neuronal ensembles to serial order. Progress in Neurobiology, 67, 85–111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F., Assadollahi, R., & Elbert, T. (2001). Neuromagnetic evidence for early semantic access in word recognition. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(1), 201–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F., Birbaumer, N., Lutzenberger, W., & Mohr, B. (1997). High-frequency brain activity: its possible role in attention, perception and language processing. Progress in Neurobiology, 52, 427–45CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F., Eulitz, C., Pantev, C., Mohr, B., Feige, B., Lutzenberger, W., Elbert, T., & Birbaumer, N. (1996). High-frequency cortical responses reflect lexical processing: an MEG study. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 98, 76–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F., Härle, M., & Hummel, F. (2000). Neurophysiological distinction of verb categories. Neuroreport, 11(12), 2789–93CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F., Hummel, F., & Härle, M. (2001). Walking or Talking?: Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of action verb processing. Brain and Language, 78, 143–68CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F., Keil, A., & Elbert, T. (1999). High-frequency brain activity: perception or active memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 250–2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F., Kujala, T., Shtyrov, Y., Simola, J., Tiitinen, H., Alku, P., Alho, K., Martinkauppi, S., Ilmoniemi, R. J., & Näätänen, R. (2001). Memory traces for words as revealed by the mismatch negativity. NeuroImage, 14, 607–16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F., Lutzenberger, W., & Preissl, H. (1999). Nouns and verbs in the intact brain: evidence from event-related potentials and high-frequency cortical responses. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 498–508CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F., & Mohr, B. (1996). The concept of transcortical cell assemblies: a key to the understanding of cortical lateralization and interhemispheric interaction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 20, 557–66CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F., Mohr, B., & Schleichert, H. (1999). Semantic or lexico-syntactic factors: what determines word-class specific activity in the human brain? Neuroscience Letters, 275, 81–4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F., Mohr, B., Sedat, N., Hadler, B., & Rayman, J. (1996). Word class specific deficits in Wernicke's aphasia. Neurocase, 2, 203–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F., & Preissl, H. (1991). A cell assembly model of language. Network: Computation in Neural Systems, 2, 455–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F., Preissl, H., Lutzenberger, W., & Birbaumer, N. (1996). Brain rhythms of language: nouns versus verbs. European Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 937–41CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermüller, F., & Schumann, J. H. (1994). Neurobiological mechanisms of language acquisition. Language Learning, 44, 681–734CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K. (2000). Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: a time-course study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 507–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rauschecker, J. P., & Singer, W. (1979). Changes in the circuitry of the kitten visual cortex are gated by postsynaptic activity. Nature, 280, 58–60CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redlich, A. N. (1993). Redundancy reduction as a strategy for unsupervised learning. Neural Computation, 3, 289–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reichardt, W., & Varju, D. (1959). Übertragungseigenschaften im Auswertesystem für das Bewegungssehen. Zeitschrift für Naturforschung, 14b, 674–89Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 188–94CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–141CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G., Luppino, G., & Matelli, M. (1998). The organization of the cortical motor system: new concepts. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 106, 283–96CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W., Johnson, D., & Boyes-Bream, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenblatt, F. (1959). Two theorems of statistical separability in the perceptron. In M. Selfridge (Ed.), Mechanisation of thought processes: proceedings of a symposium held at the National Physical Laboratory. London: HMSO
Rugg, M. D. (1983). Further study of the electrophysiological correlates of lexical decision. Brain and Language, 19, 142–52CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rugg, M. D. (1990). Event-related potentials dissociate repetition effects of high- and low-frequency words. Memory and Cognition, 18, 367–79CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G., & Williams, R. (1986). Learning internal representations by backpropagation. In D. E. Rumelhart & J. L. McClelland (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the mircrostructure of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tense of English verbs. In J. L. McClelland & D. E. Rumelhart (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1987). Learning the past tense of English verbs: implicit rules or parallel distributed processing. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274(5294), 1926–8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salmelin, R., Helenius, P., & Kuukka, K. (1999). Only time can tell – words in context. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheibel, A. B., Paul, L. A., Fried, I., Forsythe, A. B., Tomiyasu, U., Wechsler, A., Kao, A., & Slotnick, J. (1985). Dendritic organization of the anterior speech area. Experimental Neurology, 87, 109–17CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnelle, H. (1996a). Approaches to computational brain theories of language/kern2pt– a review of recent proposals. Theoretical Linguistics, 22, 49–104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnelle, H. (1996b). Die Natur der Sprache. Die Dynamik der Prozesse des Sprechens und Verstehens (2 ed.). Berlin, New York: Walter de Gruyter
Schnelle, H. (1996c). Net-linguistic approaches to linguistic structure, brain topography, and cerebral processes
Schumann, J. H. (1997). The neurobiology of affect in language. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers
Seldon, H. L. (1985). The anatomy of speech perception. Human auditory cortex. In A. Peters & E. G. Jones (Eds.), Cerebral cortex, Vol. 5. Association and auditory cortices (pp. 273–327). London: Plenum PressCrossRef
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. New York: Cambridge University Press
Shannon, C. E., & Weaver, W. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication. Urbana: University of Illinois Press
Shastri, L., & Ajjanagadde, V. (1993). From simple associations to systematic reasoning: a connectionist representation of rules, variables and dynamic bindings using temporal synchrony. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 417–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shtyrov, Y., Kujala, T., Palva, S., Ilmoniemi, R. J., & Näätänen, R. (2000). Discrimination of speech and of complex nonspeech sounds of different temporal structure in the left and right cerebral hemispheres. NeuroImage, 12(6), 657–63CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shtyrov, Y., & Pulvermüller, F. (2002a). Memory traces for inflectional affixes as shown by the mismatch negativity. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 1085–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shtyrov, Y., & Pulvermüller, F. (2002b). Neurophysiological evidence of memory traces for words in the human brain. Neuroreport. In press
Singer, W., Engel, A. K., Kreiter, A. K., Munk, M. H. J., Neuenschwander, S., & Roelfsema, P. R. (1997). Neuronal assemblies: necessity, signature and detectability. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 252–62CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, W., & Gray, C. M. (1995). Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annual Review in Neuroscience, 18, 555–86CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skrandies, W. (1998). Evoked potential correlates of semantic meaning –/kern2.5pta brain mapping study. Cognitive Brain Research, 6, 173–83CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skrandies, W. (1999). Early effects of semantic meaning on electrical brain activity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommer, F. T., & Wennekers, T. (2000). Modelling studies on the computational function of fast temporal structure in cortical circuit activity. Journal of Physiology, Paris, 94, 473–88CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sougné, J. (1998). Connectionism and the problem of multiple instantiation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 183–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, M., Kischka, U., Guckel, F., Bellemann, M. E., Kammer, T., Seyyedi, S., Weisbrod, M., Schwartz, A., & Brix, G. (1998). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of category-specific cortical activation: evidence for semantic maps. Cognitive Brain Research, 6, 309–19CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinmetz, H., Volkmann, J., Jancke, L., & Freund, H. J. (1991). Anatomical left–right asymmetry of language-related temporal cortex is different in left- and right-handers. Annals of Neurology, 29, 315–19CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterr, A., Muller, M. M., Elbert, T., Rockstroh, B., Pantev, C., & Taub, E. (1998). Perceptual correlates of changes in cortical representation of fingers in blind multifinger Braille readers. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(11), 4417–23CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swinney, D., Onifer, W., Prather, P., & Hirshkowitz, M. (1979). Semantic fascilation across sensory modalities in the processing of individual words and sentences. Memory and Cognition, 7, 159–65CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallon-Baudry, C., & Bertrand, O. (1999). Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 151–61CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tesnière, L. (1953). Esquisse d'une syntax structurale. Paris: Klincksieck
Tesnière, L. (1959). Eléments de syntaxe structurale. Paris: Klincksieck
Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1999). The neurobiology of knowledge retrieval. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsumoto, T. (1992). Long-term potentiation and long-term depression in the neocortex. Progress in Neurobiology, 39, 209–28CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullman, M., Corkin, S., Coppola, M., Hickok, G., Growdon, J., Koroshetz, W., & Pinker, S. (1997). A neural dissociation within language: evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that grammatical rules are processed by the procedural system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 266–76CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaadia, E., Haalman, I., Abeles, M., Bergman, H., Prut, Y., Slovin, H., & Aertsen, A. (1995). Dynamics of neuronal interactions in monkey cortex in relation to behavioural events. Nature, 373, 515–18CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varju, D., & Reichardt, W. (1967). Übertragungseigenschaften im Auswertesystem für das Bewegungssehen II. Zeitschrift für Naturforschung, 22b, 1343–51Google Scholar
der Malsburg, C. (1985). Nervous structures with dynamical links. Bericht der Bunsengesellschaft für Physikalische Chemie, 89, 703–10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
der Malsburg, C. (1995). Binding in models of perception and brain function. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 5, 520–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waibel, A. H., Hanazawa, T., Hinton, G., Shikano, K., & Lang, K. J. (1995). Phoneme recognition using time-delay neural networks. In Y. Chauvin & D. E. Rumelhart (Eds.), Backpropagation: theory, architectures, and applications. Developments in connectionist theory (pp. 35–61). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Warburton, E., Wise, R. J. S., Price, C. J., Weiller, C., Hadar, U., Ramsay, S., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1996). Noun and verb retrieval by normal subjects: studies with PET. Brain, 119, 159–79CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. A. (1983). Category specific access dysphasia. Brain, 106, 859–78CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. A. (1987). Categories of knowledge: further fractionations and an attempted integration. Brain, 110, 1273–96CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829–54CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiller, C., Isensee, C., Rijntjes, M., Huber, W., Müller, S., Bier, D., Dutschka, K., Woods, R. P., North, J., & Diener, H. C. (1995). Recovery from Wernicke's aphasia: a positron emission tomography study. Annals of Neurology, 37, 723–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wennekers, T., & Palm, G. (1996). Controlling the speed of synfire chains. Proceedings of the International Conference on Artificial Neural Networks, 451–6Google Scholar
Wermter, S., & Elshaw, M. (2002). A neurocognitively inspired modular approach to self-organisation of action verb processing. Connection Science, In press
Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasische Symptomencomplex. Eine psychologische Studie auf anatomischer Basis. Breslau: Kohn und Weigert
Weyerts, H., Penke, M., Dohrn, U., Clahsen, H., & Münte, T. (1997). Brain potentials indicate differences between regular and irregular German plurals. NeuroReport, 8, 957–62CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickelgren, W. A. (1969). Context-sensitive coding, associative memory, and serial order in (speech) behavior. Psychological Review, 76, 1–15CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickens, J. R. (1993). A theory of the striatum. Oxford: Pergamon Press
Willshaw, D. J.Buneman, O. P., & Longuet-Higgins, H. C. (1969). Non-holographic associative memory. Nature, 222(197), 960–2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willwacher, G. (1976). Faehigkeiten eines assoziativen Speichersystems im Vergleich zu Gehirnfunktionen. Biological Cybernetics, 24, 181–98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willwacher, G. (1982). Storage of a temporal pattern sequence in a network. Biological Cybernetics, 43, 115–26CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winograd, T. (1983). Language as a cognitive process, vol. 1: Syntax. Reading, MA: Addision-Wesley
Wise, R., Chollet, F., Hadar, U., Fiston, K., Hoffner, E., & Frackowiak, R. (1991). Distribution of cortical neural networks involved in word comprehension and word retrieval. Brain, 114, 1803–17CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical Investigations. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers
Woods, B. T. (1983). Is the left hemisphere specialized for language at birth? Trends in Neurosciences, 6, 115–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woods, W. A. (1973). An experimental parsing system for transition network grammars. In R. Rustin (Ed.), Natural language processing (pp. 111–154). New York: Algorithmics Press
Young, M. P., Scannell, J. W., & Burns, G. (1995). The analysis of cortical connectivity. Heidelberg: Springer
Zaidel, E. (1976). Auditory vocabulary of the right hemisphere following brain bisection or hemideocortication. Cortex, 12, 191–211CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaidel, E. (1985). Language in the right hemisphere. In D. F. Benson & E. Zaidel (Eds.), The dual brain (pp. 205–31). New York: Guilford
Zaidel, E.Kasher, A., Soroker, N., Batori, G., Giora, R., & Graves, D. (2000). Hemispheric contributions to pragmatics. Brain Cognition, 43(1–3), 438–43Google ScholarPubMed
Zaidel, E., & Rayman, J. (1994). Interhemispheric control in the normal brain: evidence from redundant bilateral presentation. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV: concious and subconcious information processing (pp. 477–504). Boston, MA: MIT Press
Zatorre, R. J., Evans, A. C., Meyer, E., & Gjedde, A. (1992). Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discremination in speech processing. Science, 256, 846–49CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2000). The relative time course of semantic and phonological activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26(5), 1245–65Google ScholarPubMed
Zhou, Y. D., & Fuster, J. M. (2000). Visuo-tactile cross-modal associations in cortical somatosensory cells. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 97(17), 9777–82CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zipser, D., Kehoe, B., Littlewort, G., & Fuster, J. (1993). A spiking network model of short-term active memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 13(8), 3406–20CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zohary, E. (1992). Population coding of visual stimuli by cortical neurons tuned to more than one dimension. Biological Cybernetics, 66, 265–72CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Friedemann Pulvermüller, Medical Research Council, Cambridge
  • Book: The Neuroscience of Language
  • Online publication: 15 December 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511615528.022
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Friedemann Pulvermüller, Medical Research Council, Cambridge
  • Book: The Neuroscience of Language
  • Online publication: 15 December 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511615528.022
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Friedemann Pulvermüller, Medical Research Council, Cambridge
  • Book: The Neuroscience of Language
  • Online publication: 15 December 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511615528.022
Available formats
×