Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-fwgfc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-09T01:26:31.857Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Affective processes in a multivoiced self

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 October 2009

Harke A. Bosma
Affiliation:
Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, The Netherlands
E. Saskia Kunnen
Affiliation:
Universiteit Utrecht, The Netherlands
Get access

Summary

In studying affective processes, one can start from the assumption that during human development, affective responses are increasingly influenced by the relation individuals have with themselves. To illustrate this, let's look at a crawling infant's fascination for a bouncing ball. Initially, the ball is not experienced as part of the infant's self, but the influence of self-reflectivity becomes explicit when the child is approximately two years old. When playing with another child, it may shout: “Ball mine!” The child has developed a special relationship with the ball, considering himself its “possessor.” The ball falls within an invisible boundary between Mine and not-Mine. The child's definition of himself as the possessor not only indicates that the self is explicitly involved, it also has immediate repercussions for the affective processes both between the child and the ball and between the child and others. Still later, when the child has become a member of a sports club, he may consider himself a “good soccer player” and feel very proud of this. This self-evaluation indicates that the child, being able to adopt the complex language-games of a community, applies to himself a particular standard which is, as an organizing principle, accepted by a group or community. In Mead's (1934) terms, the child is organizing his self by “taking the role of the other” and his feelings are also organized by this role-taking.

As the above examples suggest, the existence of primary affect (e.g. joy in playing with a ball) is influenced by the development of the self (“ball is mine”), which strongly colors and organizes the child's, and later the adult's, secondary affective responses to a great variety of situations.

Type
Chapter
Information
Identity and Emotion
Development through Self-Organization
, pp. 120 - 140
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2001

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×