Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wg55d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T08:53:15.385Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Statistical analysis of multichannel scalp field data

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 December 2009

Christoph M. Michel
Affiliation:
Université de Genève
Thomas Koenig
Affiliation:
University Hospital of Psychiatry, Berne, Switzerland
Daniel Brandeis
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University of Zurich, Switzerland and Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Grmany
Lorena R. R. Gianotti
Affiliation:
Universität Zürich
Jiří Wackermann
Affiliation:
Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and Mental Health, Freiburg im Breisgau, Germany
Get access

Summary

Introduction

High density spatial and temporal sampling of EEG data enhances the quality of results of electrophysiological experiments. Because EEG sources typically produce widespread electric fields (see Chapter 3) and operate at frequencies well below the sampling rate, increasing the number of electrodes and time samples will not necessarily increase the number of observed processes, but mainly increase the accuracy of the representation of these processes. This is namely the case when inverse solutions are computed.

As a consequence, increasing the sampling in space and time increases the redundancy of the data (in space, because electrodes are correlated due to volume conduction, and time, because neighboring time points are correlated), while the degrees of freedom of the data change only little. This has to be taken into account when statistical inferences are to be made from the data. However, in many ERP studies, the intrinsic correlation structure of the data has been disregarded. Often, some electrodes or groups of electrodes are a priori selected as the analysis entity and considered as repeated (within subject) measures that are analyzed using standard univariate statistics. The increased spatial resolution obtained with more electrodes is thus poorly represented by the resulting statistics. In addition, the assumptions made (e.g. in terms of what constitutes a repeated measure) are not supported by what we know about the properties of EEG data.

From the point of view of physics (see Chapter 3), the natural “atomic” analysis entity of EEG and ERP data is the scalp electric field.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Gevins, A, Le, J, Martin, NKet al. High resolution EEG: 124-channel recording, spatial deblurring and MRI integration methods. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1994;90:337–358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michel, CM, Thut, G, Morand, Set al. Electric source imaging of human brain functions. Brain Research. Brain Research Review 2001;36:108–118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tucker, DM. Spatial sampling of head electrical fields: the geodesic sensor net. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1993;87:154–163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duffy, FH, Bartels, PH, Burchfiel, JL. Significance probability mapping: an aid in the topographic analysis of brain electrical activity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1981;51:455–462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steger, J, Imhof, K, Steinhausen, H, Brandeis, D. Brain mapping of bilateral interactions in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and control boys. Clinical Neurophysiology 2000;111:1141–1156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vasey, MW, Thayer, JF. The continuing problem of false positives in repeated measures ANOVA in psychophysiology: a multivariate solution. Psychophysiology 1987;24:479–486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galan, L, Biscay, R, Rodriguez, JL, Perez-Abalo, MC, Rodriguez, R. Testing topographic differences between event related brain potentials by using non-parametric combinations of permutation tests. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1997;102:240–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenblatt, RE, Pflieger, ME. Randomization-based hypothesis testing from event-related data. Brain Topography 2004;16:225–232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karniski, W, Blair, RC, Snider, AD. An exact statistical method for comparing topographic maps, with any number of subjects and electrodes. Brain Topography 1994;6:203–210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenig, T, Melie-Garcia, L, Stein, M, Strik, W, Lehmann, C. Establishing correlations of scalp field maps with other experimental variables using covariance analysis and resampling methods. Clinical Neurophysiology 2008;119:1262–1270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lobaugh, NJ, West, R, McIntosh, AR. Spatiotemporal analysis of experimental differences in event-related potential data with partial least squares. Psychophysiology 2001;38:517–530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maris, E. Randomization tests for ERP topographies and whole spatiotemporal data matrices. Psychophysiology 2004;41:142–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strik, WK, Fallgatter, AJ, Brandeis, D, Pascual-Marqui, RD. Three-dimensional tomography of event-related potentials during response inhibition: evidence for phasic frontal lobe activation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1998;108:406–413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edgington, ES, Onghena, P. Randomization Tests. 4th edn. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 2007.Google Scholar
Manly, BFJ. Randomization, Bootstrap, and Monte Carlo Methods in Biology. 3rd edn. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/ CRC; 2007.Google Scholar
Hoeffding, W. The large sample power of tests based on permutations of observations. Annals of Mathematical Statistics 1952;23:169–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kempthorne, O, Doerfler, TE. The behaviour of some significance tests under experimental randomization. Biometrika 1969;56:231–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, M, Dierks, T, Brandeis, Det al. Plasticity in the adult language system: a longitudinal electrophysiological study on second language learning. Neuroimage 2006;33:774–783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kutas, M, Hillyard, SA. Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature 1984;307:161–163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kondakor, I, Pascual-Marqui, RD, Michel, CM, Lehmann, D. Event-related potential map differences depend on the prestimulus microstates. Journal of Medical Engineering and Technology 1995;19:66–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wirth, M, Horn, H, Koenig, Tet al. The early context effect reflects activity in the temporo-prefrontal semantic system – evidence from electrical neuroimaging of abstract and concrete word reading. Neuroimage 2008;42:423–436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, AR, Lobaugh NJ, . Partial least squares analysis of neuroimaging data: applications and advances. Neuroimage 2004;23 Suppl 1:S250–S263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schumacher, R, Wirth, M, Perrig, Wet al. ERP correlates of supraordinate category activation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 2009; in press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, KJ, Holmes, A, Poline, JB, Price, CJ, Frith, CD. Detecting activations in PET and fMRI: levels of inference and power. Neuroimage 1996;4:223–235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nichols, TE, Holmes, AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Human Brain Mapping 2002;15:1–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Worsley, KJ, Taylor, JE, Tomaiuolo, F, Lerch, J. Unified univariate and multivariate random field theory. Neuroimage 2004;23 Suppl 1:S189–S195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, KJ, Stephan, KE, Lund, TE, Morcom, A, Kiebel, S. Mixed-effects and fMRI studies. Neuroimage 2005;24:244–252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×