Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-p566r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T20:07:51.734Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

26 - Ontological concept versus shape in word learning from a crosslinguistic point of view

from Part I - Language acquisition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Chungmin Lee
Affiliation:
Seoul National University
Greg B. Simpson
Affiliation:
University of Kansas
Youngjin Kim
Affiliation:
Ajou University, Republic of Korea
Ping Li
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Children have learned over 14,000 words by age 6. It is estimated that children acquire about nine new words a day from the age of 18 months to 6 years (Carey, 1978). How do they acquire so many words at such a rapid rate? If children's acquisition of words involves an inductive process, i.e. unbiased hypothesis testing, then, their fast learning at an earlier stage is difficult to explain. Consider an example of a child learning a new word. A mother points to a rabbit and says ‘rabbit’ to her child. There seem unlimited possibilities that the child might guess the word is intended to describe. It could refer to the whole object (the rabbit), a part of the object (long ears, a tail, red eyes, a leg, etc.), the substance the object is made of (fur, bones, flesh, etc.), some property of the object (furry, white, dirty, etc.), and so on. In spite of an infinite set of possibilities about the novel word's meaning, the child rapidly and successfully connects the novel word to its meaning. This is the well-known inductive problem that Quine (1960) has posed. As a solution for this problem, many proposals have assumed that children might be equipped with certain implicit biases or constraints that narrow down the possibilities when ascribing meaning to a novel word (E. Clark, 1987; Landau, Smith & Jones, 1988; Markman, 1994; Soja, Carey & Spelke, 1991).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×