Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-gq7q9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T13:07:24.096Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Historical aspects of memory and its disorders

from Part I

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 January 2010

German E. Berrios
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
John R. Hodges
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Get access

Summary

Historical analysis can contribute to the understanding of memory complaints and disorders (e.g. Burnham, 1888–89; Berrios, 1985a, 1990, 1992b, 1995; Bulbena & Berrios, 1986; Levin et al., 1983), particularly if it takes into account their psychiatric dimension. At a surface level, history may identify those current concepts that have developed out of the clinical observation of specific patients. At a deeper level, it can identify the theoretical and social frames within which those observations were made. In general, history will inform the memory researcher of the hidden conceptual stipulations (Edgell, 1924; Schacter, 1982; Simondon, 1982) governing the nosological status of phenomena such as fugues (Hacking, 1996), déjà vu (Berrios, 1995), and the ‘memory failure’ of schizophrenia (Rund, 1988; Kirkpatrick et al., 1986).

Using the criterion of ‘amount of experimental work’, Tulving (1983) called the period before Ebbinghaus the ‘dark ages’ in the history of memory. In the same vein, Hacking (1995) proposed that ‘the sciences of memory were new in the latter part of the nineteenth century’ (p. 198) but as Murray (1976) has shown, memory and its disorders were in fact frequently discussed during and before this period. Interestingly enough, these debates also focused on narratives style, laws of association, and content of memory, features which until recently had been neglected. To be sure, ‘models of memory’ during these earlier periods are different from what Ebbinghaus (1885) was to propose towards the end of the century (e.g. Shakow, 1930; Postman, 1968; Caparrós, 1986), and from what Tulving (1983) would consider as ‘scientific’.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2000

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×