Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vfjqv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T15:48:47.911Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 November 2016

David R. Olson
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
The Mind on Paper
Reading, Consciousness and Rationality
, pp. 244 - 261
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abrahamsen, A. A. (1977). Child language: An interdisciplinary guide to theory and research. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Acocella, J. (November 11, 2013). Renaissance man: Boccaccio’s Decameron. The New Yorker, 7881.Google Scholar
Adams, M. J. (2011). Advancing our students’ language and literacy: The challenge of complex texts. American Educator, 34(4), 311.Google Scholar
Adams, M. J., Treiman, R. & Pressley, M. (1998). Reading, writing, and literacy. In Damon, W. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 275356). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Adonis, A. (2012). Education, education, education: Reforming England’s schools. London, UK: Biteback.Google Scholar
Allen, D. (2013). Our declaration: A reading of the Declaration of Independence in defense of equality. New York, NY: Liveright.Google Scholar
Apperly, I. (2011). Mindreaders: The cognitive basis of “theory of mind.” New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Appleby, A. & Langer, J. (1987). How writing shapes thinking. Washington, DC: National Council for the Teaching of English.Google Scholar
Aram, D., Fine, Y. & Ziv, M. (2013). Enhancing parent–child shared book reading interaction. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28, 111122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arendt, H. (1954). The crisis in education. Between past and future. New York, NY: Viking.Google Scholar
Assman, J. (2012). Cultural memory and the myth of the Axial Age. In Bellan, R. N & Joas, H (Eds.), The Axial Age and its consequences (pp. 366407). Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astington, J. & Baird, J. (2005). Why language matters for theory of mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astington, J., Harris, P. & Olson, D. R. (1988). Developing theories of mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Astington, J. W. & Hughes, C. (2013). Theory of mind: Self-reflection and social understanding. In Zelazo, P. D (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 398424). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Astington, J. W. & Olson, D. R. (1990). Metacognitive and metalinguistic language: Learning to talk about thought. Applied Psychology, 39, 7787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ayer, F. (1936). Language, truth and logic. London: Victor Gollancz.Google Scholar
Baines, J. (2008). The earliest Egyptian writing: development, context, purpose. In Houston, S. (Ed.), The first writing: Script invention as history and process (pp. 150189). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bakhurst, D. (2011). The formation of reason. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banfield, A. (1993). Where epistemology, style, and grammar meet literary history: The development of represented speech and thought. In Lucy, J (Ed.), Reflexive language: Reported speech and metapragmatics (pp. 339364). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. New York, NY: Chandler Publishing.Google Scholar
Beal, C. (1990). Development of knowledge about the role of inference in text comprehension. Child Development, 61, 10111023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beal, C. R. & Flavell, J. H. (1984). Development of the ability to distinguish communicative intention and literal message meaning. Child Development, 55, 920928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bereiter, C. & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berman, R. & Ravid, D. (2009). Becoming a literate language user: Oral and written text construction across adolescence. In Olson, D. R & Torrance, N (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of literacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bernstein, R. (2010). The pragmatic turn. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2000). Symbolic representation across domains in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76, 173189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biber, D. (1986). Spoken and written textual dimensions in English: Resolving the contradictory findings. Language, 62, 384414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biber, D. (2009). Are there linguistic consequences of literacy? In Olson, D. R. & Torrance, N. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of literacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, A. (1968). The Republic of Plato. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. (2009). Predicative minds: The social ontogeny of propositional thinking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, L. & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. & Rumain, B. (1981). Children’s comprehension of “or”: Evidence for a sequence of competencies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 31, 4670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandom, R. (1994). Making it explicit: Reasoning, representing and discursive commitment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brandom, R. (2011). Perspectives on Pragmatism: Classical, recent and contemporary. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bredo, E. (1994). Reconstructing educational psychology: Situated cognition and Deweyan pragmatism. Educational Psychologist, 29, 2335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, A. (1997). Transforming schools into communities of thinking and learning about serious matters. American Psychologist, 52, 399413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. S. (1960). The process of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (2002). Narrative distancing: A foundation for literacy. In Brockmeier, J, Wang, M & Olson, D. R (Eds.), Literacy, narrative and culture. Richmond, UK: Curzon.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S., Olver, R. & Greenfield, P. (Eds.). (1966). Studies in cognitive growth. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Caines, M. (May 15, 2009). Arrange one’s face: Review of H. Mantel’s “Wolf Hall.” Times Literary Supplement. Retrieved from www.the-tls.co.uk/tls/public/article758550.ece.Google Scholar
Calkins, L. (2006). A guide to the writing workshop. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Carlyle, E. I. (1947). The life of William Cobbett. London, UK: Home & Van Thal.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. M., Snowling, M. J., Stevenson, J. & Hulme, C. (2003). The development of phonological awareness in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 39, 913923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carruthers, M. (1990). The book of memory: A study of memory in medieval culture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2007). The illusion of conscious will. Synthese, 159, 197213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W. (1985). Linguistic differences produced by differences between speaking and writing. In Olson, D. R., Torrance, N. & Hildyard, A. (Eds.), Literacy, language and learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cheng, P. W. & Holyoak, K. J. (1985). Pragmatic reasoning schemas. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 391416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clanchy, M. (1993). From memory to written record: England 1066–1307 (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clanchy, M. (1997). Abelard: A medieval life. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1988). Magic words: How language augments computation. In Carruthers, P & Boucher, J (Eds.), Language and thought. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58, 719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Cole, M. & Cole, J. (2006). Rethinking the Goody myth. In Olson, D. R. & Cole, M. (Eds.), Technology, literacy, and the evolution of society. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cooper, J. S. (2008). Babylonian beginnings: The origin of the cuneiform writing system in comparative perspective. In Houston, S. (Ed.), The first writing: Script invention as history and process (pp. 7199). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Corson, D. (1985). The lexical bar. Oxford, UK: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Coulmas, F. (2013). Writing and society. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Council of Ministers of Education for Canada. (2013). Measuring up: Canadian results of the OECD PISA study for 2012. Toronto, ON: CMEC.Google Scholar
Cowen, J. (2003). A balanced approach to beginning reading instruction. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
Crain, S. & Khlentzos, D. (2010). The logic instinct. Mind and language, 25, 3065.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cronin, V. S. (2002). The syntagmatic–paradigmatic shift and reading development. Journal of Child Language, 29(1), 189204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crupi, V. & Tentori, K. (2016). Noisy probability judgment, the conjunction fallacy and rationality. Psychological Review, 123, 97102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. (2012). Self comes to mind: Constructing the conscious brain. New York, NY: Knopf Doubleday.Google Scholar
Daniels, P. (2009). Grammatology. In Olson, D. R & Torrance, N (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of literacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (2001). Subjective, intersubjective, objective. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain. New York, NY: Viking.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1991). Consciousness explained. Toronto, ON: Little, Brown and Company.Google Scholar
Derry, J. (2008). Abstract rationality in education: From Vygotsky to Brandom. Studies in Philosophy and Education, 27, 4962.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derry, J. (2013). Vygotsky philosophy and education. Oxford, UK: Wiley Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J. (2005). Can language acquisition give children a point of view? In Astington, J. W & Baird, J (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1986a). Experience and nature. In Boydston, J. A (Ed.), John Dewey – The later works: Vol. 12. 1938. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1986b). Logic: The theory of inquiry. In Boydston, J. A (Ed.), John Dewey – The later works: Vol. 12. 1938. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (2003). Art as experience. In Boydston, J. A & Hickman, L (Eds.), John Dewey – The later works: Vol. 30. 1934. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Diringer, D. (1968). The alphabet: A key to the history of mankind (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Funk & Wagnalls.Google Scholar
Doherty, M. & Perner, J. (1998). Metalinguistic awareness and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 13, 279305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donald, M. (2012). An evolutionary approach to culture: Implications for the study of the Axial Age. In Bellah, R. N & Joas, H (Eds.), The Axial Age and its consequences. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. (1978). Children’s minds. Glasgow, UK: Fontana/Collins.Google Scholar
Downing, J. (1984). Task awareness in the development of reading skill. In Downing, J. & Valtin, R (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read (pp. 2756). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Downing, J. & Valtin, R. (Eds.). (1984). Language awareness and learning to read. New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dummett, M. (1993). Frege: Philosophy of language (3rd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Eagleman, D. (2011). Incognito: The secret lives of the brain. New York, NY: Vintage.Google Scholar
Edelman, G. (2004). Wider than the sky: The phenomenological gift of consciousness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. & Wilce, L. (1980). The influence of orthography on readers’ conceptualization of the phonemic structure of words. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 371385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
EQAO [Education Quality and Accountability Office]. (2014). (Source: EQAO: www.eqao.com/ProvincialReport/Files/14/PDF/provincial-report-elementary-findings-2014.pdf).Google Scholar
Erneling, C. (2010). Towards discursive education. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eskritt, M. & Olson, D. R. (2012). From depiction to notation: How children use symbols to represent objects and events. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13, 189207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, C. (1991). Oral metalanguage. In Olson, D. R & Torrance, N (Eds.), Literacy and orality. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ferreiro, E. (1985). Literacy development: A psychogenic perspective. In Olson, D. R., Torrance, J & Hildyard, A. (Eds.), Literacy, language and learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ferreiro, E. & Teberosky, A. (1982). Literacy before schooling. Exeter, NY: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Flavell, J., Flavell, E. & Green, E. (1983). Development of the appearance–reality distinction. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 95120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. (1972). Some reflections on L. S. Vygotsky’s Thought and Language. Cognitive Psychology, 1, 8395.Google Scholar
Foucault, M. (1970). The order of things: An archaeology of human sciences. New York, NY: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Francis, H. (1973). Children’s experience of reading and notions of units in language. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 43, 1723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, T. (2015). Easy chair. Harper’s Magazine, 330, p. 27.Google Scholar
Fromkin, V., Rodman, R. & Hymes, N. (2007). An introduction to language. Boston, MA: Thomson Publishing.Google Scholar
Frye, N. (1957). Anatomy of criticism. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gee, J. (1990). Social linguistics and literacies: Ideology in discourses. Brighton, UK: Falmer Press.Google Scholar
Gelb, I. J. (1963). A study of writing (2nd ed.). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Giedion, S. (1948). Mechanization takes command. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (1991). How to make cognitive illusions disappear. European Review of Social Psychology, 2, 83115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. & the ABC Research Group. (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gillory, J. (2004). The memo and modernity. Critical Inquiry, 31, 108132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginzburg, C. (1980). The cheese and the worms: The cosmos of a sixteenth-century miller. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Gleick, J. (1993). Genius: The life and science of Richard Feynman. New York, NY: Vintage.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L. R., Gleitman, H. & Shipley, E. F. (1972). The emergence of the child as grammarian. Cognition, 1, 137164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldhill, S. (2002). The invention of prose. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldstein, R. N. (October 8, 2015). What philosophers really know. New York Review of Books, 62, 4850.Google Scholar
Goody, J. (1977). The domestication of the savage mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Astington, J. (1988). Children’s understanding of representational change and its relationship to the understanding of false belief and the appearance–reality distinction. Child Development, 59, 2637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. & Kuhl, P. (1999). The scientist in the crib: Minds, brains, and how children learn. New York, NY: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Greeno, J. G. (1997). On claims that answer the wrong questions. Educational Researcher, 27, 517.Google Scholar
Grendler, P. F. (1989). Schooling in Renaissance Italy: Literacy and learning, 1300–1600. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the ways with words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grieve, R. (1990). Children’s awareness. In Grieve, R & Hughes, M (Eds.), Understanding children: Essays in honour of Margaret Donaldson. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hass, C. & Flower, L. (1988). Rhetorical reading strategies and the recovery of meaning. College Composition and Communication, 39, 3047.Google Scholar
Hacking, I. (1975). Why does language matter to philosophy? Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hacking, I. (1983). Representing and intervening. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hacking, I. (1996). Normal people. In Olson, D. R & Torrance, N (Eds.), Modes of thought. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1985). Spoken and written language. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Halverson, J. (1991). Olson on literacy. Language in Society, 20, 619640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanson, N. R. (1958). Patterns of discovery: An inquiry into the conceptual foundations of science. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, P. (2000). The work of the imagination. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, R. (2009). Rationality and the literate mind. London, UK: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Havelock, E. A. (1982). The literate revolution in Greece and its cultural consequences. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, J. R. & Flower, L. (1980). Identifying the organization of writing processes. In Gregg, L. W. & Steinberg, E. R. (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Heath, J. (2011). Enlightenment 2.0: Restoring sanity to our politics, our economy, and our lives. Toronto, ON: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Heath, S. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedelin, E. & Hjelmquist, E. (1988). Preschool children’s mastery of the form/content distinction in spoken language. In Ekberg, K & Mjaavatn, P (Eds.), Growing into the modern world. Trondheim: Norwegian Centre for Child Research.Google Scholar
Heeschen, V. (1978). The metalinguistic vocabulary of a speech community in the highlands of Irian Jaya (West New Guinea). In Sinclair, A, Jarvella, R. & Levelt, W (Eds.), The child’s conception of language. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Henle, M. (1962). On the relation between logic and thinking. Psychological Review, 69, 366378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Himmelmann, N. P. (2006). The challenges of segmenting spoken language. In Himmelmann, N. P. & Mosel, V. (Eds.), Essentials of language documentation. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hirsch, E. D., Jr. (1987). Cultural literacy: What every American needs to know. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M. & Murphy, P. (2005). Introduction. In Hoffman, M & Murphy, P (Eds.), Essentials of the theory of fiction. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Homer, B. (2009). Literacy and metalinguistic development. In Olson, D. R & Torrance, N. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of literacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Homer, B. & Olson, D. R. (1999). Literacy and children’s conception of words. Written Language and Literacy, 2, 113140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Householder, F. (1971). Linguistic speculations. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Huey, E. B. (1910). The psychology and pedagogy of reading. New York, NY: Appleton Century Croft.Google Scholar
Hume, D. (1978). Treatise on human nature (Nidditch, P. H., Ed.). Oxford, UK: Clarendon. (Original work published 1739.)Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J. (1964). Children’s language: Word–phrase relationship. Science, 143, 264265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of language. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janack, M. (2012). What we mean by experience. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaspers, K. (2011). The origin and goal of history. London, UK: Routledge Revivals. (Original work published 1949)Google Scholar
Jaynes, J. (1976). The origin of consciousness in the breakdown of the bicameral mind. New York, NY: Mifflin.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2006). How we reason. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Joos, M. (1967). The five clocks: Five styles of English usage. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace and World.Google Scholar
Jung, C. G. (1969). Studies in word association (trans. Eder, M. D). London, UK: Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1918)Google Scholar
Kafka, F. (1946). A little fable. In The Great Wall of China: Stories and reflections. Prague: Schocken Books.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking fast and slow. Toronto, ON: Penguin.Google Scholar
Karanth, P. & Suchitra, M. (1993). Literacy acquisition and grammaticality judgments in children. In Scholes, R (Ed.), Literacy and language analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Katz, J. & Fodor, J. (1963). The structure of a semantic theory. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, S. (2001). The knower and the known in folk pedagogies. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, OISE/University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Kneale, W. & Kneale, M. (1984). The development of logic (Rev. ed.). Oxford, UK: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Koterski, A. (2015). The silhouette of ISOTYPE [Published in Polish as: Zarys ISOTYPE’u: Idea statystyki obrazkowej Metoda Wiedenska, Przeglad Filozoficzny, NS, nr 1 (2015), 227250].Google Scholar
Kozulin, A. (1986). Introduction. In Vygotsky, L., Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kramer, S. (2912). The “eye of the mind” and the eye of the body: Descartes and Leibniz on truth, mathematics, and visuality. In Barth, F. G, Giampieri Deutsch, P & Klein, H. D (Eds.), Sensory perception: Mind and matter. Vienna: Springer.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1989). We acquire vocabulary and spelling by reading: Additional evidence for the input hypothesis. Modern Language Journal, 73, 440464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D. (2005). Education for thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, D. (2015). Thinking together and alone. Educational Researcher, 44, 4653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lagemann, E. C. (2000). The elusive science: The troubling history of educational research. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lancashire, I. (2002). “Dumb significants” and Early Modern English definition. In Brockmeier, J, Wong, M. & Olson, D. R (Eds.), Literacy, narrative and culture. Richmond, UK: Curzon.Google Scholar
Landgrafova, R. (2013). The classical art of memory as immaterial writing. Pragmatics and Cognition, 21, 505520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lantolf, J. & Poehner, M. (1994). Vygotskyan approaches to second language research. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Lantolf, J. P. & Poehner, M. E. (2014). Sociocultural theory and the pedagogical imperative in L2 education: Vygotskian praxis and the research/practice divide. London, UK: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Latour, B. & Woolgar, S. (1986). Laboratory life: The construction of scientific facts. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, E., Torrance, N. & Olson, D. R. (2001). Young children and the say/mean distinction: Verbatim and paraphrase recognition in narrative and nursery rhyme contexts. Journal of Child Language, 28, 531543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leiper, K. (2013). Education, literacy and its cognitive effects: Problem-solving and decision-making in Homer and archaic and fifth-century BC Athens. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of History, Macquarie University, Australia.Google Scholar
Leong, C. K. (1982). Reading in perspective. In Downing, J & Leong, C. K (Eds.), Psychology of reading. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. (1988). Some implications of pretense for mechanisms underlying the child’s theory of mind. In Astington, J. W, Harris, P & Olson, D. R (Eds.), Developing theories of mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leslie, S. J. & Gelman, S. A. (2012). Quantified statements are recalled as generics: Evidence from preschool children and adults. Cognitive Psychology, 64, 186214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levinson, S. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levi-Strauss, C. (1961). Tristes tropiques. New York, NY: Atheneum.Google Scholar
Levi-Strauss, C. (1963). Structural anthropology. New York, NY: Doubleday Anchor Books.Google Scholar
Liberman, I. Y. & Shankweiler, D. (1985). Phonology and the problems of learning to read and write. Remedial and Special Education, 6(6), 817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, I. & Shankweiler, D. (1991). Phonology and beginning reading: A tutorial. In Rieben, L & Perfetti, C (Eds.), Learning to read: Basic research and its implications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Linell, P. (2005). The written language bias in linguistics: Its nature, origins and transformations. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1961). An essay concerning human understanding. London, UK: Dent. (Original work published 1690)Google Scholar
Logan, R. (2004). The alphabet effect: A media ecology understanding of the making of Western civilization. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.Google Scholar
Lundberg, I., Olofsson, A. & Wall, S. (1980). Reading and spelling skills in the first years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 21, 159173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. London, UK: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lyotard, J.-F. (1984). The postmodern condition. Manchester, UK: Manchester University Press.Google Scholar
Macbeth, K. (2006). Diverse, unforeseen, and quaint difficulties: The sensible responses of novices learning to follow instructions in academic writing. Research in the Teaching of English, 41, 180207.Google Scholar
March, J. (1978). Bounded rationality, ambiguity, and the engineering of choice. The Bell Journal of Economics, 9, 587607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCallister, C. (2011). Unison reading: Socially inclusive group instruction for equity and achievement. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Sage.Google Scholar
McCullers, C. (1953). The heart is a lonely hunter. New York, NY: Bantam Books.Google Scholar
McDowell, J. (1994). Mind and world. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
McLuhan, M. (1964). Understanding media. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self and society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mehan, H. (1979). What time is it Denise? Some observations on the organization and consequences of asking known information questions in classroom discourse. Theory into Practice, 18(4), 285292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menand, L. (2001). The Metaphysical Club: A story of ideas in America. New York, NY: Farrer, Straus & Giroux.Google Scholar
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W. & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78, 622646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Misak, S. (2013). The American Pragmatists. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Montaigne, M. de. (1993). The complete essays. London: Penguin. (Original work published 1580)Google Scholar
Moore, C. (2006). The development of commonsense psychology. Mahwah, NJ: LEA.Google Scholar
Moore, J. & Newell, A. (1973). How can MERLIN understand? In Gregg, L. W. (Ed.), Knowledge and cognition. Baltimore, MD: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Morais, J., Alegria, J. & Content, A. (1987). The relationship between segmental analysis and alphabetic literacy: An interactive view. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 7, 415438.Google Scholar
Morison, E. (1966). Men, machines, and modern times. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Murphy, J. P. (1990). Pragmatism: From Peirce to Davidson. San Francisco, CA: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1986). The view from nowhere. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nagy, W. E. & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed English? Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Center for Educational Statistics. (2010). The nation’s report card: Grade 12 reading and mathematics 2009 national and pilot state results (NCES 2011–455). Washington, DC: Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.Google Scholar
National Reading Panel and National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read. Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (Ed.). (1989). Narratives in the crib. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
New York Times. (October 25, 2015). Obama administration, sharing blame, calls for limits on testing in schools (Kate Zernike, reporter).Google Scholar
Noveck, I. A. (2001). When children are more logical than adults: Experimental investigation of scalar implicatures. Cognition, 78, 165188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunez, T. & Bryant, P. (2009). Children’s reading and spelling: Beyond the first steps. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Oatley, K. (1996). Inference in narrative and science. In Olson, D. R & Torrance, N (Eds.), Modes of thought. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Oatley, K. (2013). Worlds of the possible: Abstraction, imagination, consciousness. Pragmatics and Cognition, 21, 448468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oatley, K. & Djikic, M. (2008). Writing as thinking. Review of General Psychology, 12, 927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oatley, K. & Djikic, M. (in press). The creativity of literary writing. In Kaufman, J., Baer, J. & Glaveanu, V. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity across different domains. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Olson, D. R. (1970). Language and thought: Aspects of a cognitive theory of semantics. Psychological Review, 77(4), 257273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, D. R. (1975). The languages of experience: On natural language and formal education. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 28, 363373.Google Scholar
Olson, D. R. (1977). From utterance to text: The bias of language in speech and writing. Harvard Educational Review, 47(3), 257281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, D. R. (1994). The world on paper: The conceptual and cognitive implications of writing and reading. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Olson, D. R. (2003). Psychological theory and educational reform: How schooling remakes mind and society. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Olson, D. R. (2007). The self-ascription of intention: Responsibility, obligation and self-control. Synthese, 159, 297314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, D. R. (2012). Narrative, cognition and rationality. In Gee, J. P. & Handford, M. (Eds.), Routledge handbook of discourse analysis (pp. 604615). London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Olson, D. R. & Astington, J. (1990). Talking about text: How literacy contributes to thought. Journal of Pragmatics, 14, 705721.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, D. R. & Astington, J. W. (2013). Preschool children conflate pragmatic agreement and semantic truth. First Language, 33, 617627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, D. R. & Campbell, R. (1993). Constructing representations. In Pratt, C & Garton, A. F (Eds.), Systems of representation in children: Development and use (pp. 1126). Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Olson, D. R. & Oatley, K. (2014). The quotation theory of writing. Written Communication, 31, 426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olver, R. & Hornsby, J. (1966). On equivalence. In Bruner, J. S., Olver, R. & Greenfield, P. (Eds.), Studies in cognitive growth. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ontario Teachers’ Manuals: Primary Reading. (1920). Toronto: Copp Clark.Google Scholar
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. & Tannenbaum, P. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Papafragou, A. & Musolino, J. (2003). Scalar implicatures: Experiments at the semantic–pragmatic interface. Cognition, 86, 253282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papandropoulou, I. & Sinclair, H. (1974). What is a word? Experimental study of children’s ideas on grammar. Human Development, 17, 241258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pelletier, J. (2002). Young children’s “clever misunderstandings” about print. In Brockmeier, J., Wong, M & Olson, D. R (Eds.), Literacy, narrative and culture. Richmond, UK: Curzon.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. (1991). Representations and awareness in the acquisition of reading competence. In Riben, L & Perfetti, C (Eds.), Learning to read: Basic research and its implications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J. & Roessler, J. (2012). From infants’ to children’s appreciation of belief. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 519525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peskin, J. & Astington, J. W. (2004). The effects of adding metacognitive language to story texts. Cognitive Development, 19, 253273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petsinis, T. (1997). The French mathematician. New York, NY: Walker and Company.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York, NY: HarperCollins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popper, K. (1972). Objective knowledge: An evolutionary approach. Oxford, UK: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Powell, B. B. (1991). Homer and the origin of the Greek alphabet. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. & Premack, A. (1996). Why animals lack pedagogy and why some cultures have more of it than others. In Olson, D. R. & Torrance, N. (Eds.), The handbook of education and human development. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Proctor, M. (2002). The essay as a literary and academic form: Closed gate or open door? In Brockmeier, J, Wang, M. & Olson, D. R (Eds.), Literacy, narrative and culture. Richmond, UK: Curzon.Google Scholar
Proust, J. (2013). The philosophy of metarepresentation: Mental agency and self-awareness. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Proust, M. (1982). Remembrance of things past: Vol. 1. Swann’s Way. New York, NY: Vintage.Google Scholar
Quine, W. (1960). Word and object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ravitch, D. (2014, November 20). The myth of China’s super schools. New York Review of Books, 61, 18.Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D. & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). How psychological science informs the teacher of reading. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2, 3174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Read, C., Zhang, Y., Nie, H. & Ding, B. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic reading. Cognition, 24, 3144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reid, J. (1960). Learning to think about reading. Educational Research, 9, 5662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renfrew, C., Frith, C. & Malafouris, L. (2009). The sapient mind: Archaeology meets neuroscience. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, A. D. (2011). The role of metalinguistic awareness in the effective teaching of foreign languages. New York, NY: Peter Lang.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, E., Goelman, H. & Olson, D. R. (1983). Children’s understanding of the relationship between expressions (what was said) and intentions (what was meant). British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 7586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rorty, R. (1981). Nineteenth-century idealism and twentieth-century textualism. The Monist, 64, 155174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, S. (2011, February 12). Self comes to mind: Constructing the conscious brain by Antonio Damasio – Review. Guardian. Retrieved from www.theguardian.com/books/2011/feb/12/self-comes-mind-damasio-review.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, D. (1993). Thinking that one thinks. In Davis, M & Humphries, G (Eds.), Consciousness. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rozeboom, W. (1972). Problems in the psycho-philosophy of knowledge. In Royce, J & Rozeboom, W (Eds.), The psychology of knowing. New York, NY: Gordon & Beach.Google Scholar
Sady, W. (2013). Quanta appeared not in Max Planck’s mind, but on paper. Pragmatics & Cognition, 21(3), 521529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saenger, P. (1982). Silent reading: Its impact on late medieval script and society. Viator, 13, 367414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sahlberg, P. (2011). Finnish lessons. New York, NY: Teachers College, Columbia University.Google Scholar
Saussure, F. de. (1983). Course in general linguistics. London: Duckworth. (Original work published 1916)Google Scholar
Schmandt-Besserat, D. (1986). Tokens: Facts and interpretations. Visible Language, 20, 250272.Google Scholar
Schmandt-Besserat, D. (1992). Before writing. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Schmandt-Besserat, D. (2007). When writing met art: From symbol to story. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Scribner, S. & Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. (1983). Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sellars, W. (1997). Empiricism and the philosophy of mind. Introduction by Rorty, R. and study guide by Brandom, R. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Originally published in 1956)Google Scholar
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapin, S. (1985). Pump and circumstance: Robert Boyle’s literary technology. Social Studies of Science, 14, 481520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Share, D. (2008). Orthographic learning, phonology, and the self-teaching hypothesis. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 36, 3182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1957). Models of man. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1996). The sciences of the artificial (3rd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Small, J. P. (1997). Wax tablets of the mind: Cognitive studies of memory and literacy in Classical Antiquity. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Smith, D. (1990). Texts, facts, and femininity: Exploring the relations of ruling. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Smith, F. (1971). Understanding reading. Toronto, ON: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. (Sixth edition, Routledge Classics, 2014)Google Scholar
Snow, C. & Uccelli, P. (2009). The challenge of academic language. In Olson, D. R. & Torrance, N. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of literacy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. & Hulme, C. (Eds.). (2005). The science of reading: A handbook. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sousa, R. de (2004). Rational animals. Croatian Journal of Philosophy, 4(3), 365386.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. (1994). Initial knowledge: Six suggestions. Cognition, 50, 431455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. (2003). What makes us smart? Core knowledge and natural language. In Gentner, D & Goldin-Meadow, S (Eds.), Language in mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Spiro, R., DeSchryver, M., Hagerman, M., Morsink, M. & Thompson, P. (Eds.). (2015). Reading at the crossroads? New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spratt, T. (1966). History of the Royal Society of London for the improving of natural knowledge (Cope, J. & Jones, H., Eds.). St. Louis, MO: Washington University Press. (Original work published 1667)Google Scholar
Standish, P. (2013). Series editor’s preface. In Derry, J., Vygotsky philosophy and education. Oxford, UK: Wiley Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. (2009). What intelligence tests miss: The psychology of rational thought. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. & West, R. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, C. (1922). Moonlight schools: For the emancipation of adult illiterates. New York, NY: E. P. Dutton & Company.Google Scholar
Stock, B. (2015). Savoir lire et ecrire en perspective globale: Biologie, culture et societe. In Wieviorka, M, Levi-Strauss, L & Lieppe, G (Eds.), Penser global: Internationalisation et globalization des sciences humaines et sociales. Paris: Editions de la Maison des sciences de l’homme.Google Scholar
Street, B. (1984). Literacy in theory and practice. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, C. (2012). What was the Axial Revolution? In Bellah, R. N & Joas, H (Eds.), The Axial Age and its consequences. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Tetlock, P. E. & Gardner, D. (2014). Superforecasting: The art and science of prediction. New York, NY: Crown Publishers.Google Scholar
Theiner, G. (2011). Res Cogitans Extensa. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Tolchinsky, L. (2003). The cradle of culture and what children know about writing and numbers before being taught. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolchinsky-Landsmann, L. & Levin, I. (1987). Writing in four- to six-year-olds: Representation of semantic and phonetic similarities and differences. Journal of Child Language, 14, 127144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torrance, N. & Olson, D. R. (1985). Oral and literate competencies in the early school years. In Olson, D. R, Torrance, N & Hildyard, A (Eds.), Literacy, language, and learning: The nature and consequences of reading and writing (pp. 256284). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Traugott, E. C. (1987). Literacy and language change. Interchange: A Quarterly Review of Education, 18, 3247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treiman, R. & Zukowsky, A. (1991). Levels of phonological awareness. In Brady, S. A & Shankweiler, D. P (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 6783). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tunmer, W. & Bowey, J. (1984). Metalinguistic awareness and reading acquisition. In Tunmer, W., Pratt, C. & Herriman, M. (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness in children. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tunmer, W., Pratt, C. & Herriman, M. (1984). Metalinguistic awareness in children: Theory, research, and implications. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, M. & Thomas, F.-N. (2011). Clear and simple as the truth: Writing about prose. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Uccelli, P., Barr, C. D., Dodds, C. L., Phillips Galloway, E., Meneses, A. & Sanchez, E. (2014). Core academic language skills (CALS): An expanded operational construct and a novel instrument to chart school relevant language proficiency in pre-adolescent and adolescent learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 10771109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valtin, R. (2014). About the dubious role of phonological awareness in the discussion of literacy policies. Paper presented in the FELA endorsed symposium “Kindergarten literacy in Europe” at the 18th European Conference on Reading, Mons, Belgium, August 2011.Google Scholar
Van Dijk, T. & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Vellutino, F. (1979). Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vendler, Z. (1970). Say what you think. In Cowan, J. L. (Ed.), Studies in thought and language. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.Google Scholar
Vernon, S. (2002). Children’s analysis of oral and written words. In Brockmeier, J., Wang, M. & Olson, D. R. (Eds.), Literacy, narratives and culture. London, UK: Curzon.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes (Cole, M. et al., Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (Kozulin, A., Ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Watson, P. (2014). The age of atheists: How we have sought to live since the death of God. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Watson, R. & Horowitz, W. (2011). Writing science before the Greeks: A naturalistic analysis of the Babylonian astronomical treatise MUL. APIN. Leiden: Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weir, R. (1962). Language in the crib. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Wells, G. (1999). Dialogic inquiry: Towards a socio-cultural practice and theory of education. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wertsch, J. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wertsch, J., Tulviste, P. & Hagstrom, F. (1993). A sociocultural approach to agency.In Forman, E., Minick, N. & Stone, C. (Eds.),Contexts for learning (pp. 336356). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiersbicka, A. (2010). Experience, evidence, and sense: The hidden cultural legacy of English. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. (1965). Stoner. New York, NY: Viking.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (2014). The meaning of human existence. New York, NY: Liveright/Norton.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. & Keil, F. (2000). The shadows and shallows of explanation. In Keil, F. & Wilson, R. (Eds.), Explanation and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiseberg, J. (2014). Consciousness. Cambridge, UK: Polity.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1958). Philosophical investigations. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wolf, M. (2008). Proust and the squid: The story and science of the reading brain. New York, NY: Harper Perennial.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D. & Jacques, S. (1996). Children’s rule use: Representation, reflection, and cognitive control. Annals of Child Development, 12, 119176.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • David R. Olson, University of Toronto
  • Book: The Mind on Paper
  • Online publication: 24 November 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316678466.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • David R. Olson, University of Toronto
  • Book: The Mind on Paper
  • Online publication: 24 November 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316678466.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • David R. Olson, University of Toronto
  • Book: The Mind on Paper
  • Online publication: 24 November 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316678466.015
Available formats
×