Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-zzh7m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T13:23:33.776Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part I - Process and Structure of the Creative Personality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 May 2017

Gregory J. Feist
Affiliation:
San José State University, California
Roni Reiter-Palmon
Affiliation:
University of Nebraska, Omaha
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Adelstein, J. S., Shehzad, Z., Mennes, M., DeYoung, C. G., Zuo, X. N., Kelly, C., et al. (2011). Personality is reflected in the brain’s intrinsic functional architecture. PloS ONE, 6(11), e27633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allport, G. W., & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait-names: a psycho-lexical study. Psychological Monographs, 47(1), 171220.Google Scholar
Alpaugh, P. K., Parham, I. A., Cole, K. D., & Birren, J. E. (1982). Creativity in adulthood and old age: an exploratory study. Educational Gerontology: An International Quarterly, 8(2), 101–16.Google Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J., & Spreng, R. N. (2014). The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences USA, 1316(1), 2952.Google Scholar
Baer, J. (1998). The case for domain specificity of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 11(2), 173–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baer, J. (2010). Is Creativity Domain Specific? In Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity (p. 321). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Barron, F., & Harrington, D. M. (1981). Creativity, intelligence, and personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 32(1), 439–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baruch, I., Hemsley, D. R., & Gray, J. A. (1988a). Differential performance of acute and chronic schizophrenics in a latent inhibition task. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 176(10), 598606.Google Scholar
Baruch, I., Hemsley, D. R., & Gray, J. A. (1988b). Latent inhibition and “psychotic proneness” in normal subjects. Personality and Individual Differences, 9(4), 777–83.Google Scholar
Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence, and personality: a critical review of the scattered literature. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 132(4), 355429.Google Scholar
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (in press a). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences.Google Scholar
Beaty, R. E., Kaufman, S. B., Benedek, M., Jung, R. E., Kenett, Y. N., et al. (in press b). Personality and complex brain networks: the role of openness to experience in default network efficiency. Human Brain Mapping.Google Scholar
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Sommer, M., Arendasy, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: the common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence, 46, 7383.Google Scholar
Benedek, M., Mühlmann, C., Jauk, E., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). Assessment of divergent thinking by means of the subjective top-scoring method: effects of the number of top-ideas and time-on-task on reliability and validity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(4), 341.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences USA, 1124 (1), 138.Google Scholar
Carson, S. H. (2011). Creativity and psychopathology: a shared vulnerability model. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56(3), 144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 499506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Reliability, validity, and factor structure of the Creative Achievement Questionnaire. Creativity Research Journal, 17(1), 3750.Google Scholar
Chang, L., Connelly, B. S., & Geeza, A. A. (2012). Separating method factors and higher order traits of the Big Five: a meta-analytic multitrait–multimethod approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(2), 408.Google Scholar
Chermahini, S. A., & Hommel, B. (2010). The (b) link between creativity and dopamine: spontaneous eye blink rates predict and dissociate divergent and convergent thinking. Cognition, 115(3), 458–65.Google Scholar
Coan, R. W. (1972). Measurable components of openness to experience. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 39(2), 346.Google Scholar
Connelly, B. S., Ones, D. S., Davies, S. E., & Birkland, A. (2014). Opening up openness: a theoretical sort following critical incidents methodology and a meta-analytic investigation of the trait family measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 96(1), 1728.Google Scholar
Conner, T. S., Silvia, P. J. B., Gray, J. R., Peterson, J. B., & DeYoung, C. G. (2015). Creative days: a daily diary study of emotion, personality, and everyday creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9(4), 463–70.Google Scholar
Costa, P. T. Jr, & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO PI-R Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
De Manzano, O., Cervenka, S., Karabanov, A., Farde, L., & Ullen, F. (2010). Thinking outside a less intact box: thalamic dopamine D2 receptor densities are negatively related to psychometric creativity in healthy individuals. PLoS ONE, 5(5), e10670.Google Scholar
Depue, R. A., Luciana, M., Arbisi, P., Collins, P., & Leon, A. (1994). Dopamine and the structure of personality: relation of agonist-induced dopamine activity to positive emotionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(3), 485.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G. (2006). Higher-order factors of the Big Five in a multi-informant sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(6), 1138–51.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G. (2013). The neuromodulator of exploration: a unifying theory of the role of dopamine in personality. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeYoung, C. G. (2015). Cybernetic big five theory. Journal of Research in Personality, 56, 3358.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G. (2015). Openness/Intellect: A Dimension of Personality Reflecting Cognitive Exploration. In Cooper, M. L. & Larsen, R. J. (eds.), APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology: Personality Processes and Individual Differences (Vol. 4, pp. 369–99). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Grazioplene, R. G., & Peterson, J. B. (2012). From madness to genius: the openness/intellect trait domain as a paradoxical simplex. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(1), 6378.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Sources of openness/intellect: cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factors of personality. Journal of Personality, 73(4), 825–58.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 880–96.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Shamosh, N. A., Green, A. E., Braver, T. S., & Gray, J. R. (2009). Intellect as distinct from openness: differences revealed by fMRI of working memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(5), 883–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollinger, S. J., Urban, K. K., & James, T. A. (2004). Creativity and openness: further validation of two creative product measures. Creativity Research Journal, 16(1), 3547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(4), 290309.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J., & Barron, F. X. (2003). Predicting creativity from early to late adulthood: intellect, potential, and personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(2), 6288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiske, D. W. (1949). Consistency of the factorial structures of personality ratings from different sources. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 44(3), 329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitzgerald, E. T. (1966). Measurement of openness to experience: a study of regression in the service of the ego. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4(6), 655–63.Google Scholar
Ghisletta, P., & Lubart, T. (2014). Toward an integrative model of creativity and personality: theoretical suggestions and preliminary empirical testing. Journal of Creative Behavior, 50(2), 87108.Google Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”: the Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(6), 1216–29.Google Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A Broad-Bandwidth, Public Domain, Personality Inventory Measuring the Lower-Level Facets of Several Five-Factor Models. In Mervielde, I., Deary, I., De Fruyt, F., & Ostendorf, F. (eds.), Personality Psychology in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 728). Tilburg, Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.Google Scholar
Grazioplene, R. G., Chavez, R. S., Rustichini, A., & DeYoung, C. G. (2016). Personality, psychosis, and connectivity: white matter correlates of psychosis-linked traits support continuity between personality and psychopathology. Submitted.Google Scholar
Greenberg, D. M., Müllensiefen, D., Lamb, M. E., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2015). Personality predicts musical sophistication. Journal of Research in Personality, 58, 154–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jang, K. L., Hu, S., Livesley, W. J., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., et al. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on the covariance of facets defining the domains of the five-factor model of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 33(1), 83101.Google Scholar
Jauk, E., Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014). The road to experience and intellect differentially predict creative achievement in the arts and sciences: a latent variable model of ability and personality predictors. European Journal of Personality.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
John, O.P. (1990). The “Big Five” Factor Taxonomy: Dimensions of Personality in the Natural Language and in Questionnaires. In Pervin, L. A. (ed.), Handbook of Personality, Theory and Research (pp. 66100). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm Shift to the Integrative Big Five Trait Taxonomy: History, Measurement, and Conceptual Issue. In John, O. P., Robins, R. W., & Pervin, L. A. (eds), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (pp. 114– 58). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, J. A. (1994). Clarification of factor five with the help of the AB5C model. European Journal of Personality, 8(4), 311–34.Google Scholar
Johnson, J. A., & Ostendorf, F. (1993). Clarification of the five-factor model with the Abridged Big Five Dimensional Circumplex. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(3), 563.Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Grazioplene, R., Caprihan, A., Chavez, R. S., & Haier, R. J. (2010). White matter integrity, creativity, and psychopathology: disentangling constructs with diffusion tensor imaging. PloS ONE, 5(3), e9818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Karwowski, M., & Lebuda, I. (2015). The Big Five, the Huge Two, and creative self-beliefs: a meta-analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Baer, J. (2004). Sure, I’m creative – but not in mathematics!: self-reported creativity in diverse domains. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 22(2), 143–55.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.). (2010). The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., Waterstreet, M. A., Ailabouni, H. S., Whitcomb, H. J., Roe, A. K., et al. (2010). Personality and self-perceptions of creativity across domains. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 29(3), 193209.Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B. (2011). Intelligence and the Cognitive Unconscious. In Sternberg, R. J. & Kaufman, S. B. (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 442–67). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B. (2013). Opening up openness to experience: a four-factor model and relations to creative achievement in the arts and sciences. Journal of Creative Behavior, 47(4), 233–55.Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Jimenez, L., Brown, J. B., et al. (2010). Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition, 116(3), 321–40.Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., Quilty, L. C., Grazioplene, R. G., Hirsh, J. B., Gray, J. R., et al. (2015). Openness to experience and intellect differentially predict creative achievement in the arts and sciences. Journal of Personality.Google Scholar
King, L. A., Walker, L. M., & Broyles, S. J. (1996). Creativity and the five-factor model. Journal of Research in Personality, 30(2), 189203.Google Scholar
Kozbelt, A., Kaufman, S. B., Walder, D. J., Ospina, L. H., & Kim, J. (2014). The Evolutionary Genetics of the Creativity-Psychosis Connection. In Kaufman, J. C. (ed.), Creativity and Mental Illness. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kühn, S., Ritter, S. M., Müller, B. C., Baaren, R. B., Brass, M., et al. (2014). The importance of the default mode network in creativity – a structural MRI study. Journal of Creative Behavior, 48(2), 152–63.Google Scholar
Kyaga, S., Landén, M., Boman, M., Hultman, C. M., Långström, N., et al. (2013). Mental illness, suicide and creativity: 40-year prospective total population study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47(1), 8390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the HEXACO personality inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(2), 329–58.Google Scholar
Lubow, R. E. (1989). Latent Inhibition and Conditioned Attention Theory (Vol. 9). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E., Ingberg-Sachs, Y., Zalstein-Orda, N., & Gewirtz, J. C. (1992). Latent inhibition in low and high “psychotic-prone” normal subjects. Personality and Individual Differences, 13(5), 563–72.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1258–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Navas-Sánchez, F. J., Alemán-Gómez, Y., Sánchez-Gonzalez, J., Guzmán-De-Villoria, J. A., Franco, C., et al. (2014). White matter microstructure correlates of mathematical giftedness and intelligence quotient. Human Brain Mapping, 35(6), 2619–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettle, D. (2006). Schizotypy and mental health amongst poets, visual artists, and mathematicians. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(6), 876–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, B., & Rawlings, D. (2010). Relating schizotypy and personality to the phenomenology of creativity. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(2), 388–99.Google Scholar
Norman, W. T. (1963). Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes: replicated factor structure in peer nomination personality ratings. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66(6), 574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oleynick, V. C., Thrash, T. M., LeFew, M. C., Moldovan, E. G., & Kieffaber, P. D. (2014). The scientific study of inspiration in the creative process: challenges and opportunities. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8.Google Scholar
Passamonti, L., Terracciano, A., Riccelli, R., Donzuso, G., Cerasa, A., et al. (2015). Increased functional connectivity within mesocortical networks in open people. Neuroimage, 104, 301–9.Google Scholar
Peabody, D., & Goldberg, L. R. (1989). Some determinants of factor structures from personality-trait descriptors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(3), 552.Google Scholar
Penke, L., Maniega, S. M., Bastin, M. E., Valdés Hernández, M. C., Murray, C., et al. (2012). Brain white matter tract integrity as a neural foundation for general intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry, 17(10), 1026–30.Google Scholar
Perrine, N. E., & Brodersen, R. (2005). Artistic and scientific creative behavior: openness and the mediating role of interests. Journal of Creative Behavior, 39(4), 217–36.Google Scholar
Peterson, J. B., Smith, K. W., & Carson, S. (2002). Openness and extraversion are associated with reduced latent inhibition: replication and commentary. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 1137–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plucker, J. A., & Makel, M. C. (2010). Assessment of Creativity. In Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity (pp. 4873). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Poe, E. A. (1899). The Mystery of Marie Roget. New York: R. F. Fenno.Google Scholar
Reber, A. S., Walkenfeld, F. F., & Hernstadt, R. (1991). Implicit and explicit learning: individual differences and IQ. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 888–96.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Albert, R. S. (1986). Exceptional giftedness in early adolescence and intrafamilial divergent thinking. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 15(4), 335–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saucier, G. (1992). Openness versus intellect: much ado about nothing? European Journal of Personality, 6(5), 381–6.Google Scholar
Saucier, G. (1994). Trapnell versus the lexical factor: more ado about nothing? European Journal of Personality, 8(4), 291–8.Google Scholar
Saucier, G. (2009). Recurrent personality dimensions in inclusive lexical studies: Indications for a Big Six structure. Journal of Personality, 77(5), 15771614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saucier, G., & Ostendorf, F. (1999). Hierarchical subcomponents of the Big Five personality factors: a cross-language replication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(4), 613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silvia, P. J., Beaty, R. E., Nusbaum, E. C., Eddington, K. M., Levin-Aspenson, H., et al. (2014). Everyday creativity in daily life: an experience-sampling study of “little c” creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(2), 183–8.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Pretz, J. E. (2009). Is creativity domain-specific? Latent class models of creative accomplishments and creative self-descriptions. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3(3), 139–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Berg, C., Martin, C., & O’Connor, A. (2009). Openness to experience, plasticity, and creativity: exploring lower-order, high-order, and interactive effects. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(6), 1087–90.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Wigert, B., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Kaufman, J. C. (2012). Assessing creativity with self-report scales: a review and empirical evaluation. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(1), 1934.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Willse, J. T., Barona, C. M., Cram, J. T., et al. (2008). Assessing creativity with divergent thinking tasks: exploring the reliability and validity of new subjective scoring methods. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2(2), 6885.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2003). Scientific creativity as constrained stochastic behavior: the integration of product, person, and process perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4), 475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., & Kim, A. S. (2009). The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 489510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., Sassa, Y., Nagase, T., et al. (2012). The association between resting functional connectivity and creativity. Cerebral Cortex, 22(12), 2921–9.Google Scholar
Tellegen, A., & Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83(3), 268–77.Google Scholar
Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2003). Inspiration as a psychological construct. Journal of Personality and Social psychology, 84(4), 871.Google Scholar
Thrash, T. M., Maruskin, L. A., Cassidy, S. E., Fryer, J. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). Mediating between the muse and the masses: inspiration and the actualization of creative ideas. Journal of Personality and Social psychology, 98(3), 469.Google Scholar
Tupes, E. C., & Christal, R. E. (1961). Recurrent personality factors based on trait ratings (ASD-TR-61–97). Lackland Air Force Base, TX: Aeronautical Systems Division, Personnel Laboratory (reprinted in 1992, Journal of Personality, 60, 225–51).Google Scholar
Wolfradt, U., & Pretz, J. E. (2001). Individual differences in creativity: personality, story writing, and hobbies. European Journal of Personality, 15(4), 297310.Google Scholar
Woo, S. E., Chernyshenko, O. S., Longley, A., Zhang, Z. X., Chiu, C. Y., et al. (2014). Openness to experience: its lower level structure, measurement, and cross-cultural equivalence. Journal of Personality Assessment, 96(1), 2945.Google Scholar
Yeo, B. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari, D., et al. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(3), 1125–65.Google Scholar
Zacks, R. T., Hasher, L., & Sanft, H. (1982). Automatic encoding of event frequency: further findings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8(2), 106.Google Scholar
Zillig, L. M. P., Hemenover, S. H., & Dienstbier, R. A. (2002). What do we assess when we assess a Big 5 trait? A content analysis of the affective, behavioral, and cognitive processes represented in Big 5 personality inventories. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(6), 847–58.Google Scholar

References

Abraham, A., Beudt, S., Ott, D. V., & Yves von Cramon, D. (2012). Creative cognition and the brain: dissociations between frontal, parietal-temporal and basal ganglia groups. Brain Research, 1482, 5570. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2012.09.007Google Scholar
Adelstein, J. S., Shehzad, Z., Mennes, M., Deyoung, C. G., Zuo, X. N., et al. (2011). Personality is reflected in the brain’s intrinsic functional architecture. PloS One, 6(11), e27633. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0027633Google Scholar
Allen, T. A., & DeYoung, C. G. (2016). Personality Neuroscience and the Five Factor Model. In Widiger, T. A. (ed.), Oxford Handbook of the Five Factor Model. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron, 65, 550–62.Google Scholar
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2015a). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(2), 8795. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.004Google Scholar
Beaty, R. E., Kaufman, S. B., Benedek, M., Jung, R. E., Kenett, Y. N., et al. (2015b). Personality and complex brain networks: the role of openness to experience in default network efficiency. Human Brain Mapping, 37(2), 773–9. doi:10.1002/hbm.23065Google Scholar
Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 147.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B., & Drevdahl, J. E. (1955). A comparison of the personality profile (16 P.F.) of eminent researchers with that of eminent teachers and administrators, and of the general population. British Journal of Psychology, 46(4), 248–61.Google Scholar
Cohen, M. X., Schoene-Bake, J. C., Elger, C. E., & Weber, B. (2009). Connectivity-based segregation of the human striatum predicts personality characteristics. Nature Neuroscience, 12(1), 32–4. doi:10.1038/nn.2228Google Scholar
Danielian, L. E., Iwata, N. K., Thomasson, D. M., & Floeter, M. K. (2010). Reliability of fiber tracking measurements in diffusion tensor imaging for longitudinal study. Neuroimage, 49(2), 1572–80. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.062Google Scholar
Deary, I. J. (2012). Intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 453–82. doi: 10.1146/Annurev-Psych-120710-100353Google Scholar
Deary, I. J., Penke, L., & Johnson, W. (2010). The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(3), 201–11. doi: 10.1038/Nrn2793Google Scholar
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(3), 491517; discussion 518–19.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G. (2006). Higher-order factors of the Big Five in a multi-informant sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(6), 1138–51. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.6.1138Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., & Grazioplene, R. G. (2013). “They who dream by day”: parallels between openness to experience and dreaming. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 615; discussion 634–59. doi:10.1017/S0140525X13001283Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Rajeevan, N. et al. (2010). Testing predictions from personality neuroscience brain structure and the Big Five. Psychological Science, 21, 820–8.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2002). Higher-order factors of the Big Five predict conformity: Are there neuroses of health? Personality and Individual Differences, 33(4), 533–52.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. G., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Sources of openness/intellect: cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. Journal of Personality, 73(4), 825–58.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 880896.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Shamosh, N. A., Green, A. E., Braver, T. S., & Gray, J. R. (2009). Intellect as distinct from openness: differences revealed by fMRI of working memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(5), 883–92. doi:10.1037/a0016615Google Scholar
Dietrich, A. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 1011–26.Google Scholar
Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(6), 1246–56.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Biological Dimensions of Personality. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. J. (1976). Psychoticism as a Dimension of Personality. London: Hodder & Stoughton.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(4), 290309.Google Scholar
Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., Ellamil, M., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Christoff, K. (2015). The wandering brain: meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. Neuroimage, 111, 611–21. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.039Google Scholar
Friedman, S. D., Brooks, W. M., Jung, R. E., Chiulli, S. J., Sloan, J. H., et al. (1999). Quantitative proton MRS predicts outcome after traumatic brain injury. Neurology, 52(7), 1384–91.Google Scholar
Friedman, S. D., Brooks, W. M., Jung, R. E., Hart, B. L., & Yeo, R. A. (1998). Proton MR spectroscopic findings correspond to neuropsychological function in traumatic brain injury. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 19(10), 1879–85.Google Scholar
Gasparovic, C., Bedrick, E. J., Mayer, A. R., Yeo, R. A., Chen, H., et al. (2010). Test-retest reliability and reproducibility of short-echo-time spectroscopic imaging of human brain at 3T. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 66(2), 324–32. doi:10.1002/mrm.22858Google Scholar
Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., & Braver, T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 316–22.Google Scholar
Green, A. E., Fugelsang, J. A., Kraemer, D. J., Shamosh, N. A., & Dunbar, K. N. (2006). Frontopolar cortex mediates abstract integration in analogy. Brain Research, 1096(1), 125–37. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.04.024Google Scholar
Green, A. E., Kraemer, D. J., Fugelsang, J. A., Gray, J. R., & Dunbar, K. N. (2010). Connecting long distance: semantic distance in analogical reasoning modulates frontopolar cortex activity. Cerebral Cortex, 20(1), 70–6. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp081Google Scholar
Grove, M., Pearce, E., & Dunbar, R. I. (2012). Fission-fusion and the evolution of hominin social systems. Journal of Human Evolution, 62(2), 191200. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2011.10.012Google Scholar
Hirsh, J. B., Deyoung, C. G., & Peterson, J. B. (2009). Metatraits of the Big Five differentially predict engagement and restraint of behavior. Journal of Personality, 77(4), 10851102. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00575.xGoogle Scholar
Ishizu, T., & Zeki, S. (2013). The brain’s specialized systems for aesthetic and perceptual judgment. European Journal of Neuroscience, 37(9), 1413–20. doi:10.1111/ejn.12135Google Scholar
Jung, C. G. (1921). Psychologische Typen. Zurich: Rascher Verlag.Google Scholar
Jung, R. E. (2014). Evolution, creativity, intelligence, and madness: “Here Be Dragons”. Frontiers of Psychology, 5, 784. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00784Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Brooks, W. M., Chiulli, S. J., Weers, D. C., Yeo, R. A., et al. (1999a). The biochemical markers of intelligence and cognition in normal human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1999, 61.Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Brooks, W. M., Yeo, R. A., Chiulli, S. J., Weers, D. C., et al. (1999b). Biochemical markers of intelligence: a proton MR spectroscopy study of normal human brain. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 266(1426), 1375–9.Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Chiulli, S. J., Yeo, R. A., & Brooks, W. M. (1999c). “Myths of neuropsychology”: intelligence and cognitive ability revisited. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14(8), 720–1.Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Gasparovic, C., Chavez, R. S., Caprihan, A., Barrow, R., et al. (2009a). Imaging intelligence with proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Intelligence, 37(2), 192–8. doi: 10.1016/J.Intell.2008.10.009Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Gasparovic, C., Chavez, R. S., Flores, R. A., Smith, S. M., et al. (2009b). Biochemical support for the “threshold” theory of creativity: a magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(16), 5319–25. doi: 10.1523/Jneurosci.0588-09.2009Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Grazioplene, R., Caprihan, A., Chavez, R. S., & Haier, R. J. (2010a). White matter integrity, creativity, and psychopathology: disentangling constructs with diffusion tensor imaging. PloS ONE, 5(3), e9818. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009818Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2007). The parieto-frontal integration theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(2), 135. doi: 10.1017/S0140525x07001185.Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Haier, R. J., Yeo, R. A., Rowland, L. M., Petropoulos, H., et al. (2005). Sex differences in N-acetylaspartate correlates of general intelligence: an H-1-MRS study of normal human brain. Neuroimage, 26(3), 965–72. doi: 10.1016/J.Neuroimage.2005.02.039Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Mead, B. S., Carrasco, J., & Flores, R. A. (2013). The structure of creative cognition in the human brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 330. doi: 10.3389/Fnhum.2013.00330Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Segall, J. M., Bockholt, H. J., Flores, R. A., Smith, S. M., et al. (2010b). Neuroanatomy of creativity. Human Brain Mapping, 31(3), 398409. doi: 10.1002/Hbm.20874Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Yeo, R. A., Chiulli, S. J., Sibbitt, W. L., Weers, D. C., et al. (1999d). Biochemical markers of cognition: a proton MR spectroscopy study of normal human brain. Neuroreport, 10(16), 3327–31. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199911080-00014Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Yeo, R. A., Sibbitt, W. L., Ford, C. C., Hart, B. L., et al. (2001). Gerstmann syndrome in systemic lupus erythematosus: neuropsychological, neuroimaging and spectroscopic findings. Neurocase, 7(6), 515–21. doi: 10.1093/Neucas/7.6.515Google Scholar
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(4), 637–71.Google Scholar
Krain, A. L., Wilson, A. M., Arbuckle, R., Castellanos, F. X., & Milham, M. P. (2006). Distinct neural mechanisms of risk and ambiguity: a meta-analysis of decision-making. Neuroimage, 32(1), 477–84. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.047Google Scholar
Kugler, L., & Laub, M. (1971). “Puppet show” theta rhythm. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 31, 532–3.Google Scholar
Le Bihan, D. (2003). Looking into the functional architecture of the brain with diffusion MRI. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 469–80.Google Scholar
Li, W., Li, X., Huang, L., Kong, X., Yang, W., et al. (2015). Brain structure links trait creativity to openness to experience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(2), 191–8. doi:10.1093/scan/nsu041Google Scholar
Lu, H., Zou, Q., Gu, H., Raichle, M. E., Stein, E. A., et al. (2012). Rat brains also have a default mode network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 109(10), 3979–84. doi:10.1073/pnas.1200506109Google Scholar
Maulsby, R. L. (1971). An illustration of emotionally evoked theta rhythm in infancy: hedonic hypersynchrony. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 31(2), 157–65.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Ingraham, L. J. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1258–65.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60(2), 175215.Google Scholar
McIntosh, A. M., Maniega, S. M., Lymer, G. K. S., McKirdy, J., Hall, J., et al. (2008). White matter tractography in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 1088–92.Google Scholar
Mellars, P. (2004). Neanderthals and the modern human colonization of Europe. Nature, 432(7016), 461–5. doi:10.1038/nature03103Google Scholar
Mellars, P. (2006). Why did modern human populations disperse from Africa ca. 60,000 years ago? A new model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 103(25), 9381–6. doi:10.1073/pnas.0510792103Google Scholar
Niendam, T. A., Laird, A. R., Ray, K. L., Dean, Y. M., Glahn, D. C., et al. (2012). Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 12(2), 241–68. doi:10.3758/s13415-011-0083-5Google Scholar
Patel, T., & Talcott, J. B. (2014). Moderate relationships between NAA and cognitive ability in healthy adults: implications for cognitive spectroscopy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(39), 110.Google Scholar
Popa, D., Popescu, A. T., & Pare, D. (2009). Contrasting activity profile of two distributed cortical networks as a function of attentional demands. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(4), 1191–201. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4867-08.2009Google Scholar
Potts, R. (2002). Complexity and Adaptability in Human Evolution. In Moffat, M. G. a. A. S. (ed.), Probing Human Origins. Cambridge, MA: American Academy of Arts and Sciences.Google Scholar
Prabhakaran, V., Smith, J. A., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., & Gabrieli, J. D. (1997). Neural substrates of fluid reasoning: an fMRI study of neocortical activation during performance of the Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 4363.Google Scholar
Raichle, M. E. (2015). The brain’s default mode network. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 38, 433–47.Google Scholar
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., et al. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 676–82.Google Scholar
Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443–7.Google Scholar
Ross, A. J., & Sachdev, P. S. (2004). Magnetic resonance spectroscopy in cognitive research. Brain Research Reviews, 44, 83102.Google Scholar
Rowland, L. M., Mullins, P. G., Jung, R. E., Lenroot, R., Lauriello, J., et al. (2003). Neurochemistry in chronic schizophrenia: a 4T-proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Schizophrenia Research, 60(1), 245–55. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)81257-4Google Scholar
Ryman, S. G., Gasparovic, C., Bedrick, E. J., Flores, R. A., Marshall, A. N., et al. (2011). Brain biochemistry and personality: a magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. PloS ONE, 6(11), e26758. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026758Google Scholar
Sampaio, A., Soares, J. M., Coutinho, J., Sousa, N., & Goncalves, O. F. (2014). The Big Five default brain: functional evidence. Brain Structure & Function, 219(6), 1913–22. doi:10.1007/s00429-013-0610-yGoogle Scholar
Shulman, G. L., Fiez, J. A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R. L., Miezin, F. M., et al. (1997). Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(5), 648–63. doi:10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.648Google Scholar
Stafford, J. M., Jarrett, B. R., Miranda-Dominguez, O., Mills, B. D., Cain, N., et al. (2014). Large-scale topology and the default mode network in the mouse connectome. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(52), 18745–50. doi:10.1073/pnas.1404346111Google Scholar
Stawarczyk, D., & D’Argembeau, A. (2015). Neural correlates of personal goal processing during episodic future thinking and mind-wandering: an ALE meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 36(8), 2928–47. doi:10.1002/hbm.22818Google Scholar
Stough, C., Donaldson, C., Scarlata, B., & Ciorciari, J. (2001). Psychophysiological correlates of the NEO PI-R Openness, Agreeableness and Conscientiousness: preliminary results. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 41(1), 8791. doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00176-8Google Scholar
Sutin, A. R., Beason-Held, L. L., Resnick, S. M., & Costa, P. T. (2009). Sex differences in resting-state neural correlates of openness to experience among older adults. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 2797–802. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp066Google Scholar
Swick, D., & Turken, A. U. (2002). Dissociation between conflict detection and error monitoring in the human anterior cingulate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(25), 16354–9. doi:10.1073/pnas.252521499Google Scholar
Taki, Y., Thyreau, B., Kinomura, S., Sato, K., Goto, R., et al. (2013). A longitudinal study of the relationship between personality traits and the annual rate of volume changes in regional gray matter in healthy adults. Human Brain Mapping, 34(12), 3347–53. doi:10.1002/hbm.22145Google Scholar
Vessel, E. A., Starr, G. G., & Rubin, N. (2013). Art reaches within: aesthetic experience, the self and the default mode network. Frontiers of Neuroscience, 7, 258. doi:10.3389/fnins.2013.00258Google Scholar
Vincent, J. L., Patel, G. H., Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Baker, J. T., et al. (2007). Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature, 447(7140), 83–6. doi:10.1038/nature05758Google Scholar
Wren, C. D., Xue, J. Z., Costopoulos, A., & Burke, A. (2014). The role of spatial foresight in models of hominin dispersal. Journal of Human Evolution, 69, 70–8. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2014.02.004Google Scholar
Wynn, T., & Coolidge, F. L. (2010). Beyond symbolism and language. Current Anthropology, 51(1), S5–16.Google Scholar
Xu, J., & Potenza, M. N. (2012). White matter integrity and five-factor personality measures in healthy adults. Neuroimage, 59(1), 800–7. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.040Google Scholar

References

Ackerman, P. L. (1996). A theory of adult intellectual development: process, personality, interests, and knowledge. Intelligence, 22(2), 227–57.Google Scholar
Ackerman, P. L., Bowen, K. R., Beier, M., & Kanfer, R. (2001). Determinants of individual differences and gender differences in knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(4), 797825.Google Scholar
Ackerman, P. L., & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and interests: evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin, 121(2), 219–45.Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1982). Social psychology of creativity: a consensual assessment technique. Personality Processes and Individual Differences, 43(5), 9971013.Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity: a componential conceptualization. Personality Processes and Individual Differences, 45(2), 357–76.Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1988). A model of creativity and innovation in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 10, 123–67.Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M., Conti, R., Coon, H., Lazenby, J., & Herron, M. (1996). Assessing the work environment for creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 39(5), 1154–84.Google Scholar
Assouline, M., & Meir, E. I. (1987). Meta-analysis of the relationship between congruence and well-being measures. Journal of Vocational Behaviors, 31(3), 319–32.Google Scholar
Baer, J. (1991). Generality of creativity across performance domains. Creativity Research Journal, 4(1), 2339.Google Scholar
Baer, J. (2010). Is Creativity Domain Specific? In Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Creativity (pp. 321–41). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Baer, J. (2012). Domain specificity and the limits of creativity theory. Journal of Creative Behavior, 46(1), 1629.Google Scholar
Baer, M., & Oldham, G. R. (2006). The curvilinear relationship between experienced creative time pressure and creativity: moderating effects of openness to experience and support for creativity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 963–70.Google Scholar
Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Gupta, R. (2003). Meta-analysis of the relationship between the five-factor model of personality and Holland’s occupational types. Personnel Psychology, 56, 4574.Google Scholar
Batey, M. (2012). The measurement of creativity: from definitional consensus to the introduction of a new heuristic framework. Creativity Research Journal, 24(1), 5565.Google Scholar
Batey, M., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2009). Intelligence and personality as predictors of divergent thinking: the role of general, fluid and crystallized intelligence. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 4, 60–9.Google Scholar
Batey, M., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010a). Individual differences in ideational behavior: can the Big Five and psychometric intelligence predict creativity scores? Creativity Research Journal, 22(1), 90–7.Google Scholar
Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence and personality: a critical review of the scattered literature. Genetic, General and Social Psychology Monographs, 132, 355429.Google Scholar
Batey, M., Furnham, A., & Safiullina, X. (2010b). Intelligence, general knowledge and personality as predictors of creativity. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 532–5.Google Scholar
Bender, S. W., Nibbelink, B., Towner-Thyrum, E., & Vredenburg, D. (2013). Defining characteristics of creative women. Creativity Research Journal, 25(1), 3847.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Toward a Theory of Exploratory Behavior: II. Arousal Potential, Perceptual Curiosity, and Learning. In Berlyne, D. E. (ed.), Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity (pp. 193227), New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Brophy, D. R. (1998). Understanding, measuring, and enhancing individual creative problem-solving efforts. Creativity Research Journal, 11(2), 123–50.Google Scholar
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Reichenbacher, L. (2008). Effects of personality and threat of evaluation on divergent and convergent thinking. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1095–101.Google Scholar
Colquitt, J. A., & Simmering, M. J. (1998). Conscientiousness, goal orientation, and motivation to learn during the learning process: a longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(4), 654–65.Google Scholar
Conti, R., Coon, H., & Amabile, T. M. (1996). Evidence to support the componential model of creativity: secondary analyses of three studies. Creativity Research Journal, 9(4), 385–9.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). The Flow Experience and Its Significance for Human Psychology. In Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (eds.), Optimal Experience: Psychological Studies of Flow in Consciousness (pp. 1535). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Denissen, J. J., Zarrett, N. R., & Eccles, J. S. (2007). I like to do it, I’m able, and I know I am: longitudinal couplings between domain-specific achievement and self-concept, and interest. Child Development, 78(2), 430–47.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 880–96.Google Scholar
Dollinger, S. J., Urban, K. K., & James, T. J. (2004). Creativity and openness: further validation of two creative products measures. Creativity Research Journal, 16(1), 3548.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290309.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (2006). How development and personality influence scientific though, interest, and achievement. Review of General Psychology, 10(2), 163–82.Google Scholar
Ford, C. M. (1996). A theory of individual creative action in multiple social domains. Academy of Management Review, 21(4), 1112–42.Google Scholar
Ford, C. M., & Gioia, D. A. (2000). Factors influencing creativity in the domain of managerial decision making. Journal of Management, 26, 705–32.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., & Bachtiar, V. (2008). Personality and intelligence as predictors of creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 613–17.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., Batey, M., Anand, K., & Manfield, J. (2008). Personality, hypomania, intelligence, and creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1060–9.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., Crump, J., & Swami, V. (2008–9). Abstract reasoning and Big Five personality correlates of creativity in a British occupational sample. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 28(4), 361–70.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., Hughes, D. J., & Marshall, E. (2013). Creativity, OCD, narcissism, and the Big Five. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 10, 91–8.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1988). Creativity: an interdisciplinary perspective. Creativity Research Journal, 1(1), 826.Google Scholar
George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2001). When openness to experience and conscientiousness are related to creative behavior: an interactional approach. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 513–24.Google Scholar
Getzels, J. W., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The Creative Vision: A Longitudinal Study of Problem Finding in Art. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Gong, Y., Cheung, S. Y., Wang, M., & Huang, J. C. (2012). Unfolding the proactive process for creativity: integration of the employee proactivity, information exchange, and psychological safety. Journal of Management, 38(5), 1611–33.Google Scholar
Gottfredson, G. D. (1977). Career stability and redirection in adulthood. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62, 436–45.Google Scholar
Grosul, M., & Feist, G. J. (2014). The creative person in science. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(1), 3043.Google Scholar
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–54.Google Scholar
Guilford, J. P. (1956). The structure of intellect. Psychological Bulletin, 53(4), 267–93.Google Scholar
Hambrick, D. Z., Pink, J. E., Meinz, E. J., Pettibone, J. C., & Oswald, F. L. (2008). The roles of ability, personality, and interests in acquiring current events knowledge: a longitudinal study. Intelligence, 36(3), 261–78.Google Scholar
Hart, J. W., Stasson, M. F., Mahoney, J. M., & Story, P. (2007). The Big Five and achievement motivation: exploring the relationship between personality and a two-factor model of motivation. Individual Differences Research, 5(4), 267–74.Google Scholar
Hidi, S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Motivating the academically unmotivated: a critical issue of the 21st century. Review of Educational Research, 70(2), 151–79.Google Scholar
Hocevar, D. (1976). Dimensionality of creativity. Psychological Reports, 39, 869–70.Google Scholar
Holland, J. L. (1959). A theory of vocational choice. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 6(1), 3545.Google Scholar
Holland, J. L. (1985). Making Vocational Choices: A Theory of Vocational Personalities and Work Environments. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Holland, J. L. (ed.). (1997). Making Vocational Choices: A Theory of Vocational Personalities and Work Environments (3rd edn.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Holland, J. L., Sorenson, A. B., Clark, J. P., Nafziger, D. H., & Blum, Z. D. (1973). Applying an occupational classification to a representative sample of work histories. Journal of Applied Psychology, 58, 3441.Google Scholar
Hughes, D. J., Furnham, A., & Batey, M. (2013). The structure and personality of self-rated creativity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 9, 7684.Google Scholar
Karwowski, M., Lebuda, I., Wisniewska, E., & Gralewski, J. (2013). Big Five personality traits as predictors of creative self-efficacy and creative personal identity: does genera matter?. Journal of Creative Behavior, 47(3), 215–32.Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B. (2013). Opening up openness to experience: a four-factor model and relations to creative achievement in the arts and sciences. Journal of Creative Behavior, 47(4), 233–55.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., Baer, J., Cropley, D. H., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Sinnet, S. (2013). Furious activity vs. understanding: how much expertise is needed to evaluate creative work?. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(4), 332–40.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Beyond big and little: the four C model of creativity. Review of General Psychology, 13(1), 112.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2013). Do people recognize the four Cs? Examining layperson conceptions of creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(3), 229–36.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., Pumaccahua, T., & Holt, R. (2013). Personality and creativity in realistic, investigative, artistic, social, and enterprising college majors. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 913–17.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., Waterstreet, M. A., Ailabouni, H. S., Whitcomb, H. J., Roe, A. K., et al. (2009–10). Personality and self-perceptions of creativity across domains. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 29, 193209.Google Scholar
King, L., Walker, L., & Broyles, S. (1996). Creativity and the five factor model. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 189203.Google Scholar
Larson, L. M., Rottinghaus, P. J., & Borgen, F. H. (2002). Meta-analyses of Big Six interests and Big Five personality factors. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 61(2), 217–39.Google Scholar
Litman, J. A. (2005). Curiosity and the pleasures of learning: wanting and liking new information. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 793814. doi: 10.1080/02699930541000101Google Scholar
Madjar, N. (2008). Emotional and informational support from different sources and employee creativity. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 81, 83100.Google Scholar
Moneta, G. B., Amabile, T. M., Schatzel, E. A., & Kramer, S. J. (2010). Multirater assessment of creative contributions to team projects in organizations. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 19(2), 150–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1258–65.Google Scholar
Montag, T., Maertz, C. P., & Baer, M. (2012). A critical analysis of the workplace creativity criterion space. Journal of Management, 38(4), 1362–86.Google Scholar
Mumford, M. D. (2003). Where have we been, where are we going? Taking stock in creativity research. Creativity Research Journal, 15(2–3), 107–20.Google Scholar
Mumford, M. D., Baughman, W. A., Supinski, E. P., & Maher, M. A. (1996a). Process-based measured of creative problem-solving skills: II. Information encoding. Creativity Research Journal, 9(1), 7788.Google Scholar
Mumford, M. D. Baughman, W. A., Threlfall, K. V., Supinski, E. P., & Costanza, D. P. (1996b). Process-based measures of creative problem-solving skills: I. Problem construction. Creativity Research Journal, 9(1), 6376.Google Scholar
Mumford, M. D., Medeiros, K. E., & Partlow, P. J. (2012). Creative thinking: processes, strategies, and knowledge. Journal of Creative Behavior, 46(1), 3047.Google Scholar
Mumford, M.D., Supinski, E. P., Threlfall, K. V., & Baughman, W. A. (1996c). Process-based measures of creative problem-solving skills: III. Category selection. Creativity Research Journal, 9(4), 395406.Google Scholar
Mussel, P. (2013). Intellect: a theoretical framework for personality traits related to intellectual achievements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 885906.Google Scholar
Mussel, P., McKay, A., Ziegler, M., Hewig, J., & Kaufman, J. C. (2015). Predicting creativity based on the facets of the theoretical intellect framework. European Journal of Personality, 29(4), 459–67. doi: 10.1002/per.2000Google Scholar
Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2012). A comparison of self-ratings and non-self-report measures of employee creativity. Human Relations, 65(8), 1021–47.Google Scholar
Nusbaum, E. C., & Silvia, P. J. (2011). Are openness and intellect distinct aspects of openness to experience? A test of the O/I model. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 571–4. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.013Google Scholar
Nye, C. D., Su, R., Rounds, J., & Drasgow, F. (2012). Vocational interests and performance. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(4), 384403.Google Scholar
Oldham, G. R., & Cummings, A. (1996). Employee creativity: personal and contextual factors at work. Academy of Management Journal, 39(3), 607–34.Google Scholar
Perrine, N. E., & Brodersen, R. M. (2005). Artistic and scientific creative behavior: openness and the mediating role of interests. Journal of Creative Behavior, 39, 217–36.Google Scholar
Plucker, J. A. (1998). Beware of simple conclusions: the case for content generality of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 11(2), 179–82.Google Scholar
Plucker, J. A. (1999). Reanalyses of student responses to creativity checklists: evidence of content generality. Journal of Creative Behavior, 33(2), 126–37.Google Scholar
Puccio, G. J., & Cabra, J. F. (2010). Organizational Creativity: A Systems Approach. In Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Creativity (pp. 145–73). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Raine, A., Reynolds, C., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (2002). Stimulation seeking and intelligence: a prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(4), 663–74.Google Scholar
Raja, U., & Johns, G. (2010). The joint effects of personality and job scope on in-role performance, citizenship behaviors, and creativity. Human Relations, 63(7), 9811005.Google Scholar
Reiter-Palmon, R., Illies, M. Y., Cross, L. K., Buboltz, C., & Nimps, T. (2009). Creativity and domain specificity: the effect of task type on multiple indexes of creative problem-solving. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3(2), 7380.Google Scholar
Reiter-Palmon, R., Mumford, M. D., O’Connor Boes, J., & Runco, M. A. (1997). Problem construction and creativity: the role of ability, cue consistency, and active processing. Creativity Research Journal, 10(1), 923.Google Scholar
Rhodes, M. (1961). An analysis of creativity. Phi Delta Kappan, 42(7), 305–10.Google Scholar
Rounds, J., & Su, R. (2014). The nature and power of interests. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 98103.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2004). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 657–87.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Charles, R. E. (1993). Judgements of originality and appropriateness as predictors of creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 15(5), 537–46.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., Illies, J. J., & Eisenman, R. (2005). Creativity, originality, and appropriateness: what do explicit instructions tell us about their relationships? Journal of Creative Behavior, 39(2), 137–48.Google Scholar
Scott, G. M., Lonergan, D. C., & Mumford, M. D. (2005). Conceptual combination: alternative knowledge structures, alternative heuristics. Creativity Research Journal, 17(1), 7998.Google Scholar
Scratchley, L. S., & Hakstian, A. R. (2000–1). The measurement and prediction of managerial creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 13(3–4), 367–84.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J. (2006). Discovering the Psychology of Interest. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J. (2008). Another look at creativity and intelligence: exploring higher-order models and probable confounds. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1012–21.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Pretz, J. E. (2009). Is creativity domain-specific? Latent class models of creative accomplishments and creative self-descriptions. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 139–48.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Berg, C., Martin, C., & O’Conner, A. (2009). Openness to experience, plasticity, and creativity: exploring lower order, higher order, and interactive effects. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 1087–90.Google Scholar
Simmons, A. L. (2011). The influence of openness to experience and organizational justice on creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 23(1), 923.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2014). Creative performance, expertise acquisition, individual differences, and developmental antecedents: an integrative research agenda. Intelligence, 45, 6673.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Domain-generality versus domain-specificity: the life and impending death of a false dichotomy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 35(1), 115–30.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2005). Creativity or creativities? International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 63, 370–82.Google Scholar
Strickland, S., & Towler, A. (2011). Correlates of creative behaviour: the role of leadership and personal factors. Canadian Journal of Administrative Sciences, 28, 4151.Google Scholar
Su, R., & Nye, C. D. (in press). Interests and Person-Environment Fit: A New Perspective on Workforce Readiness and Success. In J. Burrus, K. D. Mattern, B. Naemi, & R. D. Roberts (eds.), Building Better Students: Preparation for the Workforce. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sullivan, D. M., & Ford, C. M. (2010). The alignment of measures and constructs in organizational research: the case of testing measurement models of creativity. Journal of Business Psychology, 25, 505–21.Google Scholar
Sung, S. Y., & Choi, J. N. (2009). Do Big Five personality factors affect individual creativity? The moderating role of extrinsic motivation. Social Behavior and Personality, 37(7), 941956.Google Scholar
Taft, R., & Rossiter, J. R. (1966). The remote associates test: divergent or convergent thinking? Psychological Reports, 19, 1313–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, S. A., & Silvia, P. J. (2006). Must interesting things be pleasant? A test of competing appraisal structures. Emotion, 6(4), 670–4.Google Scholar
Walker, B. R., & Jackson, C. J. (2014). How the five factor model and revised reinforcement sensitivity theory predict divergent thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 57, 54–8.Google Scholar
Woo, S. E., Chernyshenko, O. S., Longley, A., Zhang, Z., Chiu, C. Y., et al. (2014). Openness to experience: its lower-level structure, measurement, and cross-cultural equivalence. Journal of Personality Assessment, 96(1), 2945.Google Scholar
Zeng, L., Proctor, R. W., & Salvendy, G. (2011). Can traditional divergent thinking tests be trusted in measuring and predicting real-world creativity? Creativity Research Journal, 23(1), 24–7.Google Scholar
Zhou, J., & Hoever, I. J. (2014). Research on workplace creativity: a review and redirection. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 1, 333–59.Google Scholar
Ziegler, M., Danay, E., Heene, M., Asendorpf, J., & Bühner, M. (2012). Openness, fluid intelligence, and crystallized intelligence: toward an integrative model. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(2), 173–83.Google Scholar

References

Acar, S., & Sen, S. (2013). A multilevel meta-analysis of the relationship between creativity and schizotypy. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(3), 214–28. doi:10.1037/a0031975Google Scholar
Agnoli, S., Franchin, L., Rubaltelli, E., & Corazza, G. E. (2015). An eye-tracking analysis of irrelevance processing as moderator of openness and creative performance. Creativity Research Journal, 27(2), 125–32. doi:10.1080/10400419.2015.1030304Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual 5. Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Austin, E. J. (2005). Personality correlates of the broader autism phenotype as assessed by the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 451–60.Google Scholar
Babbage, D. R., & Ronan, K. R. (2000). Philosophical worldview and personality factors in traditional and social scientists: study the world in our own image. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 405–20.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Stott, C., Bolton, P., & Goodyer, I. (1997). Is there a link between engineering and autism? Autism, 1, 101–9.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. Bolton, P., Wheelwright, S., Short, L., Mead, G., et al. (1998). Autism occurs more often in families of physicists, engineers, and mathematicians, Autism, 2, 296301.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Burtenshaw, A., & Hobson, E. (2007). Mathematical talent is linked to autism. Human Nature, 18, 125–31. doi: 10.1007/s12110-007–9014-0.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Robinson, J., & Woodbury-Smith, M. (2005). The Adult Asperger Assessment (AAA): A diagnostic method. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 807–19.Google Scholar
Barron, F. X. (1955). The disposition toward originality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 478–85.Google Scholar
Barron, F. X. (1963). Creativity and Psychological Health. New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2008). The relationship between measures of creativity and schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 816–21.Google Scholar
Batey, M., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010). Individual differences in ideational behavior: can the Big Five and psychometric intelligence predict creativity scores? Creativity Research Journal, 22(1), 90–7. doi:10.1080/10400410903579627Google Scholar
Billington, J., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2007). Cognitive style predicts entry into physical sciences and humanities: questionnaire and performance tests of empathy and systemizing. Learning and Individual Differences, 17, 260–8.Google Scholar
Block, J. (1978). The Q-Sort Method in Personality Assessment and Psychiatric Research. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Busse, T. V., & Mansfield, R. S. (1984). Selected personality traits and achievement in male scientists. Journal of Psychology, 116, 117–31.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. M., & Yung, C. K. (2006). Sex and discipline differences in empathizing, systemizing, and autistic symptomatology: evidence from a student population. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 949–57.Google Scholar
Carson, S. H. (2011). Creativity and psychopathology: a shared vulnerability model. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 144–53.Google Scholar
Carson, S. H. (2014). Leveraging the “mad genius” debate: why we need a neuroscience of creativity and psychopathology. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 14.Google Scholar
Casanova, M. F., Switala, A. E., Trippe, J., & Fitzgerald, M. (2007). Comparative minicolumnar morphometry of three distinguished scientists. Autism, 11, 557–69.Google Scholar
Chambers, J. A. (1964). Relating personality and biographical factors to scientific creativity. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 78, 120.Google Scholar
Chang, C., Wang, J., Liang, C.-T., & Liang, C. (2014). Curvilinear effects of openness and agreeableness on the imaginative capability of student designers. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 14, 6875. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2014.09.001Google Scholar
Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2006). Creativity versus conscientiousness: which is a better predictor of student performance? Journal of Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 521–31.Google Scholar
Chiang, Y., Hsu, C., & Shih, H. (2015). Experienced high performance work system, extroversion personality, and creativity performance. Asia Pacific Journal of Management, 32(2), 531–49. doi:10.1007/s10490-014–9403-yGoogle Scholar
Claridge, G., Pryor, R., & Watkins, G. (1990). Sounds from the Bell Jar: Ten Psychotic Authors. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Conway, J. B. (1988). Differences among clinical psychologists: scientists, practitioners, and science-practitioners. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 19, 642–55.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Grazioplene, R. G., & Peterson, J. B. (2012). From madness to genius: the openness/intellect trait domain as a paradoxical simplex. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(1), 6378.Google Scholar
Dollinger, S. J., Urban, K. K., & James, T. A. (2004). Creativity and openness: further validation of two creative product measures. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 3547.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1993). Creativity and personality: suggestions for a theory. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 147–78.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The Natural History of Creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eysenck, S. G., White, O., & Eysenck, H. J. (1976). Personality and mental illness. Psychological Reports, 39, 1011–22.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1993). A structural model of scientific eminence. Psychological Science, 4, 366–71.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of the impact of personality on scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychological Review, 2, 290309.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (2006a). How development and personality influence scientific thought, interest, and achievement. Review of General Psychology, 10, 163–82.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (2006b). The Psychology of Science and the Origins of the Scientific Mind. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (2010). The Function of Personality in Creativity: The Nature and Nurture of the Creative Personality. In Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J. (eds.). Cambridge Handbook of Creativity (pp. 113–30). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (2012). Predicting interest in and attitudes toward science from personality and need for cognition. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 771–5. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.005Google Scholar
Feist, G. J., & Rosenberg, E. L. (2015). Psychology: Perspectives & Connections (3rd edn.). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Feist, J., Feist, G. J., & Roberts, T. A. (2013). Theories of Personality (8th edn.). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Francis, B., Skelton, C., & Read, B. (2012). The Identities and Practices of High Achieving Pupils: Negotiating Achievement and Peer Cultures. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Frayling, C. (2005). Mad, Bad and Dangerous? The Scientist and the cinema. London: Reaktion Books.Google Scholar
Focquaert, F., Steven, M. S., Wolford, G. L., Colden, A., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2007). Empathizing and systemizing cognitive traits in the sciences and humanities. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 619–25.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1999). Intelligence Reframed: Multiple Intelligences for the 21st Century. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gelade, G. A. (2002). Creative style, personality and artistic endeavour. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 128, 213–34.Google Scholar
Gough, H. G., & Woodworth, D. G. (1960). Stylistic variations among professional research scientists. Journal of Psychology, 49, 8798.Google Scholar
Grosul, M. & Feist, G. J. (2014). The creative person in science. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 8, 3043. doi: 10.1037/a0034828Google Scholar
Gustafsson, L. (1997). Inadequate cortical feature maps: a neural circuit theory of autism. Biological Psychiatry, 42, 1138–47.Google Scholar
Haller, C. S., & Courvoiser, D. S. (2010). Personality and thinking style in different creative domains. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 4, 149–60. doi: 10.1037/a00117084Google Scholar
Harris, J. A. (2004). Measured intelligence, achievement, openness to experience, and creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 913–29.Google Scholar
Hart, J. J. (1982). Psychology of the scientists: XLVI. Correlation between theoretical orientation in psychology and personality type. Psychological Reports, 50, 795801.Google Scholar
Haynes, R. D. (1994). From Faust to Strangelove: Representations of the Scientist in Western Literature. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Helson, R., & Crutchfield, R. S. (1970). Mathematicians: the creative researcher and the average PhD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 250–7.Google Scholar
Heller, K. A. (2007). Scientific ability and creativity. High Ability Studies, 18, 209–34.Google Scholar
Helmreich, R. L., Spence, J. T., & Pred, R. S. (1988). Making it without losing it: type A, achievement motivation and scientific attainment revisited. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 495504.Google Scholar
Iuculano, T., Rosenberg-Lee, M., Supekar, K., Lynch, C. J., Khousam, A., et al. (2014). Brain organization underlying superior mathematical abilities in children with autism. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 223–30. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.06.018.Google Scholar
Ivcevic, Z., & Brackett, M. A. (2015). Predicting creativity: interactive effects of openness to experience and emotion regulation ability. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9(4), 480–7. doi:10.1037/a0039826Google Scholar
James, I. (2003). Singular scientists. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 96, 36–9.Google Scholar
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five Taxonomy: History, Measurement, and Theoretical Perspectives. In Pervin, L. A. & John, O. P. (eds.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (pp. 102–38). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, J. A., Germer, C. K., Efran, J. S., & Overton, W. F. (1988). Personality as the basis for theoretical predilections. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 824–35.Google Scholar
Jones, R. A. (1997). The boffin: a stereotype of scientists in post-war British films (1945–1970). Public Understanding of Science, 6, 3148.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Karwowski, M., & Lebuda, I. (2016). The Big Five, the Huge Two, and creative self-beliefs: a meta-analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10(2), 214–32. doi:10.1037/aca0000035.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Beyond big and little: the Four C model of creativity. Review of General Psychology, 13, 112.Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B. (2013). Opening up openness to experience: a four-factor model and relations to creative achievement in the arts and sciences. Journal of Creative Behavior, 47, 233–55. doi: 10.1002/jocb.33Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., Kozbelt, A., Silvia, P., Kaufman, J. C., Ramesh, S., et al. (2014). Who finds Bill Gates sexy? Creative mate preferences as a function of cognitive ability, personality, and creative achievement. Journal of Creative Behavior, 48, 119. doi: 10.1002/jocb.78Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., Quilty, L. C., Grazioplene, R. G., Hirsh, J. B., Gray, J. R., et al. (2015). Openness to experience and intellect differentially predict creative achievement in the arts and sciences. Journal of Personality, 84(2), 248–58. doi:10.1111/jopy.12156Google Scholar
King, L. A., McKee, W. L., & Broyles, S. J. (1996). Creativity and the five-factor model. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 189203.Google Scholar
Ko, Y.-G., & Kim, J.-Y. (2008). Scientific geniuses’ psychopathology as a moderator in the relation between creative contribution types and eminence. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 251–61. doi: 10.1080/104004108022785677Google Scholar
Kokosh, J. (1969). MMPI personality characteristics of physical and social science students. Psychological Reports, 24, 883–93.Google Scholar
Kusumakar, V., Bond, J., & Yatham, L. N. (2002). Diagnosis and Treatment of Hypomania and Mania. In Yatham, L. N. & Kusumakar, V. (eds.), Bipolar Disorder: A Clinician’s Guide to Biological Treatments (2nd edn., pp. 118). New York: Brunner-Routledge.Google Scholar
Lippa, R. (1998). Gender-related individual differences and the structure of vocational interests: the importance of the people-things dimension. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(4), 9961009.Google Scholar
Lloyd-Evans, R., Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2006). Bipolar Disorder and Creativity: Investigating a Possible Link. In Columbus, A. & Columbus, A. (eds.), Advances in Psychology Research (Vol. 40, pp. 111–41). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Lounsbury, J. W., Foster, N., Patel, H., Carmody, P., Gibson, L. W., et al. (2012). An investigation of the personality traits of scientists versus nonscientists and their relationship with career satisfaction. R&D Management, 42, 4759.Google Scholar
Lubow, R. E. (1989). Latent Inhibition and Conditioned Attention Theory. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ludwig, A. M. (1995). The Price of Greatness: Resolving the Creativity and Madness Controversy. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Ludwig, A. M. (1998). Method and madness in the arts and sciences. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 93101.Google Scholar
MacKinnon, D. W. (1978). In Search of Human Effectiveness. Buffalo, NY: Bearly.Google Scholar
Mason, O., Claridge, G., & Jackson, M. (1995). New scales for the assessment of schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 713.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1258–65. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1258Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1999). A Five-Factor Theory of Personality. In Pervin, L. A. & John, O. P. (eds.), Personality Theory and Research (pp. 139–53). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mount, M. K., Barrick, M. R., Scullen, S. M., & Rounds, J. (2005). Higher-order dimensions of the Big Five personality traits and the Big Six vocational interest types. Personnel Psychology, 58(2), 447–78.Google Scholar
Muir, H. (2003). Einstein and Newton showed signs of autism. New Scientist, April 30.Google Scholar
Nasar, S. (1998). A Beautiful Mind. New York: Touchstone.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2006). Schizotypy and mental health amongst poets, visual artists, and mathematicians. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 876–90.Google Scholar
O’Connell, H., & Fitzgerald, M. (2003). Did Alan Turing have Asperger’s syndrome? Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine, 20, 2831. doi: 10.1017/S0790966700007503Google Scholar
Pavitra, K. S., Chandrashekar, C. R., & Choudhury, P. (2007). Creativity and mental health: a profile of writers and musicians. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 49, 3443.Google Scholar
Pickover, C. A. (1998). Strange Brains and Genius: The Secret Lives of Eccentric Scientists and Madmen. New York: Plenum Trade.Google Scholar
Post, F. (1994). Creativity and psychopathology: a study of 291 world famous men. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 2234.Google Scholar
Prabhu, V., Sutton, C., & Sauser, W. (2008). Creativity and certain personality traits: understanding the mediating effect of intrinsic motivation. Creativity Research Journal, 20(1), 5366. doi:10.1080/10400410701841955Google Scholar
Rado, S. (1953). Dynamics and classification of disordered behavior. American Journal of Psychiatry, 110, 4060–416.Google Scholar
Rawlings, D., & Locarnini, A. (2008). Dimensional schizotypy, autism, and unusual word associations in artists and scientists. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(2), 465–71.Google Scholar
Royalty, G. M., & Magoon, T. M. (1985). Correlates of scholarly productivity among counseling psychologists. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 32, 458–61.Google Scholar
Rushton, J. P., Murray, H. G., & Paunonen, S. V. (1987). Personality Characteristics Associated with High Research Productivity. In Jackson, D. & Rushton, J. P. (eds.), Scientific Excellence, (pp. 129–48). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Sacks, O. (2001). Henry Cavendish: an early case of Asperger’s syndrome? Neurology, 57, 1347Google Scholar
Selby, E. C., Shaw, E. J., & Houtz, J. C. (2005). The creative personality. Gifted Child Quarterly, 49, 300–14. doi: 10.1177/001698620504900404Google Scholar
Shurkin, J. (2006). Broken Genius: The Rise and Fall of William Shockley, Creator of the Electronic Age. New York: Macmillian.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Kaufman, J. C., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Wigert, B. (2011). Cantankerous creativity: honesty, humility, agreeableness, and the HEXACO structure of creative achievement. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(5), 687–9. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.011Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1999). Origins of Genius: Darwinian Perspectives on Creativity. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2008). Scientific talent, training, and performance: intellect, personality, and genetic endowment. Review of General Psychology, 12, 2846.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2009). Varieties of (scientific) creativity: a hierarchical model of domain-specific disposition, development, and achievement. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 441–52.Google Scholar
Suls, J., & Fletcher, B. (1983). Social comparison in the social and physical sciences: an archival study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(3), 575–80. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.44.3.575Google Scholar
Tang, C., & Kaufman, J. C. (2015). Personal characteristics that distinguish creative scientists from less creative scientists. Journal of Creative Behavior (in press). doi:10.1002/jocb.99Google Scholar
Thomson, N. D., Wurtzburg, S. J., & Centifanti, L. C. M. (2015). Empathy or science? Empathy explains physical science enrollment for men and women. Learning and Individual Differences, 40, 115120.Google Scholar
Treffert, D. A. (2006). Extraordinary People: Understanding Savant Syndrome (Updated Version). Lincoln, NE: iUniverse.Google Scholar
van Tilburg, W. P., Sedikides, C., & Wildschut, T. (2015). The mnemonic muse: nostalgia fosters creativity through openness to experience. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 59, 17. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2015.02.002Google Scholar
Van Zelst, R. H., & Kerr, W. A. (1954). Personality self-assessment of scientific and technical personnel. Journal of Applied Psychology, 38, 145–7.Google Scholar
Wertheim, M. (2011). Physics on the Fringe: Smoke Rings, Circlons, and Alternative Theories of Everything. New York: Walker.Google Scholar
Wilson, G. D., & Jackson, C. (1994). The personality of physicists. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 187–9.Google Scholar
Wood, R. (2011). “The world in our own image” explored: philosophical worldview, gender-role and masculine gender role stress in nontraditional and traditional physical and social scientists. Dissertation Abstracts International, 71, 5108.Google Scholar
Wuthrich, V., & Bates, T. C. (2001). Schizotypy and latent inhibition: non-linear linkage between psychometric and cognitive markers. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(5), 783–98.Google Scholar
Zachar, P., & Leong, F. T. L. (1992). A problem of personality: scientist and practitioner differences in psychology. Journal of Personality, 60, 667–77.Google Scholar

References

Albert, R. S. (1990). Identity, Experiences, and Career Choice among the Exceptionally Gifted and Eminent. In Runco, M. A. & Albert, R. S. (eds.), Theories of Creativity (pp. 1334). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Albert, R. S., & Runco, M. A. (1986). The Achievement of Eminence: A Model Based on a Longitudinal Study of Exceptionally Gifted Boys and Their Families. In Sternberg, R. J. & Davidson, J. E. (eds.), Conceptions of Giftedness (pp. 332–57). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Asendorpf, J. B., & Van Aken, M. A. G. (2003). Personality–relationship transaction in adolescence: core versus surface personality characteristics. Journal of Personality, 4, 629–66.Google Scholar
Bachtold, L. M., & Werner, E. E. (1972). Personality characteristics of women scientists. Psychological Reports, 31, 391–6.Google Scholar
Baer, J. (2015). Domain Specificity in Creativity. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York: W.H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Barron, F. (1983). Creative Writers. In. Albert, R. S. (ed.), Genius and Eminence: The Social Psychology of Creativity and Exceptional Achievement (pp. 302–10). Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Barron, F., & Harrington, D. M. (1981). Creativity, intelligence and personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 32, 439–76.Google Scholar
Batey, M., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010). Individual differences in ideational behavior: can the Big Five and psychometric intelligence predict creativity scores? Creativity Research Journal, 22, 90–7.Google Scholar
Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2008). The relationship between measures of creativity and schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 816–21.Google Scholar
Batey, M., Furnham, A. F., & Safiullina, X. (2010). Intelligence, general knowledge and personality as predictors of creativity. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 532–5.Google Scholar
Beghetto, R. A. (2006). Creative self-efficacy: correlates in middle and secondary students. Creativity Research Journal, 18, 447–57.Google Scholar
Beghetto, R. A., & Karwowski, M. (in press). Toward untangling creative self-beliefs. In M. Karwowski & J. C. Kaufman (eds.), The Creative Self: How Our Beliefs, Self-Efficacy, Mindset, and Identity Impact Our Creativity. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Beghetto, R. A., Kaufman, J. C., & Baxter, J. (2011). Answering the unexpected questions: student self-beliefs and teacher ratings of creativity in elementary math and science. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 5, 342–9.Google Scholar
Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 480–5.Google Scholar
Carmeli, A., & Schaubroeck, J. (2007). The influence of leaders’ and other referents’ normative expectations on individual involvement in creative work. Leadership Quarterly, 18, 3548.Google Scholar
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Reliability, validity, and factor structure of the Creative Achievement Questionnaire. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 3750.Google Scholar
Choi, J. N. (2004). Individual and contextual predictors of creative performance: the mediating role of psychological processes. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 187–99.Google Scholar
Coopersmith, S. A. (1967). The Antecedents of Self-Esteem. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Cox, C. (1926). The Early Mental Traits of Three Hundred Geniuses. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the Psychology of Discovery and Invention. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Davis, C. D., Kaufman, J. C., & McClure, F. H. (2011). Non-cognitive constructs and self-reported creativity by domain. Journal of Creative Behavior, 45, 188202.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2002). Higher-order factors of the Big Five predict conformity: are there neuroses of health? Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 533–52.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290309.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (2014). Psychometric Studies of Scientific Talent and Eminence. In Simonton, D. K. (ed.), The Wiley Handbook of Genius (pp. 6286). Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Freeman, M. (1993). Finding File Muse: A Socio-Psychological Inquiry into the Conditions of Artistic Creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., & Bachtiar, V. (2008). Personality and intelligence as predictors of creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 613–17.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., Batey, M., Anand, K., & Manfield, J. (2008). Personality, hypomania, intelligence and creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1060–9.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., Batey, M., Booth, T. W., Patel, V., & Lozinskaya, D. (2011). Individual difference predictors of creativity in art and science students. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 2, 114–21.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., Crump, J., Batey, M., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2009). Personality and ability predictors of the consequences test of divergent thinking in a large non-student sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 536–40.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., Hughes, D. J., & Marshall, E. (2013). Creativity, OCD, narcissism and the Big Five. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 10, 91–8.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., Zhang, J., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2006). The relationship between psychometric and self-estimated intelligence, creativity, personality and academic achievement. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 25, 119–45.Google Scholar
Glaveanu, V. P., & Tanggaard, L. (2013). Creativity, identity, and representation: towards a socio-cultural theory of creative identity. New Ideas in Psychology, 34, 1221.Google Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The development of markers for the Big Five factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4, 2642.Google Scholar
Haimovitz, K., Wormington, S. V., & Corpus, J. H. (2011). Dangerous mindsets: how beliefs about intelligence predict motivational change. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 747–52.Google Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2008). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Helson, R. (1990). Creativity in Women: Outer and Inner Views over Time. In Runco, M. A. & Albert, R. S. (eds.), Theories of Creativity (pp. 4658). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Helson, R., & Pals, J. L. (2000). Creative potential, creative achievement, and personal growth. Journal of Personality, 1, 127.Google Scholar
Hong, E., Peng, Y., & O’Neil, H. F. Jr. (2014). Activities and accomplishments in various domains: relationships with creative personality and creative motivation in adolescence. Roeper Review, 36, 92103.Google Scholar
Hsu, L. A., Hou, S. T., & Fan, H. L. (2011). Creative self-efficacy and innovative behavior in a service setting: optimism as a moderator. Journal of Creative Behavior, 4, 258–72.Google Scholar
Hughes, D. J., Furnham, A., & Batey, M. (2013). The structure of personality predictors of self-rated creativity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 9, 7684.Google Scholar
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Ivcevic, Z., & Nusbaum, E. (in press). From having an idea to doing something with it: self-regulation for creativity. In M. Karwowski & J. C. Kaufman (eds.), The Creative Self: How Our Beliefs, Self-Efficacy, Mindset, and Identity Impact Our Creativity. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890/1963). The Principles of Psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Dunst, B., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). The relationship between intelligence and creativity: new support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence, 41, 212–21.Google Scholar
Jaussi, K. S., Randel, A. E., & Dionne, S. D. (2007). I am, I think I can, and I do: the role of personal identity, self-efficacy, and cross-application of experiences in creativity at work. Creativity Research Journal, 2 –3, 247–58.Google Scholar
Karwowski, M. (2011a). It doesn’t hurt to ask … but sometimes it hurts to believe: Polish students’ creative self-efficacy and its predictors. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 5, 154–64.Google Scholar
Karwowski, M. (2011ba). The creative mix: teacher’s creative leadership, school creative climate, and students’ creative self-efficacy. Chowanna, 2, 2543.Google Scholar
Karwowski, M. (2012). Did curiosity kill the cat? Relationship between trait curiosity, creative self-efficacy and creative personal identity. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 8, 112.Google Scholar
Karwowski, M. (2014). Creative mindset: measurement, correlates, consequences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 6270.Google Scholar
Karwowski, M., & Barbot, B. (2016). Creative self-beliefs: their nature, development and correlates. In Kaufman, J. C. & Baer, J. (eds.), The Cambridge Companion to Creativity and Reason in Cognitive Development (pp. 302326). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Karwowski, M., & Kaufman, J. C. (eds.). (in press). The Creative Self: How Our Beliefs, Self-Efficacy, Mindset, and Identity Impact Our Creativity. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Karwowski, M., & Lebuda, I. (2016). Big Five, huge two and creative self-beliefs: a meta-analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10, 214232.Google Scholar
Karwowski, M., Lebuda, I., & Wiśniewska, E. (in press). Measurement of creative self-efficacy and creative role-identity. International Journal of Creativity and Problem Solving.Google Scholar
Karwowski, M., Lebuda, I., Wisniewska, E., & Gralewski, J. (2013). Big Five personality factors as the predictors of creative self-efficacy and creative personal identity: does gender matter? Journal of Creative Behavior, 47, 215–32.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. (2006). Self-reported differences in creativity by ethnicity and gender. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 1, 1065–82.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. (2012). Counting the muses: development of the Kaufman-Domains of Creativity Scale (K-DOCS). Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 298308.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Beyond big and little: the four C model of creativity. Review of General Psychology, 13, 112.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2013). In praise of Clark Kent: creative metacognition and the importance of teaching kids when (not) to be creative. Roeper Review, 35, 155–65.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., Beghetto, R. A., & Watson, C. (2015). Creative metacognition and self-ratings of creative performance: a 4-C perspective. Learning and Individual Differences (advanced online publication). doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2015.05.00Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., Waterstreet, M.A., Ailabouni, H. S., Whitcomb, H. J., Roe, A. K., et al. (2009–10). Personality and self-perceptions of creativity across domains. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 29, 193209.Google Scholar
Kozbelt, A. (2007). A quantitative analysis of Beethoven as self-critic: implications for psychological theories of musical creativity. Psychology of Music, 35, 147–72.Google Scholar
Kozbelt, A. (2008). Longitudinal hit ratios of classical composers: reconciling “Darwinian” and expertise acquisition perspectives on lifespan creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 221–35.Google Scholar
Lebuda, I. (2013). Kształtowanie tożsamości twórcy – opracowanie na podstawie problemowych analiz autonarracji Jerzego Oskara Stuhra. Warsaw: Niepublikowana praca magisterska, University of Social Sciences and Humanities.Google Scholar
Lee, S. W. (2011). Exploring seven- to eight-year-olds’ use of self-talk strategies. Early Child Development and Care, 181, 847–56.Google Scholar
Li, C. H., & Wu, J. J. (2011). The structural relationships between optimism and innovative behavior: understanding potential antecedents and mediating effects. Creativity Research Journal, 2, 119–28.Google Scholar
MacKinnon, D. W. (1983). Creative Architects. In. Albert, R. S. (ed.), Genius and Eminence: The Social Psychology of Creativity and Exceptional Achievement (pp. 291301). Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
McKay, A., Karwowski, M., & Kaufman, J. C. (2016). Measuring the muses: validating the Kaufman Domains of Creativity Scale. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. doi: 10.1037/aca0000074Google Scholar
Mockros, C. A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). The Social Construction of Creative Lives. In Montuori, A. & Purser, R. E. (eds.), Social Creativity (Vol. 1, pp. 175219). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.Google Scholar
Marsh, H. W., & Hattie, J. (1996). Theoretical Perspectives on the Structure of Self-Concept. In Bracken, B. A. (ed.), Handbook of Self-Concept (pp. 3890). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., Ludtke, O., Koller, O., & Baumert, J. (2006). Integration of multidimensional self-concept and core personality constructs: construct validation and relations to well-being and achievement. Journal of Personality, 74, 403–56.Google Scholar
Martindale, C. (1989). Personality, Situation, and Creativity. In Glover, J. A., Ronning, R. R., & Reynolds, C. (eds.), Handbook of Creativity (pp. 211–33). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Maslow, A. H. (1958). Emotional blocks to creativity. Journal of Individual Psychology, 14, 51–6.Google Scholar
Mathisen, G. E., & Bronnick, K. S. (2009). Creative self-efficacy: an intervention study. International Journal of Educational Research, 1, 21–9.Google Scholar
McRobbie, A. (1998). British Fashion Design: Rag Trade or Image Industry? London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ochse, R. (1990). Before the Gates of Excellence: The Determinants of Creative Genius. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Paulhus, D. L., Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Tracy, J. L. (2004). Two replicable suppressor situations in personality research. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 303–28.Google Scholar
Pretz, J. E., & McCollum, V. A. (2014). Self-perceptions of creativity do not always reflect actual creative performance. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, 227–36.Google Scholar
Rammstedt, B. (2007). The 10-Item Big Five Inventory (BFI-10): norm values and investigation of socio-demographic effects based on a German population representative sample. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23, 193201.Google Scholar
Reiter-Palmon, R., Robinson-Morral, E., Kaufman, J. C., & Santo, J. (2012). Evaluation of self-perceptions of creativity: is it a useful criterion. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 107–14.Google Scholar
Roe, A. (1953). The Making of a Scientist. New York: Dodd, Mead & Co.Google Scholar
Rogers, C. R. (1956). What It Means to Become a Person. In Moustakas, C. E. (ed.), The Self: Explorations in Personal Growth (pp. 195211). New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Dow, G. T. (2004). Assessing the accuracy of judgments of originality on three divergent thinking tests. Korean Journal of Thinking & Problem Solving, 14, 514.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J. (2008). Discernment and creativity: how well can people identify their most creative ideas? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, 139–46.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Berg, C., Martin, C., & O’Connor, A. ( 2009). Openness to experience, plasticity, and creativity: exploring lower-order, higher-order, and interactive effects. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 1087–90.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Wigert, B., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Kaufman, J. C. (2012). Assessing creativity with self-report scales: a review and empirical evaluation. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 1, 1934.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2002). Creativity as a decision. American Psychologist, 5, 376.Google Scholar
Szen-Ziemiańska, J., & Karwowski, M. (2015). Creativity as a decision? Creative personal identity moderates the relationship between creative abilities and achievement in science. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Thorndike, R. L. (1949). Personnel Selection: Test and Measurement Techniques. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2002). Creative self-efficacy: its potential antecedents and relationship to creative performance. Academy of Management Journal, 45, 1137–48.Google Scholar
Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2011). Creative self-efficacy development and creative performance over time. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 277–93.Google Scholar
van der Linden, D., de Nijenhuis, J., & Bakker, A. B. (2010). The general factor of personality: a meta-analysis of Big Five intercorrelations and a criterion-related validity study. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 315–27.Google Scholar
Werner, C. H., Tang, M., Kruse, J., Kaufman, J. C., & Spörrle, M. (2014). The Chinese version of the Revised Creativity Domain Questionnaire (CDQ-R): first evidence for its factorial validity and systematic association with the Big Five. Journal of Creative Behavior, 48, 254–75.Google Scholar
Whitbourne, S. K. (1996). Psychosocial Perspectives on Emotions: The Role of Identity in the Aging Process. In Magai, C. & McFadden, S. H. (eds.), Handbook of Emotion, Adult Development, and Aging (pp. 8398). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. B. (2014). Meta-Analysis Macros for SAS, SPSS, and Stata, available at http://mason.gmu.edu/~dwilsonb/ma.html (accessed October, 18, 2014).Google Scholar
Wolpert, R., & Richards, A. (1997). Passionate Minds: The Inner World of Scientists. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

References

Albert, R. S. (1971). Cognitive development and parental loss among the gifted, the exceptionally gifted and the creative. Psychological Reports, 29, 1926.Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in Context. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Bacon, S. F. (2005). Positive psychology’s two cultures. Review of General Psychology, 9, 181–92.Google Scholar
Barron, F. X. (1963). Creativity and Psychological Health: Origins of Personal Vitality and Creative Freedom. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Bell, S. T., Villado, A. J., Lukasik, M. A., Belau, L., & Briggs, A. L. (2010). Getting specific about demographic diversity variable and team performance relationships: a meta-analysis. Journal of Management, 37, 709–43.Google Scholar
Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(4), 480–5.Google Scholar
Berenbaum, H., & Fujita, F. (1994). Schizophrenia and personality: exploring the boundaries and connections between vulnerability and outcome. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(1), 148.Google Scholar
Berry, C. (1981). The Nobel scientists and the origin of scientific achievement. British Journal of Sociology, 32, 381–91.Google Scholar
Berry, C. (1999). Religious traditions as contexts of historical creativity: patterns of scientific and artistic achievement and their stability. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 1125–35.Google Scholar
Bleidorn, W., Klimstra, T. A., Denissen, J. J. A., Rentfrow, P. J., Potter, J., et al. (2013). Personality maturation around the world: a cross-cultural examination. Psychological Science, 24, 2530–40.Google Scholar
Carringer, D. (1974). Creative thinking abilities in Mexican youth. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 5, 492504.Google Scholar
Carson, S., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 499506.Google Scholar
Carson, S. H. (2011). Creativity and psychopathology: a shared vulnerability model. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 144–53.Google Scholar
Carson, S. (2014). Cognitive Disinhibition, Creativity, and Psychopathology. In Simonton, D. K. (ed.), The Wiley Handbook of Genius (pp. 198221). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cassandro, V. J., & Simonton, D. K. (2002). Creativity and Genius. In Keyes, C. L. M. & Haidt, J. (eds.), Flourishing: Positive Psychology and the Life Well-Lived (pp. 163–83). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Castelli, D. M., Hillman, C. H., Buck, S. M., & Erwin, H. E. (2007). Physical fitness and academic achievement in third-and fifth-grade students. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 29(2), 239.Google Scholar
Cawley, J., Heckman, J., & Vytlacil, E. (2001). Three observations on wages and measured cognitive ability. Labour Economics, 8(4), 419–42.Google Scholar
Damian, R. I., & Roberts, B. W. (2014). Integrating post-traumatic growth into a broader model of life experiences and personality change: commentary on Jayawickreme and Blackie. European Journal of Personality, 28, 334–6.Google Scholar
Damian, R. I., & Simonton, D. K. (2014). Diversifying Experiences in the Development of Genius and Their Impact on Creative Cognition. In Simonton, D. K. (ed.), The Wiley Handbook of Genius (pp. 375–93). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Damian, R. I., & Simonton, D. K. (2015). Psychopathology, adversity, and creativity: diversifying experiences in the development of eminent African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 623–36.Google Scholar
Damian, R. I., Su, R., Shanahan, M., Trautwein, U., & Roberts, B. (2015). Can personality traits and intelligence compensate for background disadvantage? Predicting status attainment in adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 473–89.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G. (2014). Openness/Intellect: A Dimension of Personality Reflecting Cognitive Exploration. In Cooper, M. L. and Larsen, R. J. (eds.), APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology: Personality Processes and Individual Differences (Vol. 4, pp. 369–99). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., Weir, D., Tsukayama, E., & Kwok, D. (2012). Who does well in life? Conscientious adults excel in both objective and subjective success. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 356.Google Scholar
Eiduson, B. (1962). Scientists: Their Psychological World. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Eisenstadt, J. M. (1978). Parental loss and genius. American Psychologist, 33, 211–23.Google Scholar
Ellis, H. (1926). A Study of British Genius (rev. edn.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The Natural History of Creativity. CambridgeUniversity Press.Google Scholar
Fee, A., & Gray, S. J. (2012). The expatriate-creativity hypothesis: a longitudinal field test. Human Relations, 65(12), 1515–38.Google Scholar
Feist, G. (1993). A structural model of scientific eminence. Psychological Science, 4, 366–71.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(4), 290309.Google Scholar
Forgeard, M. J. (2013). Perceiving benefits after adversity: the relationship between self-reported posttraumatic growth and creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(3), 245.Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1874). English Men of Science: Their Nature and Nurture. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Gladwell, M. (2013). David and Goliath: Underdogs, Misfits, and the Art of Battling Giants. New York: Hachette.Google Scholar
Gocłowska, M. A., Crisp, R. J., & Labuschagne, K. (2013). Can counter-stereotypes boost flexible thinking? Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 16(2), 217–31.Google Scholar
Goertzel, V., & Goertzel, M. G. (1962). Cradles of Eminence. Boston: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Goertzel, M. G., Goertzel, V., & Goertzel, T. G. (1978). 300 Eminent Personalities: A Psychosocial Analysis of the Famous. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Gottfredson, L. S. (2002). Where and why g matters: not a mystery. Human Performance, 15, 2546.Google Scholar
Hamers, J. F., & Blanc, M. (2000). Bilinguality and Bilingualism (2nd edn.). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Helson, R., & Crutchfield, R. (1970). Mathematicians: the creative researcher and the average PhD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 250–7.Google Scholar
Hoever, I. J., van Knippenberg, D., vanGinkel, W. P., &Barkema, H. G. (2012). Fostering team creativity: perspective taking as key to unlocking diversity’s potential. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 982–96.Google Scholar
Homan, A. C., van Knippenberg, D., Van Kleef, G. A., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2007). Bridging faultlines by valuing diversity: the effects of diversity beliefs on information elaboration and performance in diverse work groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1189–99.Google Scholar
Hudson, L., & Jacot, B. (1986). The Outsider in Science. In Bagley, C. & Verma, G. K. (eds.), Personality, Cognition and Values (pp. 323). London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Illingworth, R. S., & Illingworth, C. M. (1969). Lessons from Childhood. Edinburgh: Livingston.Google Scholar
Karlsson, J. L. (1970). Genetic association of giftedness and creativity with schizophrenia. Hereditas, 66, 177–82.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. (ed.) (2014). Creativity and Mental Illness. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Keri, S. (2009).Genes for psychosis and creativity: a promoter polymorphism of the Neuregulin 1 gene is related to creativity in people with high intellectual achievement. Psychological Science, 20, 1070–3.Google Scholar
Keri, S. (2011). Solitary minds and social capital: latent inhibition, general intellectual functions and social network size predict creative achievements. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 5, 215–21.Google Scholar
Kinney, D. K., Richards, R., Lowing, P. A., LeBlanc, D., Zimbalist, M. E., et al. (2001). Creativity in offspring of schizophrenic and control parents: an adoption study. Creativity Research Journal, 13(1), 1725.Google Scholar
Kuncel, N. R., Hezlett, S. A., & Ones, D. S. (2004). Academic performance, career potential, creativity, and job performance: can one construct predict them all? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 148–61.Google Scholar
Lee, C. S., Therriault, D. J., & Linderholm, T. (2012). On the cognitive benefits of cultural experience: exploring the relationship between studying abroad and creative thinking. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(5), 768–78.Google Scholar
Leung, A. K., Maddux, W. W., Galinsky, A. D., & Chiu, C. (2008). Multicultural experience enhances creativity: the when and how. American Psychologist, 63, 169–81.Google Scholar
Lopez, E., Esquivel, G., & Houtz, J. (1993). The creative skills of culturally and linguistically diverse gifted students. Creativity Research Journal, 6, 401–12.Google Scholar
Ludwig, A. M. (1992). Creative achievement and psychopathology: comparison among professions. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 46, 330–56.Google Scholar
Lüdtke, O., Roberts, B. W., Trautwein, U., & Nagy, G. (2011). A random walk down university avenue: life paths, life events, and personality trait change at the transition to university life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 620–37.Google Scholar
Maddux, W. W., Adam, H., & Galinsky, A. D. (2010). When in Rome … learn why the Romans do what they do: how multicultural learning experiences facilitate creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 731–74.Google Scholar
Maslow, A. H. (1970). Motivation and Personality (2nd edn.). New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (1993). The Stories We Live By: Personal Myths and the Making of the Self. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology, 5, 100–22.Google Scholar
McCurdy, H. G. (1960). The childhood pattern of genius. Horizon, 2, 33–8.Google Scholar
Miron-Spektor, E., Gino, F., & Argote, L. (2011). Paradoxical frames and creative sparks: enhancing individual creativity through conflict and integration. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 116(2), 229–40.Google Scholar
Mumford, M. D., Connelly, M. S., Scott, G., Espejo, J., Sohl, L. M., et al. (2005). Career experiences and scientific performance: a study of social, physical, life, and health sciences. Creativity Research Journal, 17(2–3), 105–29.Google Scholar
Nemeth, C. J., & Nemeth-Brown, B. (2003). Better than Individuals? The Potential Benefits of Dissent and Diversity. In Paulus, P. B. & Nijstad, B. A. (eds.), Group Creativity: Innovation through Collaboration (pp. 6384). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2006). Schizotypy and mental health amongst poets, visual artists, and mathematicians. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 876–90.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–78.Google Scholar
Page, S. E. (2007). Difference: How the Power of Diversity Creates Better Groups, Firms, Schools, and Societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Peri, G. (2012). The effect of immigration on productivity: evidence from U.S. states. Review of Economics and Statistics, 94(1), 348–58.Google Scholar
Pransky, G. S., Berndt, E., Finkelstein, S. N., Verma, S., & Agrawal, A. (2005). Performance decrements resulting from illness in the workplace: the effect of headaches. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 47(1), 3440.Google Scholar
Raskin, E. A. (1936). Comparison of scientific and literary ability: a biographical study of eminent scientists and men of letters of the nineteenth century. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 31, 2035.Google Scholar
Ricciardelli, L. (1992). Creativity and bilingualism. Journal of Creative Behavior, 26, 242–54.Google Scholar
Richards, R., Kinney, D. K., Lunde, I., Benet, M., & Merzel, A. P. C. (1988). Creativity in manic-depressives, cyclothymes, their normal relatives, and control subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 281–8.Google Scholar
Ritter, S. M., Damian, R. I., Simonton, D. K., van Baaren, R., Strick, M., et al. (2012). Diversifying experiences enhance cognitive flexibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 961–4.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W. (2006). Personality Development and Organizational Behavior (Chap. 1, pp. 141). In Staw, B. M. (ed.), Research on Organizational Behavior. New York; Elsevier Science/JAI Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W., & Caspi, A. (2003). The Cumulative Continuity Model of Personality Development: Striking a Balance between Continuity and Change in Personality Traits across the Life Course (pp. 183214). In Staudinger, R. M. & Lindenberger, U. (eds.), Understanding Human Development: Lifespan Psychology in Exchange with Other Disciplines. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. (2003). Work experiences and personality development in young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 582–93.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W., Walton, K., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 125.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W., & Wood, D. (2006). Personality Development in the Context of the Neo-Socioanalytic Model of Personality (Chap. 2, pp. 1139). In Mroczek, D. & Little, T. (eds.), Handbook of Personality Development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrance Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Roe, A. (1953). The Making of a Scientist. New York: Dodd, Mead.Google Scholar
Rounds, J., & Su, R. (2014). The nature and power of interests. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(2), 98103.Google Scholar
Saad, C. S., Damian, R. I., Benet-Martinez, V., Moons, W. G., & Robins, R. W. (2013). Multiculturalism and creativity: effects of cultural context, bicultural identity, and cognitive fluency. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 369–75.Google Scholar
Sawyer, R. K. (2012). Explaining Creativity: The Science of Human Innovation (2nd edn.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Schaefer, C., & Anastasi, A. (1968). A biographical inventory for identifying creativity in adolescent boys. Journal of Applied Psychology, 58, 42–8.Google Scholar
Schlesinger, J. (2009). Creative mythconceptions: a closer look at the evidence for the “mad genius” hypothesis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 6272.Google Scholar
Shonk, S. M., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Maltreatment, competency deficits, and risk for academic and behavioral maladjustment. Developmental Psychology, 37(1), 3.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1983). Formal education, eminence, and dogmatism: the curvilinear relationship. Journal of Creative Behavior, 17, 149–62.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1984a). Artistic creativity and interpersonal relationships across and within generations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 1273–86.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1984b). Genius, Creativity, and Leadership: Historiometric Inquiries. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1986). Biographical typicality, eminence, and achievement style. Journal of Creative Behavior, 20, 1422.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1992). The social context of career success and course for 2,026 scientists and inventors. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 452–63.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1994). Greatness: Who Makes History and Why. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1999). Origins of Genius: Darwinian Perspectives on Creativity. NewYork: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2004). Creativity in Science: Chance, Logic, Genius, and Zeitgeist. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2008). Bilingualism and Creativity. In Altarriba, J. & Heredia, R. R. (eds.), An Introduction to Bilingualism: Principles and Processes (pp. 147–66). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrance Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2009). Genius 101. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2012). Taking the US Patent Office creativity criteria seriously: a quantitative three-criterion definition and its implications. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 97106.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K., & Damian, R. I. (2013). Creativity. In Reisberg, D. (ed.), Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology (pp. 795807). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2014a). More method in the mad-genius controversy: a historiometric study of 204 historic creators. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 5361.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2014b). The mad-genius paradox: can creative people be more mentally healthy but highly creative people more mentally ill? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 470–80.Google Scholar
Tadmor, C. T., Galinsky, A. D., & Maddux, W. W. (2012). Getting the most out of living abroad: biculturalism and integrative complexity as key drivers of creative and professional success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 520–42.Google Scholar
Terman, L. M. (1954). Scientists and non-scientists in a group of 800 gifted men. PsychologicalMonographs, 68(7), 144.Google Scholar
van Knippenberg, D., & Schippers, M. (2007). Work group diversity. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 515–41.Google Scholar
Visher, S. (1947). Starred scientists: a study of their ages. American Scientist, 35, 543, 570, 572, 574, 576, 578, 580.Google Scholar
Woodward, W. (1974). Scientific genius and loss of a parent. Science Studies, 4, 265–77.Google Scholar

References

Amiot, C. E., de la Sablonniere, R., Terry, D. J., & Smith, J. R. (2007). Integration of social identities in the self: toward a cognitive-developmental model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 364388. doi: 10.1177/1088868307304091Google Scholar
Benet-Martínez, V. (2012). Multiculturalism: Cultural, Social, and Personality Processes. In Deaux, K. & Snyder, M. (eds.), Oxford Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology (pp. 623–48). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Benet-Martinez, V., & Haritatos, J. (2005). Bicultural identity integration (BII): components and psychosocial antecedents. Journal of Personality, 73, 1015–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1467–6494.2005.00337.xGoogle Scholar
Benet-Martnez, V., Leu, J., Lee, F., & Morris, M. W. (2002). Negotiating biculturalism: cultural frame switching in biculturals with oppositional versus compatible cultural identities. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 492516. doi: 10.1177/0022022102033005005Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 46, 568. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.1997.tb01087.xGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 240–50. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I. M. (2010). Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 1923. doi: 10.1177/0963721409358571Google Scholar
Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: a longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Developement, 78, 246–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1467–8624.2007.00995.xGoogle Scholar
Burnette, J. L., O’Boyle, E. H., Vanepps, E. M., Pollack, J. M., & Finkel, E. J. (2013). Mind-sets matter: a meta-analytic review of implicit theories and self-regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 655701. doi: 10.1037/a0029531Google Scholar
Carr, P. B., Rattan, A., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Implicit Theories Shape Intergroup Relations. In Devine, P. & Plant, A. (eds.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 45, pp. 127–65). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Carringer, D. C. (1974). Creative thinking abilities of Mexican youth: the relationship of bilingualism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 5, 492504. doi: 10.1177/002202217400500409Google Scholar
Chang, J. H., Hsu, C. C., Shih, N. H., & Chen, H. C. (2014). Multicultural families and creative children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45, 1288–96. doi: 10.1177/0022022114537556Google Scholar
Chang, J. H., Su, J. S., & Chen, H. C. (2015). Cultural distance between parents’ and children’s creativity: a within-country approach in Taiwan. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 21, 477–85. doi:10.1037/a0037539Google Scholar
Chen, S. X. (2015). Toward a social psychology of bilingualism and biculturalism. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 111. doi: 10.1111/ajsp.12088Google Scholar
Cheng, C.-Y., Sanchez-Burks, J., & Lee, F. (2008). Connecting the dots within: creative performance and identity integration. Psychological Science, 19, 1178–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1467–9280.2008.02220.xGoogle Scholar
Crisp, R. J., & Turner, R. N. (2011). Cognitive adaptation to the experience of social and cultural diversity. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 242–66. doi: 10.1037/a0021840Google Scholar
Fee, A., & Gray, S. J. (2012). The expatriate-creativity hypothesis: a longitudinal field test. Human Relations, 65, 1515–38. doi: 10.1177/0018726712454900Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290308. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0204_5Google Scholar
Fischer, R. (2011). Cross-cultural training effects on cultural essentialism beliefs and cultural intelligence. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 35, 767–75. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.08.005Google Scholar
Gocłowska, M. A., & Crisp, R. J. (2014). How dual-identity processes foster creativity. Review of General Psychology, 18, 216–36. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000008Google Scholar
Godart, F. C., Maddux, W. W., Shipilov, A. V., & Galinsky, A. D. (2015). Fashion with a foreign flair: professional experiences abroad facilitate the creative innovations of organizations. Academy of Management Journal, 58, 195220. doi: 10.5465/amj.2012.0575Google Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hennessey, B. A., & Amabile, T. M. (2010). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 569–98. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100416Google Scholar
Hong, Y. Y., Chao, M. M., & No, S. (2009). Dynamic interracial/intercultural processes: the role of lay theories of race. Journal of Personality, 77, 12831310. doi: 10.1111/j.1467–6494.2009.00582.xGoogle Scholar
Hong, Y. Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C. Y., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: a dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709–20. doi: 10.1037//0003–066X.55.7.709Google Scholar
Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Ego depletion – is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychological Science, 21, 1686–93. doi: 10.1177/0956797610384745Google Scholar
Karwowski, M. (2014). Creative mindsets: measurement, correlates, consequences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 6270. doi:10.1037/a0034898Google Scholar
Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2014). The cognitive neuroscience of insight. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 7193. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213–115154Google Scholar
Lee, C. S., Therriault, D. J., & Linderholm, T. (2012). On the cognitive benefits of cultural experience: exploring the relationship between studying abroad and creative thinking. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 768–78. doi: 10.1002/acp.2857Google Scholar
Leung, A. K.-Y., & Chiu, C.-Y. (2008). Interactive effects of multicultural experiences and openness to experience on creative potential. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 376–82. doi: 10.1080/10400410802391371Google Scholar
Leung, A. K.-Y., & Chiu, C.-Y. (2010). Multicultural experience, idea receptiveness, and creativity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 723–41. doi: 10.1177/0022022110361707Google Scholar
Leung, A. K.-Y., Maddux, W. W., Galinsky, A. D., & Chiu, C.-Y. (2008). Multicultural experience enhances creativity: the when and how. American Psychologist, 63, 169–81. doi: 10.1037/0003–066X.63.3.169.Google Scholar
Maddux, W. W., Adam, H., & Galinsky, A. D. (2010). When in Rome … learn why the Romans do what they do: how multicultural learning experiences facilitate creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 731–41. doi: 10.1177/0146167210367786Google Scholar
Maddux, W. W., Bivolaru, E., Hafenbrack, A. C., Tadmor, C. T., & Galinsky, A. D. (2014). Expanding opportunities by opening your mind: multicultural engagement predicts job market success through longitudinal increases in integrative complexity. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, 608–15. doi: 10.1177/1948550613515005Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P., & Pals, J. L. (2006). A new Big Five: fundamental principles for an integrative science of personality. American Psychologist, 61, 204–17. doi: 10.1037/0003–066x.61.3.204Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1258–65. doi: 10.1037//0022–3514.52.6.1258Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509–16.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60, 175215. doi: 10.1111/j.1467–6494.1992.tb00970.xGoogle Scholar
Mischel, W. (2004). Toward an integrative science of the person. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 122. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.042902.130709Google Scholar
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102, 246–68. doi: 10.1037/0033–295X.102.2.246Google Scholar
Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in psychology: a lay theories approach to self-regulation, social perception, and social development. American Psychologist, 61, 192203. doi: 10.1037/0003–066X.61.3.192Google Scholar
No, S., Hong, Y. Y., Liao, H. Y., Lee, K., Wood, D., et al. (2008). Lay theory of race affects and moderates Asian Americans’ responses toward American culture. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 9911004. doi: 10.1037/a0012978Google Scholar
Rokeach, M. (1960). The Open and Closed Mind. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2004). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 657–87. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141502Google Scholar
Saad, C. S., Damian, R. I., Benet-Martinez, V., Moons, W. G., & Robins, R. W. (2013). Multiculturalism and creativity: effects of cultural context, bicultural identity, and ideational fluency. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 369–75. doi: 10.1177/1948550612456560Google Scholar
Saucier, G. (1994). Mini-Markers: a brief version of Goldberg Unipolar Big-5 Markers. Journal of Personality Assessment, 63, 506–16. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6303_8Google Scholar
Schleider, J. L., Abel, M. R., & Weisz, J. R. (2015). Implicit theories and youth mental health problems: a random-effects meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 35, 19. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2014.11.001Google Scholar
Tadmor, C. T., Chao, M. M., Hong, Y. Y., & Polzer, J. T. (2013). Not just for stereotyping anymore: racial essentialism reduces domain-general creativity. Psychological Science, 24, 99105. doi: 10.1177/0956797612452570Google Scholar
Tadmor, C. T., Galinsky, A. D., & Maddux, W. W. (2012). Getting the most out of living abroad: biculturalism and integrative complexity as key drivers of creative and professional success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 520–42. doi: 10.1037/a0029360Google Scholar
Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1049–62. doi: 10.1037/0022–3514.67.6.1049Google Scholar
Woo, S. E., Chernyshenko, O. S., Longley, A., Zhang, Z. X., Chiu, C. Y., et al. (2014). Openness to experience: its lower level structure, measurement, and cross-cultural equivalence. Journal of Personality Assessment, 96, 2945. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2013.806328Google Scholar
Wu, J.-J., & Albanese, D. (2010). Asian creativity, chapter one: creativity across three Chinese societies. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 5, 150–4. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2010.10.002Google Scholar
Wu, J.-J., Chen, F.-Y., Kuo, C.-H., Lin, W.-W., Liu, S.-H., et al. (1999). The Newly Developed Creativite Thinking Test: A Research Report for the Ministry of Education’s Six-Year Student Counseling Program. Taipei, Taiwan: Foundation for Scholarly Exchange.Google Scholar
Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Mindsets that promote resilience: when students believe that personal characteristics can be developed. Educational Psychologist, 47, 302–14. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2012.722805Google Scholar
Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Creativity as flexible cognitive control. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 136–43. doi: 10.1037/a0017379Google Scholar

References

Acar, S., & Runco, M. A. (2012). Psychoticism and creativity: a meta-analytic review. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(4), 341–50. doi: 10.1037/a0027497Google Scholar
Ambrose, D., Cohen, L. N. M., & Tannenbaum, J. A. (eds.) (2003). Creative Intelligence: Toward Theoretic Integration. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.Google Scholar
Barron, F., & Harrington, D. (1981). Creativity, intelligence, and personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 32, 439–76.Google Scholar
Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence, and personality: a critical review of the scattered literature. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 132(4), 355429.Google Scholar
Bink, M. L., & Marsh, R. L. (2000). Cognitive regularities in creative activity. Review of General Psychology, 4(1), 5978.Google Scholar
Brophy, D. R. (2000). Comparing the attributes, activities, and performance of divergent, convergent, and combination thinkers. Creativity Research Journal, 13(3–4), 439–55.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380400.Google Scholar
Caprara, G. V., & Perugini, M. (1994). Personality described by adjectives: the generalizability of the Big Five to the Italian lexical context. European Journal of Personality, 8(5), 357–69. doi: 10.1002/per.2410080502Google Scholar
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 499506. doi: 10.1037/0022–3514.85.3.499Google Scholar
Chaiken, S., & Ledgerwood, A. (2012). A Theory of Heuristic and Systematic Information Processing. In Van Lange, P., Kruglanski, A., & Higgins, E. (eds.), Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 246–67). London: Sage. doi: doi: 10.4135/9781446249215.n13Google Scholar
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1999). Temperament: A New Paradigm for Trait Psychology. In Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (pp. 399423). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Costa, P. T. J., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Four ways five factors are basic. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 653–65.Google Scholar
Cropley, A. J. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 18(3), 391404.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the Psychology of Discovery and Invention. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
DeDreu, C. K. W., Baas, M., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). Hedonic tone and activation level in the mood–creativity link: toward a dual pathway to creativity model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(5), 739–56. doi: 10.1037/0022–3514.94.5.739Google Scholar
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(3), 491517.Google Scholar
De Raad, B., Hendriks, A. A. J., & Hofstee, W. K. B. (1992). Towards a refined structure of personality traits. European Journal of Personality, 6(4), 301–19. doi: 10.1002/per.2410060405Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G. (2006). Higher-order factors of the Big Five in a multi-informant sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(6), 1138–51.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Grazioplene, R. G., & Peterson, J. B. (2012). From madness to genius: the openness/intellect trait domain as a paradoxical simplex. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(1), 6378. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.12.003Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 880–96. doi: 10.1037/0022–3514.93.5.880Google Scholar
Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1246–56.Google Scholar
Domino, G., & Giuliani, I. (1997). Creativity in three samples of photographers: a validation of the adjective check list creativity scale. Creativity Research Journal, 10(2), 193200. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj1002&3_7Google Scholar
Epstein, S. (1990). Cognitive-Experiential Self-Theory: An Integrative Theory of Personality. In Pervin, L. (ed.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (pp. 165–92). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes, R. V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 390405.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1992a). Four ways five factors are not basic. Personality and Individual Differences, 13(6), 667–73. doi: 10.1016/0191–8869(92)90237-JGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1992b). The definition and measurement of psychoticism. Personality and Individual Differences, 13(7), 757–85. doi: 10.1016/0191–8869(92)90050-YGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1993). Creativity and personality: suggestions for a theory. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 147–78.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The Natural History of Creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(4), 290309.Google Scholar
Feldman, D. H., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Gardner, H. (1994). Changing the World: A Framework for the Study of Creativity. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers/Greenwood Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Fürst, G., Ghisletta, P., & Lubart, T. (2014). Toward an integrative model of creativity and personality: theoretical suggestions and preliminary empirical testing. Journal of Creative Behavior (available online at http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/. Doi: 10.1002/jocb.71/epdfGoogle Scholar
Galenson, D. W. (2006). Old Masters and Young Geniuses: The Two Life Cycles of Artistic Creativity. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Gocłowska, M. A., & Crisp, R. J. (2014). How dual-identity processes foster creativity. Review of General Psychology, 18(3), 216–36. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000008Google Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4, 2642.Google Scholar
Gough, H. G. (1979). A creative personality scale for the Adjective Check List. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(8), 13981405. doi: 10.1037/0022–3514.37.8.1398Google Scholar
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–54.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R. (1995). Fluid Concepts and Creative Analogies: Computer Models of the Fundamental Mechanisms of Thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122–31.Google Scholar
John, O. P., Robins, R. W., & Pervin, L. A. (2008). Handbook of Personality. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five Trait Taxonomy: History, Measurement, and Theoretical Perspectives. In Pervin, L. A. & John, O. P. (eds.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (pp. 102–38). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, J. A. (1994). Clarification of factor five with the help of the AB5C model. European Journal of Personality, 8(4), 311–34. doi: 10.1002/per.2410080408Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Kéri, S. (2011). Solitary minds and social capital: latent inhibition, general intellectual functions and social network size predict creative achievements. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts (available online at http://doi.org/10.1037/a0022000).Google Scholar
Kirton, M. (1976). Adaptors and innovators: a description and measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61(5), 622–9.Google Scholar
Kozhevnikov, M. (2007). Cognitive styles in the context of modern psychology: toward an integrated framework of cognitive style. Psychological Bulletin, 133(3), 464–81. doi: 10.1037/0033–2909.133.3.464Google Scholar
Kwang, N. A., & Rodrigues, D. (2002). A Big-Five personality profile of the adaptor and innovator. Journal of Creative Behavior, 36(4), 254–68. doi: 10.1002/j.2162–6057.2002.tb01068.xGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E., & Gewirtz, J. C. (1995). Latent inhibition in humans: data, theory, and implications for schizophrenia. Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 87103. doi: 10.1037/0033–2909.117.1.87Google Scholar
Lubow, R. E., Ingberg-Sachs, Y., Zalstein-Orda, N., & Gewirtz, J. C. (1992). Latent inhibition in low and high “psychotic-prone” normal subjects. Personality and Individual Differences, 13(5), 563–72. doi: 10.1016/0191–8869(92)90197-WGoogle Scholar
Martindale, C. (1999). Biological Bases of Creativity. In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.), Handbook of Creativity (pp. 137–52). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1258–65.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1997). Conceptions and Correlates of Openness to Experience. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J., & Briggs, S. (eds.), Handbook of Personality Psychology (pp. 825–47). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1999). A Five-Factor Theory of Personality. In Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (pp. 139–53). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Molenaar, P. C. M. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Measurement, 2(4), 201–18.Google Scholar
Molenaar, P. C. M., & Campbell, C. G. (2009). The new person-specific paradigm in psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 112.Google Scholar
Mussel, P. (2013). Intellect: a theoretical framework for personality traits related to intellectual achievements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(5), 885906. doi: 10.1037/a0031918Google Scholar
Newell, A., & Simon, H. (1972). Human Problem Solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Nusbaum, E. C., & Silvia, P. J. (2010). Are intelligence and creativity really so different? Fluid intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use in divergent thinking. Intelligence, 39(1), 3645. doi: 16/j.intell.2010.11.002Google Scholar
Peterson, J. B., & Carson, S. H. (2000). Latent inhibition and openness to experience in a high-achieving student population. Personality and Individual Differences, 28(2), 323–32. doi: 16/S0191-8869(99)00101–4Google Scholar
Peterson, J. B., Smith, K. W., & Carson, S. H. (2002). Openness and extraversion are associated with reduced latent inhibition: replication and commentary. Personality and Individual Differences, 33(7), 1137–47. doi: 16/S0191-8869(02)00004–1Google Scholar
Plucker, J. A., Beghetto, R. A., & Dow, G. T. (2004). Why isn’t creativity more important to educational psychologists? potentials, pitfalls, and future directions in creativity research. Educational Psychologist, 39(2), 8396.Google Scholar
Rea, D. (2003). Optimal Motivation for Creative Intelligence. In Creative Intelligence: Toward Theoretic Integration (pp. 211–35). New York: Hampton Press.Google Scholar
Richards, R., Kinney, D. K., Lunde, I., Benet, M., & Merzel, A. P. L. (1988). Creativity in manic-depressives, cyclothymes, their normal relatives and control subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 281–8.Google Scholar
Rothenberg, A. (1996). The janusian process in scientific creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 9(2), 207231. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj0902&3_8Google Scholar
Saucier, G. (1994). Mini-markers: a brief version of Goldberg’s unipolar Big-Five markers. Journal of Personality Assessment, 63(3), 506–16.Google Scholar
Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (2001). Lexical studies of indigenous personality factors: premises, products, and prospects. Journal of Personality, 69(6), 847–79. doi: 10.1111/1467–6494.696167Google Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1991). Happy and Mindless, but Sad and Smart? The Impact of Affective States on Analytic Reasoning. In Forgas, J. P. (ed.), Emotion and Social Judgment (pp. 5571). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Schuldberg, D. (2010). Chaos Theory and Creativity. In Runco, M. A. & Pritzker, S. R. (eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity (Vol. 1, pp. 259–72). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Pretz, J. E. (2009). Is creativity domain-specific? latent class models of creative accomplishments and creative self-descriptions. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3(3), 139–48. doi: 10.1037/a0014940Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., & Kimbrel, N. A. (2010). A dimensional analysis of creativity and mental illness: do anxiety and depression symptoms predict creative cognition, creative accomplishments, and creative self-concepts? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(1), 210. doi: 37/a0016494Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Berg, C., Martin, C., & O’Connor, A. (2009). Openness to experience, plasticity, and creativity: exploring lower-order, high-order, and interactive effects. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(6), 1087–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.04.015Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1997). Creative productivity: a predictive and explanatory model of career trajectories and landmarks. Psychological Review, 104(1), 6689.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the Crowd: Cultivating Creativity in a Culture of Conformity. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Suler, J. R. (1980). Primary process thinking and creativity. Psychological Bulletin, 88(1), 144–65.Google Scholar
Vosburg, S. K. (1998). The effect of positive and negative mood on divergent thinking performance. Creativity Research Journal, 11(2), 165–72.Google Scholar
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1993). Behavioral Disinhibition versus Constraint: A Dispositional Perspective. In Wegner, D. M. & Pennebaker, J. W. (eds.), Handbook of Mental Control (pp. 506–27). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Zenasni, F., Besançon, M., & Lubart, T. (2008). Creativity and tolerance of ambiguity: an empirical study. Journal of Creative Behavior, 42(1), 6173. doi: 10.1002/j.2162–6057.2008.tb01080.xGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, M., & Cloninger, C. R. (1996). Relationships between Cloninger’s, Zuckerman’s, and Eysenck’s dimensions of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 21(2), 283–5. doi: 10.1016/0191–8869(96)00042–6Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., Joireman, J., Teta, P., & Kraft, M. (1993). A comparison of three structural models for personality: the Big Three, the Big Five, and the Alternative Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 757–68.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×