Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-jwnkl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-13T20:37:21.206Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

18 - Is there a general skill in perceiving others accurately?

from Part III - Conclusions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2016

Judith A. Hall
Affiliation:
Northeastern University, Boston
Marianne Schmid Mast
Affiliation:
Université de Lausanne, Switzerland
Tessa V. West
Affiliation:
New York University
Get access

Summary

Abstract

Over the past decades, there has been increased interest in and recognition of the importance of interpersonal accuracy (IPA) for successful social interaction. Yet, while there has been some success in creating instruments that assess individuals’ accuracy in inferring others’ personality, affect and emotions, thoughts and feelings, deception, and social affordances, there have been no systematic efforts to examine the degree of overlap between these measures. The relatively few attempts in which relationships have been reported have shown surprisingly low correlations. Here, we utilized meta-analytic techniques to map out how different assessments of IPA are related to each other and to evaluate the question whether there is a global IPA skill. The results revealed a modest but significant overall correlation that was moderated by domain and nonverbal channels available in each measure. Implications for a unified construct of IPA are discussed.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1993). Half a minute: Predicting teacher evaluations from thin slices of nonverbal behavior and physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 431441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambady, N. E., & Skowronski, J. J. (2008). First impressions. New York: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
App, B., McIntosh, D. N., Reed, C. L., & Hertenstein, M. J. (2011). Nonverbal channel use in communication of emotion: How may depend on why. Emotion, 11, 603617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bänziger, T., Grandjean, D., Scherer, K. R., & Scherer, K. R. (2009). Emotion recognition from expressions in face, voice, and body. The Multimodal Emotion Recognition Test (MERT). Emotion, 9, 691704.Google ScholarPubMed
Bänziger, T., Scherer, K. R., Hall, J. A., & Rosenthal, R. (2011). Introducing the MiniPONS: A Short Multichannel Version of the Profile of Nonverbal Sensitivity (PONS). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 35, 189204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biehl, M., Matsumoto, D., Ekman, P., Hearn, V., Heider, K., Kudoh, T., & Ton, V. (1997). Matsumoto and Ekman’s Japanese and Caucasian Facial Expressions of Emotion (JACFEE): Reliability data and cross-national differences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 21, 321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bollen, K., & Lennox, R. (1991). Conventional wisdom on measurement: A structural equation perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 305314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boone, R. T., & Buck, R. (2003). Emotional expressivity and trustworthiness: The role of nonverbal behavior in the evolution of cooperation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 163182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boone, R. T., & Buck, R. (2004). Emotion receiving ability: A new view of measuring individual differences in the ability to accurate. In Geher, G. (Ed.), Measuring emotional intelligence: Common ground and controversy (pp. 7389). New York: Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Boone, R. T., & Macy, M. W. (1999). Unlocking the doors to the prisoner’s dilemma: Dependence, selectivity, and cooperation. Social Psychology Quarterly, 62, 3252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogart, K. R., & Matsumoto, D. (2010). Facial mimicry is not necessary to recognize emotion: Facial expression recognition by people with Moebius syndrome. Social Neuroscience, 5, 241251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bombari, D., Schmid Mast, M., Brosch, T., & Sander, D. (2013). How interpersonal power affects empathic accuracy: Differential roles of mentalizing versus mirroring? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1992). Trait inferences: Sources of validity at zero acquaintance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 645657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the representative design of psychological experiments (2nd ed.). Berkeley: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buck, R. (1976). A test of nonverbal receiving ability: Preliminary studies. Human Communication Research, 2, 162171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buck, R. (1984). The communication of emotion. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Buck, R. (2014). Emotion: A biosocial synthesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlston, D. E. (1992). Impression formation and the modular mind: The Associated Systems Theory. In Martin, L. L. & Tesser, A. (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 301341). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carlston, D. E. (1994). Associated Systems Theory: A systematic approach to cognitive representations of persons. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carney, D. R., Colvin, C. R., & Hall, J. A. (2007). A thin slice perspective on the accuracy of first impressions. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 10541072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castro, V. L., & Boone, R. T. (2015). Sensitivity to spatiotemporal percepts predicts the perception of emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 39, 215240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christman, S. D. (2001). Individual differences in Stroop and local-global processing: A possible role of interhemispheric interaction. Brain and Cognition, 45, 97118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costanzo, M., & Archer, D. (1989). Interpreting the expressive behavior of others: The Interpersonal Perception Task. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 225245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeMeijer, M. (1989). The contribution of general features of body movement to the attribution of emotions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 247268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DePaulo, B. M., Kirkendol, S. E., Tang, J., & O’Brien, T. P. (1988). The motivational impairment effect in the communication of deception: Replications and extensions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12, 177202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DePaulo, B. M., LeMay, C. S., & Epstein, J. A. (1991). Effects of importance of success and expectations for success on effectiveness at deceiving. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 1424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeVellis, R. F. (2012). Scale development: Theory and applications (Vol. 26). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Dimitrov, D. M. (2003). Marginal true-score measures and reliability for binary items as a function of their IRT parameters. Applied Psychological Measurement, 27, 440458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial Action Coding System: A technique for the measurement of facial movement. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1974). Nonverbal behavior and psychopathology. In Friedman, R. J. & Katz, M. (Eds.), The psychology of depression: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 331). Washington, DC: Winston & Sons.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face. New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., O’Sullivan, M., Chan, A., Diacoyanni-Tarlatzis, I., Heider, K., … & Tzavaras, A. (1987). Universals and cultural differences in the judgments of facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 712717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Embretson, S. E., & Reise, S. P. (2000). Item response theory for psychologists. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Epskamp, S., Cramer, A. O., Waldorp, L. J., Schmittmann, V. D., & Borsboom, D. (2012). Qgraph: Network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. Journal of Statistical Software, 48, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, M. G., & Stennett, J. (2001). The forced-choice paradigm and the perception of facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 7585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franks, R. H. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of emotions. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Fruchterman, T. M., & Reingold, E. M. (1991). Graph drawing by force-directed placement. Software: Practice and Experience, 21, 11291164.Google Scholar
Frühholz, S., Schlegel, K., & Grandjean, D. (2015). Amygdala structure and core dimensions of the affective personality. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 396403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halberstadt, A. G., Parker, A. E., & Castro, V. L. (2013). Nonverbal communication: Developmental perspectives. In Hall, J. A. & Knapp, M. L. (Eds.), Handbook of Communication Science (Vol. 2, pp. 93128). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hall, J. A. (2001). The PONS Test and the psychometric approach to measuring interpersonal sensitivity. In Hall, J. A. & Bernieri, F. J. (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 143160). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, J. A., Andrzejewski, S. A., Murphy, N. A., Schmid Mast, M., & Feinstein, B. (2008). Accuracy of judging others’ traits and states: Comparing mean levels across tests. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 14761489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, J. A., Andrzejewski, S. A., & Yopchick, J. E. (2009). Psychosocial correlates of interpersonal sensitivity: A meta-analysis. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33, 149180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, J. A., Bernieri, F. J., & Carney, D. R. (2005). Nonverbal behavior and interpersonal sensitivity. In Harrigan, J. A., Rosenthal, R., & Scherer, K. R. (Eds.), Handbook of nonverbal behavior research methods in the affective sciences (pp. 237281). New York: Oxford.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, J. A., & Schmid Mast, M. (2008). Are women always more interpersonally sensitive than men? Impact of goals and content domain. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 144155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, J. A., & Matsumoto, D. (2004). Gender differences in judgments of multiple emotions from facial expressions. Emotion, 4, 201206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1993). Emotional contagion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 9699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heberlein, A. S., & Saxe, R. R. (2005). Dissociation between emotion and personality judgments: Convergent evidence from functional neuroimaging. NeuroImage, 28, 770777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, U., & Blairy, S. (2001). Facial mimicry and emotional contagion to dynamic emotional facial expressions and their influence on decoding accuracy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40, 129141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, C. E. (1994). Innate and universal facial expressions: Evidence from developmental and cross-cultural research. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 288299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kang, S.-M. (2012). Individual differences in recognizing spontaneous emotional expressions: Their implications for positive interpersonal relationships. Psychology, 3, 11831188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, D. A. (2013). Issues in the measurement of judgmental accuracy. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Lombardo, M. V. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: perspectives from developmental social neuroscience (pp. 104116). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Social cognitive neuroscience. In Fiske, S. T., Gilbert, D. T., & Gardner, L. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed., pp. 143193). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., LeRoux, J., Wilson-Cohn, C., Raroque, J., Kooken, K., Ekman, P., … Goh, A. (2000). A new Test to measure emotion recognition ability: Matsumoto and Ekman’s Japanese and Caucasian Brief Affect Recognition Test (JACBART). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 179209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (1994). Individual differences in the nonverbal communication of affect: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy Scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orbell, J., & Dawes, R. (1993). Social welfare, cooperator’s advantage, and the option of not playing the game. American Sociological Review, 58, 787800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puccinelli, N. M., & Tickle-Degnen, L. (2004). Knowing too much about others: Moderators of the relationship between eavesdropping and rapport in social interaction. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 223243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., & Archer, R. (1979). Sensitivity to nonverbal communication: The PONS Test. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Rosip, J. C., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Knowledge of nonverbal cues, gender, and nonverbal decoding accuracy. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 267286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rule, N. O., Garrett, J. V., & Ambady, N. (2010). On the perception of religious group membership from faces. PLoS ONE 5: e14241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expressions? A review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 102141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. R., & Oshinsky, J. S. (1977). Cue utilization in emotion attribution from auditory stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 331346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K. R., & Scherer, U. (2011). Assessing the Ability to Recognize Facial and Vocal Expressions of Emotion: Construction and Validation of the Emotion Recognition Index. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 35, 305326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schlegel, K., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2012). Emotion recognition: Unidimensional ability or a set of modality- and emotion-specific skills? Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 1621.Google Scholar
Schlegel, K., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2014). Introducing the Geneva Emotion Recognition Test: An example of Rasch-based test development. Psychological Assessment, 26, 666672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternglanz, R., & DePaulo, B. (2004). Reading nonverbal cues to emotions: The advantages and liabilities of relationship closeness. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 245266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tickle-Degnen, L. (1998). Working well with others: The prediction of students’ clinical performance. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 52, 133142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Overwalle, F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 829858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. (2011). Reintegrating the study of accuracy into social cognition research. Psychological Inquiry, 22, 159182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zebrowitz, L. A. (2001). Groping for the elephant of interpersonal sensitivity. In Hall, J. A. & Bernieri, F. J. (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 333350). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×