Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4rdrl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-21T05:02:34.369Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2015

Aleksandra Maria Rogowska
Affiliation:
Politechnika Opolska
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abdi, H. (2003). Partial least squares (PLS-regression). In Lewis-Beck, M., Bryman, A., and Futing, T. (Eds.), Encyclopedia for research methods for the social sciences (pp. 792–5). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Abdi, H. (2010). Partial least squares regression and projection on latent structure regression (PLS regression). Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Computational Statistics, 2, 97106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Afra, P., Funke, M., and Matsuo, F. (2009). Acquired auditory-visual synesthesia: a window to early cross-modal sensory interactions. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 2, 31–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Ahmadi, J., Keshtkar, M., and Pridmore, S. (2011). Methamphetamine induced synesthesia: a case report. American Journal on Addictions, 20(3), 306.Google Scholar
Allbutt, J., Ling, J., Rowley, M., and Shafiullah, M. (2011). Vividness of visual imagery and social desirable responding: correlations of the vividness of visual imagery questionnaire with the balanced inventory of desirable responding and the Marlowe-Crowne Scale. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 791–9.Google Scholar
Alstadhaug, K. B., and Benjaminsen, E. (2010). Synesthesia and migraine: case report. BMC Neurology, 10, 121.Google Scholar
Amin, M., Olu-Lafe, O., Claessen, L. E., Sobczak-Edmans, M., Ward, J., Williams, A. L., and Sagiv, N. (2011). Understanding grapheme personification: a social synaesthesia? Journal of Neuropsychology, 5, 255–82.Google Scholar
Anderson, K. J. (1990). Arousal and the inverted-U hypothesis: a critique of Neiss’s ‘Reconceptualizing arousal.’ Psychological Bulletin, 107, 96100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angleitner, A., and Riemann, R. (1991). What can we learn from the discussion of personality questionnaires for the construction of temperament inventories? In Strelau, J. and Angleitner, A. (Eds.), Explorations in temperament: international perspectives on theory and measurement (pp. 191204). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armel, K. C., and Ramachandran, V. S. (1999). Acquired synesthesia in retinitis pigmentosa. Neurocase, 5, 293–6.Google Scholar
Arnau, R. C., Green, B. A., Rosen, D. H., Gleaves, D. H., and Melancon, J. G. (2003). Are Jungian preferences really categorical?: an empirical investigation using taxometric analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 233–51.Google Scholar
Arteaga, C., and Poblano, A. (2008). Handedness of children determines preferential facial and eye movements related to hemispheric specialization. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 66, 488–93.Google Scholar
Asano, M., and Yokosawa, K. (2012). Synesthetic colors for Japanese late acquired graphemes. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 983–93.Google Scholar
Asher, J. E., Aitken, M. R. F., Farooqi, N., Kurmani, S., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Diagnosing and phenotyping visual synaesthesia: a preliminary evaluation of the Revised Test of Genuineness (TOG-R). Cortex, 42, 137–46.Google Scholar
Asher, J. E., Lamb, J. A., Brocklebank, D., Cazier, J.-B., Maestrini, E., Addis, L., Sen, M., Baron-Cohen, S., and Monaco, A. P. (2009). A whole-genome scan and fine-mapping linkage study of auditory-visual synesthesia reveals evidence of linkage to chromosomes 2q24, 5q33, 6p12, and 12p12. American Journal of Human Genetics, 84, 279–85.Google Scholar
Bailey, M. E. S., and Johnson, K. J. (1997). Synaesthesia: is genetic analysis feasible? In Baron-Cohen, S. and Harrison, J. (Eds.), Synaesthesia: classic and contemporary readings (pp. 182210). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Banissy, M. J., Cohen Kadosh, R., Maus, G. W., Walsh, V., and Ward, J. (2009). Prevalence, characteristics and a neurocognitive model of mirror-touch synaesthesia. Experimental Brain Research, 198, 261–72.Google Scholar
Banissy, M. J., Garrido, L., Kusnir, F., Duchaine, B., Walsh, V., and Ward, J. (2011). Superior facial expression, but not identity recognition, in mirror-touch synesthesia. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1820–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banissy, M. J., Walsh, V., and Ward, J. (2009). Enhanced sensory perception in synaesthesia. Experimental Brain Research, 196, 565–71.Google Scholar
Banissy, M. J., and Ward, J. (2007). Mirror-touch synaesthesia is linked with empathy. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 815–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barber, T. X., and Wilson, S. C. (1979). Guided imagining and hypnosis: theoretical and empirical overlap and convergence in a new creative imagination scale. In Sheikh, A. A. and Shaffer, J. T. (Eds.), The potential of fantasy and imagination (pp. 6788). New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Barclay, J. R. (1993). The regulative theory of temperament: implications for assessment and teaching. McGill Journal of Education, 28, 181212.Google Scholar
Bargary, G., Barnett, K. J., Mitchell, K. J., and Newell, F. N. (2009). Colored-speech synaesthesia is triggered by multisensory, not unisensory, perception. Psychological Science, 20, 529–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargary, G., and Mitchell, K. J. (2008). Synaesthesia and cortical connectivity. Trends in Neurosciences, 31, 335–42.Google Scholar
Barnett, K. J., Feeney, J., Gormley, M., and Newell, F. N. (2009). An exploratory study of linguistic–colour associations across languages in multilingual synaesthetes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1343–55.Google Scholar
Barnett, K. J., Finucane, C., Asher, J. E., Bargary, G., Corvin, A. P., Newell, F. N., and Mitchell, K. J. (2008). Familial patterns and the origins of individual differences in synaesthesia. Cognition, 106, 871–93.Google Scholar
Barnett, K. J., Foxe, J. J., Molholm, S., Kelly, S. P., Shalgi, S., Mitchell, K. J., and Newell, F. N. (2008). Differences in early sensory-perceptual processing in synesthesia: a visual evoked potential study. NeuroImage, 43(3), 605–13.Google Scholar
Barnett, K. J., and Newell, F. N. (2008). Synaesthesia is associated with enhanced, self-rated visual imagery. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 1032–9.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Bor, D., Billington, J., Asher, J., Wheelwright, S., and Ashwin, C. (2007). Savant memory in a man with colour form-number synaesthesia and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14, 237–51.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Burt, L., Smith-Laittan, F., Harrison, J., and Bolton, P. (1996). Synaesthesia: prevalence and familiarity. Perception, 25, 1073–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Harrison, J., Goldstein, L. H., and Wyke, M. (1993). Coloured speech perception: is synaesthesia what happens when modularity breaks down? Perception, 22, 419–26.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wyke, M. A., and Binnie, C. (1987). Hearing words and seeing colours: an experimental investigation of a case of synaesthesia. Perception, 16, 761–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, A. L. (1992). Benton Visual Retention Test (5th edn.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Berman, G. (1999). Synesthesia and the arts. Leonardo, 32, 1522.Google Scholar
Berteletti, I., Hubbard, E. M., and Zorzi, M. (2010). Implicit versus explicit interference effects in a number-color synesthete. Cortex, 46, 170–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bien, N., ten Oever, S., Goebel, R., and Sack, A. T. (2012). The sound of size crossmodal binding in pitch-size synesthesia: a combined TMS, EEG and psychophysics study. NeuroImage, 59, 663–72.Google ScholarPubMed
Biocca, F., Kim, J., and Choi, Y. (2001). Visual touch in virtual environments: an exploratory study of presence, multimodal interfaces, and cross-modal sensory illusions. Presence, 10, 247–65.Google Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J., Bristow, D., Bird, G., Frith, C., and Ward, J. (2005). Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision–touch synaesthesia. Brain, 128, 1571–83.Google Scholar
Bor, D., Billington, J., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2007). Savant memory for digits in a case of synaesthesia and Asperger syndrome is related to hyperactivity in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Neurocase, 13, 311–19.Google Scholar
Borst, G., and Kosslyn, S. M. (2008). Visual mental imagery and visual perception: structural equivalence revealed by scanning processes. Memory and Cognition, 36, 849–62.Google Scholar
Brang, D., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2008). Psychopharmacology of synaesthesia: the role of serotonin S2a receptor activation. Medical Hypotheses, 70, 903–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brang, D., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2010). Visual field heterogeneity, laterality, and eidetic imagery in synesthesia. Neurocase, 16, 169–74.Google Scholar
Brang, D., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2011). Survival of the synesthesia gene: why do people hear colors and taste words? PloS Biology, 9, e1001205. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1001205.Google Scholar
Brang, D., Rouw, R., Ramachandran, V. S., and Coulson, S. (2011). Similarly shaped letters evoke similar colors in grapheme–color synesthesia. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1355–8.Google Scholar
Brang, D., Teuscher, U., Miller, L. E., Ramachandran, V. S., and Coulson, S. (2011). Handedness and calendar orientations in time–space synaesthesia. Journal of Neuropsychology, 5, 323–32.Google Scholar
Brang, D., Teuscher, U., Ramachandran, V. S., and Coulson, S. (2010). Temporal sequences, synesthetic mappings, and cultural biases: the geography of time. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 311–20.Google Scholar
Brang, D., Williams, L. E., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2012). Grapheme-color synaesthetes show enhanced crossmodal processing between auditory and visual modalities. Cortex, 48, 630–7.Google Scholar
Brickenkamp, R. (1962). Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test (Test d2). [The d2 Test of Attention]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Brickenkamp, R. (1994). Test d2 Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test. Handanweisung (8th expanded and revised edition). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Brickenkamp, R. (2002). Test d2. Test badania uwagi. Podręcznik [The d2 Test of Attention. Manual].Warszawa: ERDA.Google Scholar
Brown, T. (1827). Of the external affections combined with desire. In Brown, T. (Ed.), A treatise on the philosophy of the human mind, being the lectures of the late Thomas Brown (vol. I, pp. 209–20). Cambridge, MA: Hilliard and Brown. doi:10.1037/11701-016.Google Scholar
Brzozowski, P., and Drwal, R. Ł. (2008). Kwestionariusz Osobowości Eysencka. Polska adaptacja EPQ-R. Podręcznik [Eysenck Personality Questionnaire: Polish adaptation of the EPQ-R. Manual]. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.Google Scholar
Buss, A., and Plomin, R. (1975). A temperament theory of personality development. New York: Wiley-Interscience.Google Scholar
Buss, A., and Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Callejas, A., Acosta, A., and Lupiáñez, J. (2007). Green love is ugly: emotions elicited by synesthetic grapheme-color perceptions. Brain Research, 1127, 99107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carriere, J. S. A., Eaton, D., Reynolds, M. G., Dixon, M. J., and Smilek, D. (2008). Grapheme–color synesthesia influences overt visual attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 246–58.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: their structure, growth, and action. New York: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Cawley, V. (2010). The synaesthete: a new type of ‘gifted student’ and how to teach them. Procedia – Social and Behavioral Sciences, 7(C), 574–9.Google Scholar
Cazeaux, C. (2002). Metaphor and categorization of senses. Metaphor and Symbol, 17, 326.Google Scholar
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Furnham, A., and Ackerman, P. L. (2006). Ability and personality correlates of general knowledge. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 419–29.Google Scholar
Chiou, R., Stelter, M., and Rich, A. N. (2013). Beyond colour perception: auditory-visual synaesthesia induces experiences of geometric objects in specific locations. Cortex, 49(6), 1750–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clements, A. M., Rimrodt, S. L., Abel, J. R., Blankner, J. G., Mostofsky, S. H., Pekar, J. J., Denckla, M. B., and Cutting, L. E. (2006). Sex differences in cerebral laterality of language and visuospatial processing. Brain and Language, 98, 150–8.Google Scholar
Cohen Kadosh, R., Cohen Kadosh, K., and Henik, A. (2007). The neuronal correlate of bidirectional synesthesia: a combined event-related potential and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 2050–9.Google Scholar
Cohen Kadosh, R., and Henik, A. (2006). Color congruity effect: where do colors and numbers interact in synesthesia? Cortex, 42, 259–63.Google Scholar
Cohen Kadosh, R., and Henik, A. (2007). Can synaesthesia research inform cognitive science? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 177–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen Kadosh, R., Henik, A., Catena, A., Walsh, V., and Fuentes, L. J. (2009). Induced cross-modal synaesthetic experience without abnormal neuronal connections. Psychological Science, 20, 258–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen Kadosh, R., Henik, A., and Walsh, V. (2007). Small is bright and big is dark in synaesthesia. Current Biology, 17, 834–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen Kadosh, R., Henik, A., and Walsh, V. (2009). Synaesthesia: learned or lost? Developmental Science, 12, 484–91.Google ScholarPubMed
Cohen Kadosh, R., Sagiv, N., Linden, D. E. J., Robertson, L. C., Elinger, G., and Henik, A. (2005). When blue is larger than red: colors influence numerical cognition in synesthesia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1766–73.Google Scholar
Cohen Kadosh, R., and Walsh, V. (2008). Synaesthesia and cortical connections: cause or correlation? Trends in Neurosciences, 31, 549–50.Google Scholar
Collier, G. L. (1996). Affective synesthesia: extracting emotion space from simple perceptual stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 20, 132.Google Scholar
Collins, A. M., and Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240–7.Google Scholar
Colom, R., Jung, R. E., and Haier, R. J. (2006). Finding the g-factor in brain structure using the method of correlated vectors. Intelligence, 34, 561−70.Google Scholar
Coricelli, G. (2005). Two-levels of mental states attribution: from automaticity to voluntariness. Neuropsychologia, 43, 294300.Google Scholar
Corulla, W. J., Strelau, J., Angleitner, A., and Ruch, W. (1993). The development of the Revised Pavlovian Temperament Survey in England: continuing research. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 323–8.Google Scholar
Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO personality inventory professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Cudeck, R., and O’Dell, L. L. (1994). Applications of standard error estimates in unrestricted factor analysis: significance tests for factor loadings and correlations. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 475–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cui, X., Jeter, C. B., Yang, D., Montague, P. R., and Eagleman, D. M. (2007). Vividness of mental imagery: individual variability can be measured objectively. Vision Research, 47, 474–8.Google Scholar
Cytowic, R. E. (1993). The man who tasted shapes (4th edn.). London: Abacus.Google Scholar
Cytowic, R. E. (1997). Synaesthesia: phenomenology and neuropsychology – a review of current knowledge. In Baron-Cohen, S. and Harrison, J. (Eds.), Synaesthesia: classic and contemporary readings (pp. 1739). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cytowic, R. E. (2002). Synaesthesia: a union of the senses (2nd edn.). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Dael, N., Sierro, G., and Mohr, C. (2013). Affect-related synesthesias: a prospective view on their existence, expression and underlying mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(754), 19 (www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3798864).Google Scholar
Dailey, A., Martindale, C., and Borkum, J. (1997). Creativity, synesthesia, and physionomic perception. Creativity Research Journal, 10, 18.Google Scholar
Dajek, E. R. (2010). Polska standaryzacja Testu d2, testu badania uwagi R. Brickenkampa [Polish standardization of Brickenkamp’s d2 Test of Attention]. Warszawa: ERDA.Google Scholar
Day, L. B., and MacNeilage, P. F. (1996). Postural asymmetries and language lateralization in humans (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110, 8896.Google Scholar
Day, S. (2005). Some demographic and socio-cultural aspects of synesthesia. In Robertson, L. C. and Sagiv, N. (Eds.), Synesthesia: perspectives from cognitive neuroscience (pp. 1133). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
de Pascalis, V., Zawadzki, B., and Strelau, J. (2000). The Formal Characteristics of Behaviour – Temperament Inventory (FCB-TI): preliminary results of the Italian version. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 287–98.Google Scholar
de Thornley Head, P. (2006). Synaesthesia: pitch-colour isomorphism in RGB-space? Cortex, 42, 164–74.Google Scholar
Devereux, G. (1966). An unusual audio-motor synesthesia in an adolescent. Psychiatric Quarterly, 40(1–4), 459–71.Google Scholar
de Vignemont, F., and Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: how, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 435–41.Google Scholar
Díaz, B., Hintz, F., Kiebel, S. J., and von Kriegstein, K. (2012). Dysfunction of the auditory thalamus in developmental dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 13841–6.Google Scholar
Dixon, M. J., and Smilek, D. (2005). The importance of individual differences in grapheme-color synesthesia. Neuron, 45, 821–3.Google Scholar
Dixon, M. J., Smilek, D., Cudahy, C., and Merikle, P. M. (2000). Five plus two equals yellow. Nature, 406, 365.Google Scholar
Dixon, M. J., Smilek, D., Duffy, P. L., Zanna, M. P., and Merikle, P. M. (2006). The role of meaning in grapheme-colour synaesthesia. Cortex, 42, 243–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, M. J., Smilek, D., and Merikle, P. (2004). Not all synaesthetes are created equal: projector versus associator synaesthetes. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 335–43.Google Scholar
Dollinger, S. J., Urban, K. K., and James, T. A. (2004). Creativity and openness: further validation of two creative product measures. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 3547.Google Scholar
Domino, G. (1989). Synesthesia and creativity in fine arts students: an empirical look. Creativity Research Journal, 2, 1729.Google Scholar
Domino, G. (1999). Synesthesia. In Runco, M. A. and Pritzker, S. R. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity (vol. II, pp. 597604). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Eagleman, D. M. (2012). Synaesthesia in its protean guises. British Journal of Psychology, 103, 1619.Google Scholar
Eagleman, D. M., Kagan, A. D., Nelson, S. S., Sagaram, D., and Sarma, A. K. (2007). A standardized test battery for the study of synesthesia. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 159, 139–45.Google Scholar
Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183201.Google Scholar
Edgar, C., McRorie, M., and Sneddon, I. (2012). Emotional intelligence, personality and the decoding of non-verbal expressions of emotion. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 295300.Google Scholar
Edquist, J., Rich, A. N., Brinkman, C., and Mattingley, J. B. (2006). Do synaesthetic colours act as unique features in visual search? Cortex, 42, 222–31.Google Scholar
Elias, L. J., and Bryden, M. P. (1998). Footedness is a better predictor of language lateralization than handedness. Laterality, 3, 4151.Google Scholar
Elias, L. J., Saucier, D. M., Hardie, C., and Sarty, G. E. (2003). Dissociating semantic and perceptual components of synaesthesia: behavioural and functional neuroanatomical investigations. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 232–7.Google Scholar
Evans, K. K., and Treisman, A. (2010). Natural cross-modal mappings between visual and auditory features. Journal of Vision, 10, 112.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Thomas.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1992). A reply to Costa and McCrae. P or A and C - the role of theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 867–8.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J., and Eysenck, S. B. G. (1964). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Inventory. University of London Press.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J., and Eysenck, S. B. G (1975) Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. London: Hodder and Stoughton.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J., and Eysenck, S. B. G (1976). Psychoticism as a dimension of personality. London: Hodder and Stoughton.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J., Eysenck, S. G. B., and Barrett, P. (1985). A revised version of the Psychoticism Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 21–9.Google Scholar
Eysenck, M. W. (1975). Arousal and speed of recall. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 14, 269–77.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290309.Google Scholar
Finch, J. F., and Graziano, W. G. (2001). Predicting depression from temperament, personality, and patterns of social relations. Journal of Personality, 69, 27–55.Google Scholar
Fitzgibbon, B. M., Enticott, P. G., Bradshaw, J. L., Giummarra, M. J., Chou, M., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., and Fitzgerald, P. B. (2012). Enhanced corticospinal response to observed pain in pain synesthetes. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 406–18.Google Scholar
Fitzgibbon, B. M., Enticott, P. G., Rich, A. N., Giummarra, M. J., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., and Bradshaw, J. L. (2012). Mirror-sensory synaesthesia: exploring ‘shared’ sensory experiences as synaesthesia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 645–57.Google Scholar
Fitzgibbon, B. M., Giummarra, M. J., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., Enticott, P. G., and Bradshaw, J. L. (2010). Shared pain: from empathy to synaesthesia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 500–12.Google Scholar
Förster, J. (2012). GLOMOsys: the how and why of global and local processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 1519.Google Scholar
Freedberg, D., and Gallese, V. (2007). Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 197203.Google Scholar
Frith, U. (2003). Autism: explaining the enigma (2nd edn.). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
Galaburda, A. M., Sherman, G. F., Rosen, G. D., Aboitiz, F., and Geschwind, N. (1985). Developmental dyslexia: four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies. Annals of Neurology, 18, 222–33.Google Scholar
Galeyev, B. (1993). The problem of synaesthesia in the arts. Languages of Design, 1, 201–3.Google Scholar
Ganiban, J. M., Saudino, K. J., Ulbricht, J., Neiderhiser, J. M., and Reiss, D. (2008). Stability and change in temperament during adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 222–36.Google Scholar
Gazor, S., and Zhang, W. (2003). Speech probability distribution. IEEE Signal Processing Letters, 10, 204–7.Google Scholar
Gheri, C., Chopping, S., and Morgan, M. J. (2008). Synaesthetic colours do not camouflage form in visual search. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 275, 841–6.Google Scholar
Giummarra, M. J., and Bradshaw, J. L. (2009). Synaesthesia for pain: feeling pain with another. In Pineda, J. A. (Ed.), Mirror neuron systems (pp. 287307). New York: Humana Press.Google Scholar
Glicksohn, J., Alon, A., Perlmutter, A., and Purisman, R. (2001). Symbolic and syncretic cognition among schizophrenics and visual artists. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 133–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glicksohn, J., and Barrett, T. R. (2003). Absorption and hallucinatory experience. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 833–49.Google Scholar
Glicksohn, J., Salinger, O., and Roychman, A. (1992). An exploratory study of syncretic experience: eidetics, synaesthesia and absorption. Perception, 21, 637–42.Google Scholar
Glicksohn, J., Steinbach, I., and Elimalach-Malmilyan, S. (1999). Cognitive dedifferentation in eidetics and synaesthesia: hunting for the ghost once more. Perception, 28, 109–20.Google Scholar
Glisky, M. L., Tataryn, D. J., Tobias, B. A., and Kihlstrom, J. F. (1991). Absorption, openness to experience, and hypnotizability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 263–72.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, H. H., and Rothbart, J. K. (1991). Contemporary instruments for assessing early temperament by questionnaires and in the laboratory. In Strelau, J. and Angleitner, A. (Eds.), Explorations in temperament: international perspectives on theory and measurement (pp. 249–72). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Goller, A. I., Richards, K., Novak, S., and Ward, J. (2013). Mirror-touch synaesthesia in the phantom limbs of amputees. Cortex, 49(1), 243–51.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1964). Strength of the nervous system and levels of arousal: a reinterpretation. In Gray, J. A. (Ed.), Pavlov’s typology: recent theoretical and experimental developments from the laboratory of B. M. Teplov (pp. 289366). Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Goller, A. I., Richards, K., Novak, S., and Ward, J. (1981). A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality. In Eysenck, H. J. (Ed.), A model for personality (pp. 246–76). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Goller, A. I., Richards, K., Novak, S., and Ward, J. (2005). Synesthesia: a window on the hard problem of consciousness. In Robertson, L. C. and Sagiv, N. (Eds.), Synesthesia: perspectives from cognitive neuroscience (pp. 127–46). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A., Williams, S. C. R., Nunn, J., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Possible implications of synaesthesia for the hard question of consciousness. In Baron-Cohen, S. and Harrison, J. (Eds.), Synaesthesia: classic and contemporary readings (pp. 173–81). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Green, J. A. K., and Goswami, U. (2008). Synesthesia and number cognition in children. Cognition, 106, 463–73.Google Scholar
Griffin, M., and McDermott, M. R. (1998). Exploring a tripartite relationship between rebelliousness, openness to experience and creativity. Social Behavior and Personality, 26, 347–56.Google Scholar
Gross, V. C., Neargarderô, S., Caldwell-Harris, C. L., and Cronin-Golomb, A. (2011). Superior encoding enhances recall in color-graphemic synesthesia. Perception, 40, 196208.Google Scholar
Grossenbacher, P. G. (1997). Perception and sensory information in synesthetic experience. In Baron-Cohen, S. and Harrison, J. (Eds.), Synaesthesia: classic and contemporary readings (pp. 148–72). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Grossenbacher, P. G., and Lovelace, C. T. (2001). Mechanisms of synesthesia: cognitive and physiological constraints. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 3641.Google Scholar
Guencer, B., and Oral, G. (1993). Relationship between creativity and nonconformity to school discipline as perceived by teachers of Turkish elementary schoolchildren, by controlling for their grade and sex. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 20, 208–14.Google Scholar
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Habib, M. (2000). The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia: an overview and working hypothesis. Brain, 123, 2373–99.Google Scholar
Halpern, S. F. (1992). Sex differences in cognitive abilities. New York: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hänggi, J., Wotruba, D., and Jäncke, L. (2011). Globally altered structural brain network topology in grapheme-color synesthesia. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 5816–28.Google Scholar
Harrison, J. E., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Synaesthesia: reconciling the subjective with the objective. Endeavour, 19, 157–60.Google Scholar
Harrison, J. E., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Synaesthesia: an introduction. In Baron-Cohen, S. and Harrison, J. E. (Eds.), Synaesthesia: classic and contemporary readings (pp. 316). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Heaton, P., and Wallace, G. L. (2004). Annotation: the savant syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 45, 899911.Google Scholar
Heim, S., and Keil, A. (2004). Large-scale neural correlates of developmental dyslexia. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 125–40.Google Scholar
Hernaandez, S., Camacho-Rosales, J., Nieto, A., and Barroso, J. (1997). Cerebral asymmetry and reading performance: effect of language lateralization and hand preference. Child Neuropsychology, 3, 206–25.Google Scholar
Hertz, P. (1999). Synesthetic art – an imaginary number? Leonardo, 32, 399404.Google Scholar
Hochel, M., and Milán, E. G. (2008). Synaesthesia: the existing state of affairs. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 93117.Google Scholar
Hochel, M., Milán, E. G., Martín, J. L. M., González, A., García, E. D., Tornay, F., and Vila, J. (2009). Congruence or coherence? Emotional and physiological responses to colours in synaesthesia. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 703–23.Google Scholar
Holle, H., Banissy, M., Wright, T., Bowling, N., and Ward, J. (2011). “That’s not a real body”: identifying stimulus qualities that modulate synaesthetic experiences of touch. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 720–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, N. P., and Spence, C. (2005). Visual bias of unseen hand position with a mirror: spatial and temporal factors. Experimental Brain Research, 166, 489–97.Google Scholar
Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., and Mullen, M. R. (2008). Structural equation modelling: guidelines for determining model fit. Electronic Journal of Business Research Methods, 6, 5360.Google Scholar
Houde, O., and Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2003). Neural foundations of logical and mathematical cognition. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 4, 507−14.Google Scholar
Hubbard, E. M. (2007). Neurophysiology of synesthesia. Current Psychiatry Reports, 9, 193–9.Google Scholar
Hubbard, E. M., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2005). Neurocognitive mechanisms of synesthesia. Neuron, 48, 509–20.Google Scholar
Hubbard, T. I. (1996). Synesthesia-like mappings of lightness, and melodic interval. American Journal of Psychology, 109, 219–28.Google Scholar
Hupka, R. B., Zaleski, Z., Otto, J., Reidl, L., and Tarabrina, N. V. (1997). The colors of anger, envy, fear, and jealousy: a cross-cultural study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28, 156–71.Google Scholar
Jackson, C. J., and Francis, L. J. (1999). Interpreting the correlation between neuroticism and Lie Scale stores. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 5963.Google Scholar
Jacome, D. E. (1999). Volitional monocular Lilliputian visual hallucinations and synesthesia. European Neurology, 42, 54–6.Google Scholar
Jäncke, L., Beeli, G., Eulig, C., and Hänggi, J. (2009). The neuroanatomy of grapheme–color synesthesia. European Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1287–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jansari, A. S., Spiller, M. J., and Redfern, S. (2006). Number synaesthesia: when hearing ‘four plus five’ looks like gold. Cortex, 42, 253–8.Google Scholar
Jarick, M., Dixon, M. J., and Smilek, D. (2011). 9 is always on top: assessing the automaticity of synaesthetic number-forms. Brain and Cognition, 77, 96105.Google Scholar
Jarick, M., Dixon, M. J., Stewart, M. T., Maxwell, E. C., and Smilek, D. (2009). A different outlook on time: visual and auditory month names elicit different mental vantage points for a time-space synaesthete. Cortex, 45, 1217–28.Google Scholar
Jawer, M. (2005). Environmental sensitivity: a neurobiological phenomenon? Seminars in Integrative Medicine, 3, 104–9.Google Scholar
Jaworowska, A. (2007). Test Pamięci Wzrokowej Bentona. Polska normalizacja [Benton Visual Retention Test. Polish Normalization]. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.Google Scholar
Jaworowska, A., and Matczak, A. (2005). Popularny Kwestionariusz Inteligencji Emocjonalnej PKIE. Podręcznik [The Popular Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (PEIQ). Manual]. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.Google Scholar
Jaworowska, J., and Szustrowa, T. (2000). Test Matryc Ravena w wersji Standard. Formy: klasyczna, równoległa, Plus. Polska standaryzacja. Podręcznik [Polish adaptation of Raven’s Progressive Matrices test: Classic, Parallel, and Plus versions. Manual]. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.Google Scholar
Jellen, H. G., and Urban, K. K. (1989). Assessing creative potential world-wide: the first cross-cultural application of the Test for Creative Thinking – Drawing Production (TCT-DP). Gifted Education International, 6, 7886.Google Scholar
Jung, C. G. (1971). Psychological types. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Jürgens, U. M., and Nikolić, D. (2012). Ideaesthesia: conceptual processes assign similar colours to similar shapes. Translational Neuroscience, 3(1), 22–7.Google Scholar
Just, C. (2011). A review of literature on the general factor of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 765–71.Google Scholar
Kafka, J. S. (1997). Romantic and classic visions in therapy of psychosis: a personal perspective and evolving theory of schizophrenia. Psychiatry, 60, 262–71.Google Scholar
Kagan, J., and Snidman, N. (1991). Temperamental factors in human development. American Psychologist, 46, 856–62.Google Scholar
Kalbfleisch, M. L. (2009). The neural plasticity of giftedness. In Shavinina, L. V. (Ed.), International handbook on giftedness (vol. II, pp. 275–93). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Kansaku, K., and Kitazawa, S. (2001). Imaging studies on sex differences in the lateralization of language. Neuroscience Research, 41, 333–7.Google Scholar
Keith, A. (2009). The connotations of English colour terms: colour-based X-phemisms. Journal of Pragmatics, 41, 626–37.Google Scholar
Kensinger, E. A., and Corkin, S. (2003). Memory enhancement for emotional words: are emotional words more vividly remembered than neutral words? Memory and Cognition, 31, 1169–80.Google Scholar
Kim, C. -Y., Blake, R., and Palmeri, T. J. (2006). Perceptual interaction between real and synesthetic colors. Cortex, 42(2), 195203.Google Scholar
King, L. A., McKee Walker, L., and Broyles, S. J. (1996). Creativity and the five factor model. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 189203.Google Scholar
Koles, Z. J., Lind, J. C., and Flor-Henry, P. (2010). Gender differences in brain functional organization during verbal and spatial cognitive challenges. Brain Topography, 23, 199204.Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. (1994). Image and brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kustubayeva, A. (2010). Post-Soviet psychology and individual differences in cognition: a psychophysiological perspective. In Gruszka, A., Matthews, G., and Szymura, B. (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in cognition: attention, memory and cognition (pp. 145–68). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Laeng, B., Hugdahl, K., and Specht, K. (2011). The neural correlate of colour distances revealed with competing synaesthetic and real colours. Cortex, 47, 320–31.Google Scholar
Laeng, B., Svartdal, F., and Oelmann, H. (2004). Does color synaesthesia pose a paradox for early-selection theories of attention? Psychological Science, 15, 277–81.Google Scholar
Lee, K. H., Choi, Y. Y., Gray, J. R., Cho, S. H., Chae, J., Lee, S., and Kim, K. (2006). Neural correlates of superior intelligence: stronger recruitment of posterior parietal cortex. NeuroImage, 29, 578–86.Google Scholar
Lengua, L. J., Long, A. C., Smith, K. I., and Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). Pre-attack symptomatology and temperament as predictors of children’s responses to the September 11 terrorist attacks. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 631–45.Google Scholar
Linssen, M. I. (2011). Synaesthesia and enhanced memory performance: a review of the literature. Macquarie Matrix, 1, 3453.Google Scholar
Loring, D. W., Meador, K., Lee, G., Murro, A., Smith, J., Flanigin, B., Gallagher, D., and King, D. (1990). Cerebral language lateralization: evidence from intracarotid amobarbital testing. Neuropsychologia, 28, 831–8.Google Scholar
Luke, D. (2011). Discarnate entities and dimethyltryptamine (DMT): psychopharmacology, phenomenology and ontology. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research, 75, 2642.Google Scholar
Lupiáñez, J., and Callejas, A. (2006). Automatic perception and synaesthesia: evidence from colour and photism naming in a Stroop-negative priming task. Cortex, 42, 204–12.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1968). The mind of a mnemonist: a little book about a vast memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Makioka, S. A. (2009). Self-organizing learning account of number-form synaesthesia. Cognition, 112, 397414.Google Scholar
Mann, H., Korzenko, J., Carriere, J. S. A., and Dixon, M. J. (2009). Time–space synaesthesia – a cognitive advantage? Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 619–27.Google Scholar
Marks, L. E. (1975). On colored-hearing synesthesia: cross-modal translations of sensory dimensions. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 303–31.Google Scholar
Marks, L. E. (1989). On cross-modal similarity: the perceptual structure of pitch, loudness, and brightness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 589602.Google Scholar
Marks, L. E., Hammeal, R. J., and Bornstein, M. H. (1987). Perceiving similarity and comprehending metaphor. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 52(1), 1102.Google Scholar
Martino, G., and Marks, L. E. (2001). Synesthesia: strong and weak forms. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 61–5.Google Scholar
Masunaga, H., Kawashima, R., Horn, J. L., Sassa, Y., and Sekiguchi, A. (2008). Neural substrates of the Topology Test to measure fluid reasoning: an fMRI study. Intelligence, 36, 607–15.Google Scholar
Matczak, A., Jaworowska, A., and Stańczak, J. (2000). Rysunkowy Test Twórczego Myślenia K. K. Urbana i H. G. Jellena TCT-DP. Podręcznik [Test for Creative Thinking – Drawing Production (TCT-DP) by K. K. Urban and H. G. Jellen. Manual]. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.Google Scholar
Mathiesen, K. S., and Tambs, K. (1999). The EAS temperament questionnaire – factor structure, age trends, reliability, and stability in a Norwegian sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 40, 431–9.Google Scholar
Mattingley, J. B., Payne, J. M., and Rich, A. N. (2006). Attentional load attenuates synaesthetic priming effects in grapheme-color synaesthesia. Cortex, 42, 213–21.Google Scholar
Mattingley, J. B., Rich, A. N., Yelland, G., and Bradshaw, J. L. (2001). Unconscious priming eliminates automatic binding of colour and alphanumeric form in synaesthesia. Nature, 410, 580–2.Google Scholar
Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., and Salovey, P. (1999). Emotional intelligence meets traditional standards for an intelligence. Intelligence, 27, 267–98.Google Scholar
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D. R., and Sitarenios, G. (2001). Emotional intelligence as a standard intelligence. Emotion, 1, 232–42.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1258–65.Google Scholar
McDonald, R. P., and Ho, M.-H. R. (2002). Principles and practice in reporting statistical equation analyses. Psychological Methods, 7, 6482.Google Scholar
McDougall, S., and Pfeifer, G. (2012). Personality differences in mental imagery and the effects on verbal memory. British Journal of Psychology, 103, 556–73.Google Scholar
Mednick, S. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220–32.Google Scholar
Mednick, S. A., and Mednick, M. T. (1967). Examiner’s manual: Remote Associates Test. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Meier, B., and Rothen, N. (2007). When conditioned responses ‘fire back’: bidirectional cross-activation creates learning opportunities in synesthesia. Neuroscience, 147, 569–72.Google Scholar
Meier, B., and Rothen, N. (2009). Training grapheme-colour associations produces a synaesthetic Stroop effect, but not a conditioned synaesthetic response. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1208–11.Google Scholar
Michielsen, H. J., de Vries, J., and van Heck, G. L. (2003). In search of personality and temperament predictors of chronic fatigue: a prospective study. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1073–87.Google Scholar
Mikolaczak, M., Roy, E., Verstrynge, V., and Luminet, O. (2009). An exploration of the moderating effect of trait emotional intelligence on memory and attention in neutral and stressful conditions. British Journal of Psychology, 100, 699715.Google Scholar
Milán, E. G., Iborra, O., Hochel, M., Artacho, M. A. R., Delgado-Pastor, L. C., Salazar, E., and González-Hernández, A. (2012). Auras in mysticism and synaesthesia: a comparison. Consciousness and Cognition, 2, 258–68.Google Scholar
Mills, C. B., Boteler, E. H., and Oliver, G. K. (1999). Digit synaesthesia: a case study using a Stroop-type test. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16, 181–91.Google Scholar
Mills, C. B., Innis, J., Westendorf, T., Owsianiecki, L., and McDonald, A. (2006). Effect of a synesthete’s photisms on name recall. Cortex, 42, 155–63.Google Scholar
Milne, E., and Szczerbinski, M. (2009). Global and local perceptual style, field-independence, and central coherence: an attempt at concept validation. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 5, 126.Google Scholar
Mon-Williams, M., Wann, J. P., Jenkinson, M., and Rushton, K. (1997). Synaesthesia in the normal limb. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 264, 1007–10.Google Scholar
Morton, B. E., and Rafto, S. E. (2010). Behavioral laterality advance: neuroanatomical evidence for the existence of hemisity. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 3442.Google Scholar
Moutafi, J., Furnham, A., and Crump, J. (2006). What facets of openness and conscientiousness predict fluid intelligence score? Learning and Individual Differences, 16, 3142.Google Scholar
Mroczko, A., Metzinger, T., Singer, W., and Nikolić, D. (2009). Immediate transfer of synesthesia to a novel inducer. Journal of Vision, 9, 18.Google Scholar
Mulvenna, C. M., and Walsh, V. (2005). Synaesthesia. Current Biology, 15, 399400.Google Scholar
Mulvenna, C. M., and Walsh, V. (2006). Synaesthesia: supernormal integration? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 350–2.Google Scholar
Murray, A. L. (2010). Can the existence of highly accessible concrete representations explain savant skills? Some insights from synaesthesia. Medical Hypotheses, 74, 1006–12.Google Scholar
Musek, J. (2007). A general factor of personality: evidence for the Big One in the five-factor model. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 1213–33.Google Scholar
Mussell, P. (2010). Epistemic curiosity and related constructs: lacking evidence of discriminant validity. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 506–10.Google Scholar
Myers, I. B., and McCaulley, M. H. (1985). Manual: a guide to the development and use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Myles, K. M., Dixon, M. J., Smilek, D., and Merikle, P. M. (2003). Seeing double: the role of meaning in alphanumeric-colour synaesthesia. Brain and Cognition, 53, 342–5.Google Scholar
Naerde, A., Røysamb, E., and Tambs, K. (2004). Temperament in adults – reliability, stability, and factor structure of the EAS Temperament Survey. Journal of Personality Assessment, 82, 71–9.Google Scholar
Nelson, M. R., and Hitchon, J. C. (1999). Loud tastes, colored fragrances, and scented sounds: how and when to mix the senses in persuasive communications. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 76(2), 354–72.Google Scholar
Newberry, B. H., Clark, W. B., Crawford, R. L., Strelau, J., Angleitner, A., Jones, J. H., and Eliasz, A. (1997). An American English version of the Pavlovian Temperament Survey. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 105–14.Google Scholar
Niccolai, V., van Leeuwen, T. M., Blakemore, C., and Stoerig, P. (2012). Synaesthetic perception of colour and visual space in a blind subject: an fMRI case study. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 889–99.Google Scholar
Nijboer, T. C. W., Satris, G., and van der Stigchel, S. (2011). The influence of synesthesia on eye movements: no synesthetic pop-out in an oculomotor target selection task. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1193–200.Google Scholar
Nijboer, T. C. W., and van der Stigchel, S. (2009). Is attention essential for inducing synesthetic colors? Evidence from oculomotor distractors. Journal of Vision, 9, 19.Google Scholar
Nikolić, D. (2009). Is synaesthesia actually ideaesthesia? An inquiry into the nature of the phenomenon. Proceedings of the Third International Congress on Synaesthesia, Science & Art, Granada, Spain, 2629 April (www.danko-nikolic.com/wp-content/uploads/2011/09/Synesthesia2009-Nikolic-Ideaesthesia.pdf).Google Scholar
Nikolić, D., Jürgens, U. M., Rothen, N., Meier, B., and Mroczko, A. (2011). Swimming-style synesthesia. Cortex, 47, 874–9.Google Scholar
Nold, M. G. (1997). Synesthesia and blindness: a personal account and informal survey. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 91, 1415.Google Scholar
Nosal, C. S. (1992). Diagnoza typów umysłu [Diagnosis of types of mind]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.Google Scholar
Nosal, C. S. (2001). Psychologia myślenia i działania menadżera [Psychology of the thinking and acting of a manager]. Kraków: Wydawnictwo AKADE.Google Scholar
Novich, S., Cheng, S., and Eagleman, D. M. (2011). Is synaesthesia one condition or many? A large-scale analysis reveals subgroups. Journal of Neuropsychology, 5, 353–71.Google Scholar
O’Donovan, D. (1969). An historical review of the Lie Scale: with particular reference to the Maudsley Personality Inventory. Papers in Psychology, 3, 1319.Google Scholar
O’Grady, K. (1980). The absorption scale: a factor analytic study. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 28, 281–8.Google Scholar
Oniszczenko, W. (1997). Kwestionariusz temperamentu EAS Arnolda Bussa i Roberta Plomina. Wersje dla dorosłych i dla dzieci. Adaptacja polska. Podręcznik [Arnold Buss and Robert Plomin’s EAS Temperament Survey for adults and children: Polish adaptation. Manual]. Warsaw: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.Google Scholar
Ott, U., Reuter, M., Hennig, J., and Vaitl, D. (2005). Evidence for a common biological basis of the absorption trait, hallucinogen effects, and positive symptoms: epistasis between 5-HT2a and COMT polymorphisms. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 137 B, 2932.Google Scholar
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: a dual coding approach. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Palmeri, T. J., Blake, R., Marois, R., Flanery, M. A., and Whetsell, W. (2002). The perceptual reality of synesthetic colors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 4127–31.Google Scholar
Patrick, C. J., Curtin, J. J., and Tellegen, A. (2002). Development and validation of a brief form of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 14, 150–63.Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1941). The conditioned reflex. In Gantt, W. H. (Ed.), Lectures on conditioned reflexes: conditioned reflexes and psychiatry (vol. II, pp. 168–70). New York: International Publishers.Google Scholar
Pekala, R. J., Wenger, C. F., and Levine, R. L. (1985). Individual differences in phenomenological experience: states of consciousness as a function of absorption. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 125–32.Google Scholar
Pernet, C. R., Poline, J. B., Demonet, J. F., and Rousselet, G. A. (2009). Brain classification reveals the right cerebellum as the best biomarker of dyslexia. BMC Neuroscience, 10, 119.Google Scholar
Perona-Garcelán, S., Carrascoso-López, F., García-Montes, J. M., Vallina-Fernández, O., Pérez-Álvarez, M., Ductor-Recuerda, M. J., Salas-Azcona, R., Cuevas-Yust, C., and Gómez-Gómez, M. T. (2011). Depersonalization as a mediator in the relationship between self-focused attention and auditory hallucinations. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 12, 535–48.Google Scholar
Peters, M. (1995). Handedness and its relation to other indices of cerebral lateralization. In Davidson, R. and Hugdahl, A. (Eds.), Brain asymmetry (pp. 183215). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1936). Origins of intelligence in the child. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Podoll, K., and Robinson, D. (2002). Auditory-visual synaesthesia in a patient with basilar migraine. Journal of Neurology, 249, 476–7.Google Scholar
Popek, S. (2000). Kwestionariusz Twórczego Zachowania KANH [The Creative Behaviour Questionnaire (KANH)]. Lublin: UMCS.Google Scholar
Powell, J. L., Kemp, G. J., and García-Finaña, M. (2012). Association between language and spatial laterality and cognitive ability: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 59, 1818–29.Google Scholar
Pozella, D. J., and Hassen, J. L. (1997). Aesthetic preferences for combinations of color and music. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 960–2.Google Scholar
Prabhakaran, V., Smith, J. A. L., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1997). Neural substrates of fluid reasoning: an fMRI study of neocortical activation during performance of Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 4363.Google Scholar
Price, M. (2009). Spatial forms and mental imagery. Cortex, 45, 1229–45.Google Scholar
Putnam, S. P., Ellis, L. K., and Rothbart, M. K. (2001). The structure of temperament from infancy through adolescence. In Eliasz, A. and Angeleitner, A. (Eds.), Advances in research on temperament (pp. 165–82). Lengerich: Pabst Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Rader, C. M., and Tellegen, A. (1981). A comparison of synesthetes and nonsynesthetes. In Klinger, E. (Ed.), Imagery: concepts, results, and applications (vol. II, pp. 153–63). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Rader, Ch. M., and Tellegen, A. (1987). An investigation of synesthesia. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 981–7.Google Scholar
Radvansky, G. A., Gibson, B. S., and McNerney, M. W. (2011). Synesthesia and memory: color congruency, von Restorff, and false memory effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 219–29.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., and Brang, D. (2008). Tactile-emotion synesthesia. Neurocase, 14, 390–9.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., and Hubbard, E. M. (2001a). Psychophysical investigations into the neural basis of synaesthesia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 268, 979–83.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., and Hubbard, E. M. (2001b). Synaesthesia – a window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 334.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., and Rogers-Ramachandran, D. (1996). Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 263, 377–86.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., and Rogers-Ramachandran, D. (2000). Phantom limbs and neural plasticity. Archives of Neurology, 57, 317–20.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., Rogers-Ramachandran, D., and Cobb, S. (1995). Touching the phantom limb. Nature, 377, 489–90.Google Scholar
Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., and Frith, U. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841–65.Google Scholar
Rao, A., Nobre, A. C., Alexander, I., and Ceowy, A. (2007). Auditory evoked visual awareness following sudden ocular blindness: an EEG and TMS investigation. Experimental Brain Research, 176, 288–98.Google Scholar
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., and Court, J. H. (1998). Raven manual: Section 3. Standard Progressive Matrices (this edition introduces the Parallel and Plus versions). Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Reeves, A., Fuller, H., and Fine, E. M. (2005). The role of attention in binding shape to color. Vision Research, 45, 3343–55.Google Scholar
Rich, A. N., Bradshaw, J. L., and Mattingley, J. B. (2005). A systematic, large-scale study of synaesthesia: implications for the role of early experience in lexical-colour associations. Cognition, 98, 5384.Google Scholar
Rich, A. N., and Mattingley, J. B. (2002). Anomalous perception in synaesthesia: a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 4352.Google Scholar
Rich, A. N., and Mattingley, J. B. (2005). Can attention modulate colour–graphemic synaesthesia? In Robertson, L. C. and Sagiv, N. (Eds.), Synaesthesia: perspectives from cognitive neuroscience (pp. 108–23). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rich, A. N., and Mattingley, J. B. (2010). Out of sight, out of mind: the attentional blink can eliminate synaesthetic colours. Cognition, 114, 320–8.Google Scholar
Rich, A. N., Williams, M. A., Puce, A., Syngeniotis, A., Howard, M. A., McGlone, F., and Mattingley, J. B. (2006). Neural correlates of imagined and synaesthetic colours. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2918–25.Google Scholar
Rizzo, M., and Eslinger, P. I. (1989). Colored hearing synesthesia: an investigation of neural factors. Neurology, 39, 781–4.Google Scholar
Ro, T., Farne, A., Johnson, R. M., Wedeen, V., Chu, Z., Wang, Z. J., and Beauchamp, M. S. (2007). Feeling sounds after a thalamic lesion. Annals of Neurology, 62, 433–41.Google Scholar
Robertson, L. C. (2003). Binding, spatial attention and perceptual awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 93102.Google Scholar
Roche, S. M., and McConkey, K. M. (1990). Absorption: nature, assessment, and correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 91101.Google Scholar
Rogowska, A. (2007). Synestezja [Synaesthesia]. Opole: Oficyna Wydawnicza PO.Google Scholar
Rogowska, A. (2011). Categorization of synaesthesia. Review of General Psychology, 15, 213–27.Google Scholar
Rollins, M. (2001). The strategic eye: Kosslyn’s theory of imagery and perception. Minds and Machines, 11, 267–86.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., and Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) and Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: social, emotional and personality development (5th edn., vol. III, pp. 105–76). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rothen, N., and Meier, B. (2009). Do synesthetes have a general advantage in visual search and episodic memory? A case for group studies. PLoS ONE, 4, e5037.Google Scholar
Rothen, N., and Meier, B. (2010a). Grapheme-colour synesthesia yields an ordinary rather than extraordinary memory advantage: evidence from a group study. Memory, 18, 258–64.Google Scholar
Rothen, N., and Meier, B. (2010b). Higher prevalence of synaesthesia in art students. Perception, 39, 718–20.Google Scholar
Rothen, N., Meier, B., and Ward, J. (2012). Enhanced memory ability: insights from synaesthesia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 1952–63.Google Scholar
Rothen, N., Nyffeler, T., von Wartburg, R., Müri, R., and Meier, B. (2010). Parieto-occipital suppression eliminates implicit bidirectionality in grapheme-colour synaesthesia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3482–7.Google Scholar
Rouw, R., and Scholte, H. S. (2007). Increased structural connectivity in grapheme-color synesthesia. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 792–7.Google Scholar
Rouw, R., and Scholte, H. S. (2010). Neural basis of individual differences in synesthetic experiences. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 6205–13.Google Scholar
Rouw, R., Scholte, H. S., and Colizoli, O. (2011). Brain areas involved in synaesthesia: a review. Journal of Neuropsychology, 5, 214–42.Google Scholar
Royce, J. R., and Diamond, S. R. (1980). A multifactor-system dynamics theory of emotion: cognitive-affective interaction. Edmonton: Center for Advanced Study in Theoretical Psychology, University of Alberta.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2007). Creativity: theories and themes: research, development, and practice. Burlington, MA: Elsevier Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rushton, J. P. (1985). Differential K theory: the sociobiology of individual and group differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 441–52.Google Scholar
Rushton, J. P. (1990). Sir Francis Galton, epigenetic rules, genetic similarity theory, and human life history analysis. Journal of Personality, 58, 117–40.Google Scholar
Rushton, J. P., and Irwing, P. (2011). The general factor of personality: normal and abnormal. In Chamorro-Premuzic, T., von Stumm, S., and Furnham, A. (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of individual differences (pp. 134–63). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Sacks, O., Wasserman, R. L., Zeki, S., and Siegel, R. M. (1988). Sudden colour blindness of cerebral origin. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 14, 1251.Google Scholar
Sagiv, N., Heer, J., and Robertson, L. (2006). Does binding of synaesthetic color to the evoking grapheme require attention? Cortex, 42, 232–42.Google Scholar
Sagiv, N., and Robertson, L. C. (2005). Synaesthesia and the binding problem. In Robertson, L. C. and Sagiv, N. (Eds.), Synaesthesia: perspectives from cognitive neuroscience (pp. 90107). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Schaefer, M., Noennig, N., Heinze, H. J., and Rotte, M. (2006). Fooling your feelings: artificially induced referred sensations are linked to a modulation of the primary somatosensory cortex. NeuroImage, 29, 6773.Google Scholar
Schlegel, K., Grandjean, D., and Scherer, K. R. (2012). Emotion recognition: unidimensional ability or a set of modality- and emotion-specific skills? Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 1621.Google Scholar
Schmid, J., and Leiman, J. M. (1957). The development of hierarchical factor solutions. Psychometrika, 22, 8390.Google Scholar
Schwenzer, M., and Mathiak, K. (2012). The correlation of inductive reasoning with multi-dimensional perception. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 903–7.Google Scholar
Seitz, J. A. (2005). The neural, evolutionary, developmental, and bodily basis of metaphor. New Ideas in Psychology, 23, 7495.Google Scholar
Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarksi, P., Constable, T., Marchione, K. E., Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., and Gore, J. C. (2002). Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 101–10.Google Scholar
Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Pugh, K. R., Fulbright, R. K., Constable, R. T., Mencl, W. E., Shankweiler, D. P., Liberman, A. M., Skudlarski, P., Fletcher, J. M., Katz, L., Marchione, K. E., Lacadie, C., Gatenby, C., and Gore, J. C. (1998). Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 2636–41.Google Scholar
Simner, J. (2007). Beyond perception: synaesthesia as a psycholinguistic phenomenon. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 23–9.Google Scholar
Simner, J. (2012a). Defining synaesthesia. British Journal of Psychology, 103, 115.Google Scholar
Simner, J. (2012b). Defining synaesthesia: a response to two excellent commentaries. British Journal of Psychology, 103, 24–7.Google Scholar
Simner, J., Gärtner, O., and Taylor, M. D. (2011). Cross-modal personality attributions in synaesthetes and non-synaesthetes. Journal of Neuropsychology, 5, 283301.Google Scholar
Simner, J., Glover, L., and Mowat, A. (2006). Linguistic determinants of word colouring in grapheme-colour synaesthesia. Cortex, 42, 281–9.Google Scholar
Simner, J., Harrold, J., Creed, H., Monro, L., and Foulkes, L. (2009). Early detection of markers for synaesthesia in childhood populations. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 132, 5764.Google Scholar
Simner, J., and Holenstein, E. (2007). Ordinal linguistic personification as a variant of synesthesia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 694703.Google Scholar
Simner, J., and Hubbard, E. M. (2006). Variants of synesthesia interact in cognitive tasks: evidence for implicit associations and late connectivity in cross-talk theories. Neuroscience, 143, 805–14.Google Scholar
Simner, J., Mayo, N., and Spiller, M. J. (2009). A foundation for savantism? Visuo-spatial synaesthetes present with cognitive benefits. Cortex, 45, 1246–60.Google Scholar
Simner, J., Mulvenna, C., Sagiv, N., Tsakanikos, E., Witherby, S. A., Fraser, C., Scott, K., and Ward, J. (2006). Synaesthesia: the prevalence of atypical cross-modal experiences. Perception, 35, 1024–33.Google Scholar
Simner, J., Ward, J., Lanz, M., Jansari, A., Noonan, K., Glover, L., and Oakley, D. A. (2005). Non-random associations of graphemes to colours in synaesthetic and non-synaesthetic populations. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 1069–85.Google Scholar
Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J. M., Bergman, E., and Papanicolaou, A. C. (2000). Cerebral mechanisms involved in word reading in dyslexic children: a magnetic source imaging approach. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 809–16.Google Scholar
Sinke, C., Halpern, J. H., Zedler, M., Neufeld, J., Emrich, H. M., and Passie, T. (2012). Genuine and drug-induced synesthesia: a comparison. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 1419–34.Google Scholar
Siuta, J. (1987). Zastosowanie Inwentarza Wyobraźni T. X. Barbera i S. C. Wilson w badaniach nad hipnozą [Application of the Imagination Inventory in T. X. Barber and S. C. Wilson’s research on hypnosis]. Przegląd Psychologiczny, 1, 245–57.Google Scholar
Siuta, J. (1996). The Imagination Inventory and its correlates with imagery and hypnotizability. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 39, 115–25.Google Scholar
Skelton, R., Ludwig, C., and Mohr, C. (2009). A novel, illustrated questionnaire to distinguish projector and associator synaesthetes. Cortex, 45, 721–9.Google Scholar
Smilek, D., Callejas, A., Dixon, M. J., and Merikle, P. M. (2007). Ovals of time: time-space associations in synaesthesia. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 507–19.Google Scholar
Smilek, D., and Dixon, M. J. (2002). Towards a synergistic understanding of synaesthesia: combining current experimental findings with synaesthetes’ subjective descriptions. Psyche, 8, Article 01 (www.theassc.org/files/assc/2529.pdf).Google Scholar
Smilek, D., Dixon, M. J., Cudahy, C., and Merikle, P. M. (2001). Synaesthetic photisms influence visual perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 930–6.Google Scholar
Smilek, D., Dixon, M. J., Cudahy, C., and Merikle, P. M. (2002). Synesthetic color experiences influence memory. Psychological Science, 13, 548–52.Google Scholar
Smilek, D., Dixon, M. J., and Merikle, P. M. (2003). Synaesthetic photisms guide attention. Brain and Cognition, 53, 364–7.Google Scholar
Smilek, D., Dixon, M. J., and Merikle, P. M. (2005). Synaesthesia: discordant male monozygotic twins. Neurocase, 11, 363–70.Google Scholar
Smilek, D., Dixon, M. J., and Merikle, P. M. (2006). Revisiting the category effect: the influence of meaning and search strategy on the efficiency of visual search. Brain Research, 1080, 7390.Google Scholar
Smilek, D., Moffatt, B. A., Pasternak, J., White, B. N., Dixon, M. J., and Merikle, P. M. (2002). Synaesthesia: a case study of discordant monozygotic twins. Neurocase, 8, 338–42.Google Scholar
Smith, G. J. W. (1999). Trait and process in personality theory: defined within two contemporary research traditions. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40, 269–76.Google Scholar
Sneath, P. H. A., and Sokal, R. R. (1973). Numerical taxonomy: the principles and practice of numerical classification. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Spalding, J. M. K., and Zangwill, O. L. (1950). Disturbance of number-form in a case of brain injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 13, 24–9.Google Scholar
Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: a tutorial review. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 971–95.Google Scholar
Sperling, J. M., Prvulovic, D., Linden, D. E. J., Singer, W., and Stirn, A. (2006). Neuronal correlatates of colour-graphemic synaesthesia: an fMRI study. Cortex, 42, 295303.Google Scholar
Spiller, M. J., and Jansari, A. S. (2008). Mental imagery and synaesthesia: is synaesthesia from internally-generated stimuli possible? Cognition, 109, 143–51.Google Scholar
Springer, S. P., and Deutsch, G. (1997). Left brain, right brain: perspectives from cognitive neuroscience (5th edn.). New York: Worth Publishers.Google Scholar
Steen, C. (2001). Visions shared: a firsthand look into synesthesia and art. Leonardo, 34, 203–8.Google Scholar
Stefanics, G., Fosker, T., Huss, M., Mead, N., Szucs, D., and Goswami, U. (2011). Auditory sensory deficits in developmental dyslexia: a longitudinal ERP study. NeuroImage, 57, 723–32.Google Scholar
Strelau, J. (1983). Temperament-personality-activity. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Strelau, J. (1987). The concept of temperament in personality research. European Journal of Personality, 1, 107–17.Google Scholar
Strelau, J. (1996). The regulative theory of temperament: current status. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 131–42.Google Scholar
Strelau, J. (1998). Temperament: a psychological perspective. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Strelau, J., and Angleitner, A. (1994). Cross-cultural studies on temperament: theoretical considerations and empirical studies based on the Pavlovian Temperament Survey (PTS). Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 331–42.Google Scholar
Strelau, J., Angleitner, A., and Newberry, B. H. (1999). The Pavlovian Temperament Survey (PTS): an international handbook. Goettingen: Hogrefe and Huber.Google Scholar
Strelau, J., and Zawadzki, B. (1993). The Formal Characteristics of Behavior-Temperament Inventory (FCB-TI): theoretical assumptions and scale construction. European Journal of Personality, 7, 313–36.Google Scholar
Strelau, J., and Zawadzki, B. (1995). The Formal Characteristics of Behaviour – Temperament Inventory (FCB-TI): validity studies. European Journal of Personality, 9, 207–29.Google Scholar
Strelau, J., and Zawadzki, B. (1998). Kwestionariusz temperamentu PTS. Podręcznik [Temperament Questionnaire PTS. Manual]. Warszawa: Pracowania Testów Psychologicznych PTP.Google Scholar
Strelau, J., Zawadzki, B., and Angleitner, A. (1995). Kwestionariusz Temperamentu PTS: próba psychologicznej interpretacji podstawowych cech układu nerwowego według Pawłowa [The Pavlovian Temperament Survey (PTS): an attempt at a psychological interpretation of the Pavlovian dimensions of basic nervous system properties]. Studia Psychologiczne, 33, 948.Google Scholar
Studerus, E., Gamma, A., Kometer, M., and Vollenweider, F. X. (2012). Prediction of psilocybin response in healthy volunteers. PLoS ONE 7(2), e30800.Google Scholar
Studerus, E., Gamma, A., and Vollenweider, F. X. (2010). Psychometric evaluation of the Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale (OAV). PLoS ONE, 5(8), 119, e12412.Google Scholar
Swenson, R. S. (2006). Review of clinical and functional neuroscience. In Holmes, G. L., Levin, M., and Cohen, J. A. (Eds.), Educational review manual in neurology. New York: Castle Connolly Graduate Medical Publishing (www.dartmouth.edu/~rswenson/NeuroSci/).Google Scholar
Tammet, D. (2006). Born on a blue day: the gift of an extraordinary mind. London: Hodder and Stoughton.Google Scholar
Tellegen, A. (1982). Manual for the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript. Minneapolis, University of Minnesota.Google Scholar
Tellegen, A., and Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (‘absorption’), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 268–77.Google Scholar
Terhune, D. B. (2009). The incidence and determinants of visual phenomenology during out-of-body experiences. Cortex, 45, 236–42.Google Scholar
Terhune, D. B., Cardeña, E., and Lindgren, M. (2010). Disruption of synaesthesia by posthypnotic suggestion: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3360–4.Google Scholar
Terhune, D. B., Tai, S., Cowey, A., Popescu, T, and Cohen Kadosh, R. (2011). Enhanced cortical excitability in grapheme-color synesthesia and its modulation. Current Biology, 21, 2006–9.Google Scholar
Thalbourne, M. A., Houran, J., Alias, A. G., and Grugger, P. (2001). Transliminality, brain function, and synesthesia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189, 190–2.Google Scholar
Thingujam, N. S., Laukka, P., and Elfenbein, H. A. (2012). Distinct emotional abilities converge: evidence from emotional understanding and emotion recognition through the voice. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 350–4.Google Scholar
Thomas, A., and Chess, S. (1985). The behavioral study of temperament. In Strelau, J., Farley, F. H., and Gale, A. (Eds.), The biological bases of personality and behavior: theories, measurement techniques, and development (pp. 213–25). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Tomson, S. N., Avidan, N., Lee, K., Sarma, A. K., Tushe, R., Milewicz, D. M., Bray, M., Leal, S. M., and Eagleman, D. M. (2011). The genetics of colored sequence synesthesia: suggestive evidence of linkage to 16q and genetic heterogeneity for the condition. Behavioural Brain Research, 223, 4852.Google Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1966). The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking – Norms-Technical Manual Research Edition – Verbal Tests, Forms A and B – Figural Tests, Forms A and B. Princeton, NJ: Personnel Press.Google Scholar
Toyota, H. (2011). Individual differences in emotional intelligence and incidental memory of words. Japanese Psychological Research, 53, 213–20.Google Scholar
Treisman, A., and Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97136.Google Scholar
Urban, K. K. (1996). Test for Creative Thinking – Drawing Production (TCT-DP) by K. K. Urban and H. G. Jellen. Manual. Frankfurt: Swets and Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Urban, K. K. (2005). Assessing creativity: the Test for Creative Thinking – Drawing Production (TCT-DP). International Education Journal, 6, 272–80.Google Scholar
van Campen, C. (2008). The hidden sense: synesthesia in art and science. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
van Campen, C., and Froger, C. (1999). Artistic and psychological experiments with synesthesia. Leonardo, 32, 914.Google Scholar
van der Ven, A. H. G. S., and Ellis, J. L. (2000). A Rasch analysis of Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 4564.Google Scholar
van de Ven, V., and Merckelbach, H. (2003). The role of schizotypy, mental imagery, and fantasy proneness in hallucinatory reports of undergraduate students. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 889–96.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, T. M., den Ouden, H. E. M., and Hagoort, P. (2011). Effective connectivity determines the nature of subjective experience in grapheme-color synesthesia. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 9879–84.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, T. M., Petersson, K. M., and Hagoort, P. (2010). Synaesthetic colour in the brain: beyond colour areas. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of synaesthetes and matched controls. PLoS ONE, 5(8), e12074.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, T. M., van den Peper, J. S., Berg, S. M., Brouwer, R. M., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Kahn, R. S., and Boomsma, D. I. (2009). A genetic analysis of brain volumes and IQ in children. Intelligence, 37, 181–91.Google Scholar
van Prooijen, J.-W., and van der Kloot, W. A. (2001). Confirmatory analysis of exploratively obtained factor structures. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 61, 777–92.Google Scholar
Villemure, C., Wassimi, S., Bennett, G. J., Shir, Y., and Bushnell, M. C. (2006). Unpleasant odors increase pain processing in a patient with neuropathic pain: psychophysical and fMRI investigation. Pain, 120, 213–20.Google Scholar
Voyer, D. (1996). On the magnitude of laterality effects and sex differences in functional lateralities. Laterality, 1, 5183.Google Scholar
Waldo, T. G., and Merritt, R. D. (2000). Fantasy proneness, dissociation, and DSM–IV Axis II symptomatology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 355–8.Google Scholar
Ward, J. (2004). Emotionally mediated synaesthesia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 761–72.Google Scholar
Ward, J. (2007). Acquired auditory-tactile synesthesia. Annals of Neurology, 62, 429–30.Google Scholar
Ward, J., Huckstep, B., and Tsakanikos, E. (2006). Sound-colour synaesthesia: to what extent does it use cross-modal mechanisms common to us all? Cortex, 42, 264–80.Google Scholar
Ward, J., Jonas, C., Dienes, Z., and Seth, A. (2010). Grapheme-colour synaesthesia improves detection of embedded shapes but without pre-attentive ‘pop-out’ of synaesthetic colour. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Section B: Biological Sciences, 277, 1021–6.Google Scholar
Ward, J., Li, R., Salih, S., and Sagiv, N. (2007). Varieties of grapheme-colour synaesthesia: a new theory of phenomenological and behavioural differences. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 913–31.Google Scholar
Ward, J., and Simner, J. (2003). Lexical-gustatory synaesthesia: linguistic and conceptual factors. Cognition, 89, 237–61.Google Scholar
Ward, J., Simner, J., and Auyeung, V. (2005). A comparison of lexical-gustatory and grapheme-colour synaesthesia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 2841.Google Scholar
Ward, J., Thompson-Lake, D., Ely, R., and Kaminski, F. (2008). Synaesthesia, creativity and art: what is the link? British Journal of Psychology, 99, 127–41.Google Scholar
Ward, J., Tsakanikos, E., and Bray, A. (2006). Synaesthesia for reading and playing musical notes. Neurocase, 12, 2734.Google Scholar
Watson, M. R., Akins, K. A., and Enns, J. T. (2012). Second-order mappings in grapheme–color synesthesia. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19, 211–17.Google Scholar
Weiss, P. H., and Fink, G. R. (2009). Grapheme-colour synaesthetes show increased grey matter volumes of parietal and fusiform cortex. Brain, 132, 6570.Google Scholar
Weiss, P. H., Shah, N. J., Toni, I., Zilles, K., and Fink, G. R. (2001). Associating colours with people: a case of chromatic-lexical synaesthesia. Cortex, 37, 750–3.Google Scholar
Wherry, R. J. (1959). Hierarchical factor solutions without rotation. Psychometrika, 24, 4551.Google Scholar
Wicker, F. W., and Holahan, C. K. (1978). Analogy training and synesthetic phenomena. Journal of General Psychology, 98, 113–22.Google Scholar
Wild, T. C., Kuiken, D., and Schopflocher, D. (1995). The role of absorption in experiential involvement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 569679.Google Scholar
Wilson, S. C., and Barber, T. X. (1978). The creative imagination scale as a measure of hypnotic responsiveness: applications to experimental and clinical hypnosis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 20, 235–49.Google Scholar
Wilson, S. C., and Barber, T. X. (1981). Vivid fantasy and hallucinatory abilities in the life histories of excellent hypnotic subjects (‘somnambules’): preliminary report with female subjects. In Klinger, E. (Ed.), Imagery: concepts, results, and applications (vol. II, pp. 133–49). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Wilson, S. C., and Barber, T. X. (1983a). Inventory of Childhood Memories and Imaginings. Framingham, MA: Cushing Hospital.Google Scholar
Wilson, S. C., and Barber, T. X. (1983b). The fantasy-prone personality: implications for understanding imagery, hypnosis, and parapsychological phenomena. In Sheikh, A. A. (Ed.), Imagery: current theory, research and application (pp. 340–90). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Witkin, H. A., Dyk, R. B., Faterson, H. F., Goodenough, D. R., and Karp, S. (1962). Psychological differentiation: studies of development. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Witten, I. H., Frank, E., and Hall, M. A. (2011). Data mining: practical machine learning tools and techniques (3rd edn.). Burlington, MA: Morgan Kaufmann Publishers.Google Scholar
Witthoft, N., and Winawer, J. (2006). Synesthetic colors determined by having colored refrigerator magnets in childhood. Cortex, 42, 175–83.Google Scholar
Witthoft, N., and Winawer, J. (2013). Learning, memory, and synesthesia. Psychological Science, 24(3), 258–65.Google Scholar
Wold, H. (1966). Estimation of principal components and related models by iterative least squares. In Krishnaiah, P. R. (Ed.), Multivariate analysis (pp. 391420). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Yaro, C., and Ward, J. (2007). Searching for Shereshevskii: what is superior about the memory of synaesthetes? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 681–95.Google Scholar
Zawadzki, B., and Strelau, J. (1997) Formalna charakterystyka zachowania–kwestionariusz temperamentu (FCZ–KT). Podręcznik [Formal Characteristics of Behavior–Temperament Inventory (FCB–TI). Manual]. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.Google Scholar
Zawadzki, B., and Strelau, J. (2010). Structure of personality: search for a general factor viewed from a temperament perspective. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 7782.Google Scholar
Zawadzki, B., Strelau, J., Szczepaniak, P., and Śliwińska, M. (1998). Inwentarz osobowości NEO-FFI Costy i McCrae: adaptacja polska. Podręcznik [Personality inventory NEO-FFI by Costa and McCrae: Polish adaptation. Manual]. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.Google Scholar
Zingrone, N. L., Alvarado, C. S., and Agee, N. (2009). Psychological correlates of aura vision: psychic experiences, dissociation, absorption, and synaesthesia-like experiences. Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 37, 131–68.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: beyond the optimal level of arousal. London: Wiley.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Aleksandra Maria Rogowska, Politechnika Opolska
  • Book: Synaesthesia and Individual Differences
  • Online publication: 05 April 2015
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316156230.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Aleksandra Maria Rogowska, Politechnika Opolska
  • Book: Synaesthesia and Individual Differences
  • Online publication: 05 April 2015
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316156230.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Aleksandra Maria Rogowska, Politechnika Opolska
  • Book: Synaesthesia and Individual Differences
  • Online publication: 05 April 2015
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316156230.010
Available formats
×