Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-5nwft Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T16:58:32.933Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 15 - Religion and Frontotemporal Dementia

from II.I - Clinical Conditions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 October 2019

Alasdair Coles
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Joanna Collicutt
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Get access

Summary

In the setting of disease, religion can play a positive role in a person’s life, helping them manage their perception of an illness, bolstering the ability of the patient and their family to manage the diagnosis, day-to-day functioning and treatment [1]. Individuals who have practised religion earlier in life may show new vigour in their belief after diagnosis of a serious illness, or religion can be discovered for the first time. Increased religiosity can be a means for support and comfort through difficult periods in a person’s life.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Jaynes, J. 1976. The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Park, C. L. 2005. Religion as a meaning making framework in coping with life stress. Journal of Social Issues 61: 707–29.Google Scholar
Geschwind, N. 1983. Pathogenesis of behavior change in temporal lobe epilepsy. Research Publications – Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disorders 61: 355–70.Google ScholarPubMed
Bear, D. M., and Fedio, P.. 1977. Quantitative analysis of interictal behavior in temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives in Neurology 34: 454–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devinsky, O., and Lai, G.. 2008. Spirituality and religion in epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior 12: 636–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trimble, M., and Freeman, A.. 2006. An investigation of religiosity and the Gastaut-Geschwind syndrome in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behavior 9: 407–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards-Lee, T., Miller, B. L., Benson, D. F., Cummings, J. L., Russell, G. L., Boone, K. et al. 1997. The temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 120: 1027–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, B. L., Seeley, W. W., Mychack, P., Rosen, H. J., Mena, I. and Boone, K.. 2001. Neuroanatomy of the self: evidence from patients with frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 57: 817–21. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11552010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, T. Q., Miller, Z. A., Adhimoolam, B., Zackey, D. D., Khan, B. K., Ketelle, R. et al. 2013. Verbal creativity in semantic variant primary progressive aphasia. Neurocase, 12 December. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24329034.Google Scholar
Block, N. R., Perry, D. C., Sturm, V. E., Miller, Z. A. and Miller, B. L.. 2013. Religious Dogmatism in Neurodegenerative Disease. Boston: Alzheimer’s Association International Conference.Google Scholar
Kapogiannis, D., Barbey, A. K., Su, M., Krueger, F. and Grafman, J.. 2009. Neuroanatomical variability of religiosity. PLoS ONE 4(9): e7180.Google Scholar
Han, S., Mao, L., Gu, X., Zhu, Y., Ge, J. and Ma, Y.. 2008. Neural consequences of religious belief on self-referential processing. Social Neuroscience 3: 115. doi:10.1080/17470910701469681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M, Bermpohl, F, Dobrowolny, H and Panksepp, J.. 2006. Self-referential processing in our brain – a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 31: 440–57.Google Scholar
Northoff, G., and Bermpohl, F.. 2004. Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 102–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grèzes, J., Frith, C. D. and Passingham, R. E.. 2004. Inferring false beliefs from the actions of oneself and others: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 21: 744–50. www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811903006657.Google Scholar
Azari, N., Nickel, J., Wunderlich, G, Niedeggen, M, Hefter, H, Tellmann, L et al. 2001. Neural correlates of religious experience. European Journal 13: 1649–52. http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01527.x/full.Google Scholar
Newberg, A., Pourdehnad, M., Alavi, A and d’Aquili, E. G.. 2003. Cerebral blood flow during meditative prayer: preliminary findings and methodological issues. Perceptual and Motor Skills 97: 625–30. www.amsciepub.com/doi/abs/10.2466/pms.2003.97.2.625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newberg, A., Alavi, A., Baime, M, Pourdehnad, M, Santanna, J and d’Aquili, E.. 2001. The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during the complex cognitive task of meditation: a preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 106: 113–22. www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0925492701000749.Google ScholarPubMed
Beauregard, M., and Paquette, V.. 2006. Neural correlates of a mystical experience in Carmelite nuns. Neuroscience Letters 405: 186–90. www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0304394006006392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, A. D., Hayward, R. D., Koenig, H. G, Steffens, D. C and Payne, M. E.. 2011. Religious factors and hippocampal atrophy in late life. PLoS One 6, no. 3: e17006. www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3068149.Google Scholar
Benson, D. F. 1991. The Geschwind syndrome. Advances in Neurology 55: 411–21.Google ScholarPubMed
Dewhurst, K., and Beard, A. W.. 2003. Sudden religious conversions in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior 4: 7887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Postiglione, A., Milan, G., Pappatà, S, De Falco, C, Lamenza, F, Schiattarella, V, Gallotta, G, Sorrentino, P and Striano, S.. 2008. Fronto-temporal dementia presenting as Geschwind’s syndrome. Neurocase 14: 264–70. doi:10.1080/13554790802269976.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazzarella, E., Ramsey, R., Conson, M and Hamilton, A.. 2013. Brain systems for visual perspective taking and action perception. Social Neuroscience 8: 248–67. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23350907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Veluw, S. J., and Chance, S. A.. 2014. Differentiating between self and others: an ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind. Brain Imaging and Behavior 8: 2438. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24535033.Google Scholar
Irish, M., Piguet, O. and Hodges, J. R.. 2011. Self-projection and the default network in frontotemporal dementia. Nature Reviews Neurology 8: 152–61. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22331029.Google Scholar
Call, J., and Tomasello, M.. 2008. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Science 12: 187–92. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18424224.Google Scholar
Frith, C. 1992. The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Ed. Erlbaum, L.. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Dein, S., and Littlewood, R.. 2011. Religion and psychosis: a common evolutionary trajectory? Transcultural Psychiatry 48: 318–35. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21742955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×