Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-v5vhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-30T07:02:51.515Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Full Transfer in L3/Ln Acquisition

Evidence from Two Clusters of Studies

from Part II - L3/Ln across Linguistic Domains

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 July 2023

Jennifer Cabrelli
Affiliation:
University of Illinois, Chicago
Adel Chaouch-Orozco
Affiliation:
The Hong Kong Polytechnic University
Jorge González Alonso
Affiliation:
Universidad Nebrija, Spain and UiT, Arctic University of Norway
Sergio Miguel Pereira Soares
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
Eloi Puig-Mayenco
Affiliation:
King's College London
Jason Rothman
Affiliation:
UiT, Arctic University of Norway and Universidad Nebrija, Spain
Get access

Summary

The aim of the chapter is to first discuss the theoretical motivations for Full Transfer as well as some methodological considerations one needs to adopt when testing whether there is Full or property-by-property transfer in L3/Ln acquisition. The chapter then provides an overview of two clusters of studies providing some empirical evidence towards Full Transfer at the initial stages of L3/Ln acquisition. The first cluster of studies examined is that of the acquisition of Brazilian Portuguese by bilingual English–Spanish speakers, illustrating how bringing together these 16 studies can be helpful when evaluating the tenets of the theoretical proposals that predict Full Transfer. The second is that of Catalan–Spanish bilinguals acquiring L3 English. By reviewing these seven different studies, it is shown how Full Transfer applies in cases where structural similarity between the Lx/Ly and L3 is not straightforward. The chapter ends by suggesting some directions for future research.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abrahamsson, N., & Hyltenstam, K. (2009). Age of Onset and Nativelikeness in a Second Language: Listener Perception Versus Linguistic Scrutiny. Language Learning, 59(2), 249306.Google Scholar
Angelovska, T., & Hahn, A. (Eds.) (2017). L3 Syntactic Transfer: Models, New Developments and Implications. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bardel, C., & Falk, Y. (2007). The Role of the Second Language in Third Language Acquisition: The Case of Germanic Syntax. Second Language Research, 23(4), 459484.Google Scholar
Bardel, C., & Falk, Y. (2012). Behind the L2 Status Factor: A Neurolinguistic Framework for L3 Research. In Cabrelli Amaro, J., Flynn, S., & Rothman, J. (Eds.), Third Language Acquisition in Adulthood (pp. 6178). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bardel, C., & Sánchez, L. (2017). The L2 Status Factor Hypothesis Revisited: The Role of Metalinguistic Knowledge, Working Memory, Attention and Noticing in Third Language Learning. In Angelovska, T & Hahn, A. (Eds.), L3 Syntactic Transfer: Models, New Developments and Implications (pp. 85102). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bayona, P. (2009). The Acquisition of Spanish Middle and Impersonal Passive Constructions from SLA and TLA Perspectives. In Leung, Y. I. (Ed.), Third Language Acquisition and Universal Grammar (pp. 129). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Berkes, É., & Flynn, S. (2012). Further Evidence in Support of the Cumulative-Enhancement Model. In Cabrelli Amaro, J., Flynn, S., & Rothman, J. (Eds.), Third Language Acquisition in Adulthood (pp. 143164). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1979). Explicit and Implicit Judgments of L2 Grammaticality. Language Learning, 29(1), 81103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1982). On the Relationship between Knowing and Using Linguistic Forms. Applied Linguistics, 3(3), 181206.Google Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R. (1989). What Is the Logical Problem of Foreign Language Learning? In Gass, S. & Schachter, J. (Eds.), Linguistic Perspectives on Second Language Acquisition (pp. 4168). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R. (2009). The Evolving Context of the Fundamental Difference Hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 31(2), 175198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borg, K. (2013). The Acquisition of Future of Probability in L3 Spanish. In Cabrelli Amaro, J., Judy, T., & Pascual y Cabo, D. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 12th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition (GASLA, 2003) (pp. 1121). Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Bowles, M. (2011). Meaning Implicit and Explicit Linguistic Knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 247271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruhn De Garavito, J., & Perpiñán, S. (2014). Subject Pronouns and Clitics in the Spanish Interlanguage of French L1 Speakers. In Teddiman, L. (Ed.), Canadian Linguistic Association Conference Proceedings (pp. 111). Ottawa: Canadian Linguistic Association.Google Scholar
Bylund, E., Abrahamsson, N., & Hyltenstam, K. (2012). Does First Language Maintenance Hamper Nativelikeness in a Second Language? A Study of Ultimate Attainment in Early Bilinguals. In Abrahamsson, N. & Hyltenstam, K. (Eds.), High-Level L2 Acquisition, Learning and Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2013a). Raising across Experiencers in L3 Portuguese: Further Evidence for Psychotypological Transfer. In Stavrakaki, S., Konstantinopoulou, P., & Lalioti, M. (Eds.), Advances in Language Acquisition (pp. 272281). Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Press.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2013b). The Phonological Permeability Hypothesis: Measuring Regressive L3 Influence to Test L2 Phonological Representations. Doctoral dissertation. University of Florida.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2017a). The Impact of Portuguese on the Study of Third Language Acquisition. Hispania, 100(5), 5562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2017b). Testing the Phonological Permeability Hypothesis: L3 Phonological Effects on L1 versus L2 Systems. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 698717.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J., & Pichan, C. (2021). Initial Phonological Transfer in L3 Brazilian Portuguese and Italian. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11(2), 131167.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J., Amaro, F. & Rothman, J. (2015). The Relationship between L3 Transfer and Structural Similarity across Development. In Peukert, H. (Ed.), Transfer Effects in Multilingual Language Development (pp. 2152). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cabrelli, J., & Iverson, M. (2023). Why do Learners Overcome Non-facilitative Transfer Faster from an L2 than an L1? The Cumulative Input Threshold Hypothesis. International Journal of Multilingualism.Google Scholar
Cabrelli, J., & Puig-Mayenco, E. (2020). Theoretical and Methodological Considerations for a Robust Evaluation of the Linguistic Proximity Model. Second Language Research, 37(3), 465471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cabrelli, J., Iverson, M., Giancaspro, D., & Halloran González, B. (2020). Implications of L1 versus L2 Transfer in L3 Rate of Morphosyntactic Acquisition. In Molsing, K., Becker Lopes Perna, C., & Tramunt Ibaños, A. M. (Eds.), Linguistic Approaches to Portuguese as an Additional Language (pp. 1133). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Carvalho, A. M., & Da Silva, J. B. (2006). Cross-Linguistic Influence in Third Language Acquisition: The Case of Spanish–English Bilinguals’ Acquisition of Portuguese. Foreign Language Annals, 39(2), 185202.Google Scholar
Cenoz, J., Hufeisen, B., & Jessner, U. (2001). Cross-Linguistic Influence in Third Language Acquisition: Psycholinguistics Perspectives. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Child, M. W. (2017). The Typological Primacy Model and Bilingual Types: Transfer Differences between Spanish/English Bilinguals in L3 Portuguese Acquisition. In Bellamy, K., Child, M. W., González, P., Muntendam, A., & Parfita Couto, M. C. (Eds.), Multidisciplinary Approaches to Bilingualism in the Hispanic and Lusophone World (pp. 4164). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Child, M. W. (2019). Transfer in L3 Cognate Language Acquisition: The Role of Language Background on Instructed L3 Portuguese Acquisition. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 60(2), 143168.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. H., & Felser, C. (2006). Grammatical Processing in Language Learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(1), 342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Angelis, G. (2007). Third or Additional Language Acquisition. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
De Angelis, G., & Dewaele, J.-M. (Eds.) (2011). New Trends in Crosslinguistic Influence and Multilingualism Research. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2000). The Robustness of Critical Period Effects in Second Language Acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22(4), 499533.Google Scholar
Dussias, P. (2004). Parsing a First Language Like a Second: The Erosion of L1 Parsing Strategies in Spanish–English Bilinguals. International Journal of BIlingualism, 8(3), 355371.Google Scholar
Dussias, P. E. (2003). Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution in L2 Learners: Some Effects of Bilinguality on L1 and L2 Processing Strategies. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 529557.Google Scholar
Efeoglu, G., & Schroeder, C. (2021). Acquisition of Object Pronouns in EFL in Germany by Heritage Speakers of Turkish. Sustainable Multilingualism, 18(1), 7184.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (2005). Measuring Implicit and Explicit Knowledge of a Second Language: A Psychometric Study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 111139.Google Scholar
Falk, Y., & Bardel, C. (2011). Object Pronouns in German L3 Syntax: Evidence for the L2 Status Factor. Second Language Research, 27(1), 5982.Google Scholar
Falk, Y., & Bardel, C. (2021). Transfer Patterns in L3 Learning Discussed. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11(1), 5053.Google Scholar
Fallah, N., & Jabbari, A. A. (2018). L3 Acquisition of English Attributive Adjectives: Dominant Language of Communication Matters for Syntactic Cross-Linguistic Influence. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(2), 193216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fallah, N., Jabbari, A., & Fazilatfar, A. (2016). Source(s) of Syntactic CLI: The Case of L3 Acquisition of English Possessives by Mazandarani–Persian Bilinguals. Second Language Research, 32(7), 825843.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernández-Berkes, É., & Flynn, S. (2021). Vindicating the Need for a Principled Theory of Language Acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11(1), 3036.Google Scholar
Fernandez Acosta, D. (2020). The Syntax-Pragmatics Interface: Intransitivity and Word Order in L3 Spanish. PhD thesis. University of Western Ontario.Google Scholar
Flynn, S., Foley, C., & Vinnitskaya, I. (2004). The Cumulative-Enhancement Model for Language Acquisition: Comparing Adults’ and Children’s Patterns of Development in First, Second and Third Language Acquisition. International Journal of Multilingualism, 1(1), 316.Google Scholar
García Mayo, M. Del P. (1998). The Null Subject Parameter in Adult Second Language Acquisition. Atlantis, 20(1), 5758.Google Scholar
Giancaspro, D., Halloran, B., & Iverson, M. (2015). Transfer at the Initial Stages of L3 Brazilian Portuguese: A Look at Three Groups of English/Spanish Bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2).Google Scholar
Goad, H., White, L., & Steele, J. (2003). Missing Inflection in L2 Acquisition: Defective Syntax or L1-Constrained Prosodic Representations? Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 48, 243263.Google Scholar
Godfroid, A., Loewen, S., Jung, S., et al. (2015). Timed and Untimed Grammaticality Judgments Measure Distinct Types of Knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37, 269297.Google Scholar
González Alonso, J., & Rothman, J. (2017). Coming of Age in L3 Initial Stages Transfer Models: Deriving Developmental Predictions and Looking towards the Future. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 683697.Google Scholar
González Alonso, J., & Rothman, J. (2020). Avoiding the Cum hoc ergo propter hoc Fallacy: Comments and Questions Regarding Full Transfer Potential. Second Language Research, 37(3), 423428.Google Scholar
González Alonso, J., Puig-Mayenco, E., Fábregas, A., Chaouch-Orozco, A., & Rothman, J. (2021). On the Status of Transfer in Adult Third Language Acquisition of Early Bilinguals. PLOS ONE, 16(3), E0247976.Google Scholar
Gorgone, M. (2018). The Acquisition of English as an L3 by Catalan/Spanish Bilinguals. Master’s thesis. Uit, The Arctic University of Norway.Google Scholar
Granena, G., & Long, M. (2013). Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude, and Ultimate L2 Attainment. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Grüter, T. (2006). Another Take on the L2 Initial State: Evidence from Comprehension in L2 German. Language Acquisition, 12(4), 287317.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Casillas, G. (2008). Explaining Frequency of Verb Morphology in Early L2 Speech. Lingua, 118, 595612.Google Scholar
Heil, J., & Ebert, S. (2018). Extra-syntactic Factors in the That-Trace Effect. In MacDonald, J. E. (Ed.), Contemporary Trends in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics: Selected Papers from the Hispanic Linguistic Symposium 2015 (pp. 309332). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hermas, A. (2010). Language Acquisition as Computational Resetting: Verb Movement in L3 Initial State. International Journal of Multilingualism, 7(4), 202224.Google Scholar
Hermas, A. (2014). Multilingual Transfer: L1 Morphosyntax in L3 English. International Journal of Language Studies, 8(2), 124.Google Scholar
Hopp, H. (2019). Cross-Linguistic Influence in the Child Third Language Acquisition of Grammar: Sentence Comprehension and Production among Turkish–German and German Learners of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23(2), 567583.Google Scholar
Ionin, T., & Scutti Santos, H. (2021). Sources of Knowledge in L3 Acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11(1), 6569.Google Scholar
Ionin, T., Grolla, E., Santos, H., & Montrul, S. (2015). Interpretation of NPs in Generic and Existential Contexts in L3 Brazilian Portuguese. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 5, 215251.Google Scholar
Iverson, M. (2009). N-Drop at the Initial State of L3 Portuguese: Comparing Simultaneous and Additive Bilinguals of English/Spanish. In Pires, A. & Rothman, J. (Eds.), Minimalist Inquiries into Child and Adult Language Acquisition: Case Studies Across Portuguese (pp. 221244). Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Iverson, M. (2010). Informing the Age of Acquisition Debate: L3 as a Litmus Test. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL), 48(2–3), 221243.Google Scholar
Jabbari, A. A., Achard-Bayle, G., & Ablali, D. (2018). Acquisition of L3 French Wh-Question Structure by Persian–English Bilinguals. Cogent Education, 5(1), 118.Google Scholar
Jegerski, J., Keating, G., & VanPatten, B. (2016). On-line Relative Clause Attachment Strategy in Heritage Speakers of Spanish. International Journal of Bilingualism, 20(3), 254268.Google Scholar
Kopečkov, R., Gut, U., Wrembel, M., & Balas, A. (2022). Phonological Cross-linguistic Influence at the Initial Stages of L3 Acquisition. Second Language Research. https://doi.org/10.1177/02676583221123994.Google Scholar
Kupisch, T., Snape, N., & Stangen, I. (2013). Foreign Language Acquisition in Heritage Speakers. In Duarte, J. & Gogolin, I. (Eds.), Linguistic Superdiversity in Urban Areas: Research Approaches (pp. 99122). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kupisch, T., Pereira Soares, S. M., Puig-Mayenco, E., & Rothman, J. (2021). Multilingualism and Chomsky’s Generative Grammar. In Allot, N., Lohndal, T., & Rey, G. (Eds.), Wiley-Blackwell Companion to Chomsky. New York: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lago, S. (2021). Some Challenges of Relating Wholesale Transfer Approaches to L3 Linguistic Behavior. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11(1), 7578.Google Scholar
Lakashmanan, U. (1986). The Role of Parametric Variation in Adult Second Language Acquisition: The Case of the Pro-Drop Parameter. Papers in Applied Linguistics – Michigan, 2, 97118.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. (2000). Mapping Features to Forms in Second Language Acquisition. In Archibald, J. (Ed.), Second Language Acquisition and Linguistic Theory (pp. 102129). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. (2009). Some Thoughts on a Contrastive Analysis of Features in Second Language Acquisition. Second Language Research, 25(2), 173227.Google Scholar
Llisterri, J., & Poch-Olivé, D. (1987). Phonetic Interference in Bilingual’s Learning of a Third Language. In ICPhS (Ed.), Proceedings of the XIth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (vol. 5; pp. 134147). Tallinn: Institute of Language and Literature, Academy of Sciences of the Estonian S.S.R.Google Scholar
Lloyd-Smith, A., Gyllstad, H., Kupisch, T., & Quaglia, S. (2021). Heritage Language Proficiency Does Not Predict Syntactic CLI into L3 English. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 24(3), 435451Google Scholar
Long, M. (2005). Problems with Supposed Counter-Evidence to the Critical Period Hypothesi. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 43(4), 287317.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Dias, R., & Santos, H. (2009). On Some Null Subject Parameter-Related Properties in the L3 Acquisition of Brazilian Portuguese. Revista Estudos Da Língua(Gem), 7, 163198.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Dias, R., & Santos, H. (2011). Clitics and Object Expression in the L3 Acquisition of Brazilian Portuguese. Structural Similarity Matters for Transfer. Second Language Research, 27(1), 2158.Google Scholar
Mykhaylyk, R., Mitrofanova, N., Rodina, Y., & Westergaard, M. (2015). The Linguistic Proximity Model: The Case of Verb-Second Revisited. In Grillo, E. & Jepson, K. (Eds.), 39th Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Nogueira, L. (2020). Morphosyntactic Transfer in L3 English Beyond the Initial Stages: A Study of 2L1 Speakers of Catalan and Spanish. Master’s thesis. Norwegian University of Science and Technology.Google Scholar
Ortin, R., & Fernandez-Florez, C. (2019). Transfer of Variable Grammars in Third Language Acquisition. International Journal of Multilingualism, 16(4), 442458.Google Scholar
Parma, A. (2017). Cross-Linguistic Transfer of Object Clitic Structure: A Case of L3 Brazilian Portuguese. Languages, 2(3), article 14.Google Scholar
Pereira Soares, S. M., & Rothman, J. (2021). Cognitive States in Third Language Acquisition and Beyond. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11(1), 8995.Google Scholar
Perpiñán, S. (2017). Catalan–Spanish Bilingualism Continuum: The Expression of Non-personal Catalan Clitics in the Adult Grammar of Early Bilinguals. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism2, 7(5), 477513.Google Scholar
Perpiñán, S. (2018). Ongoing Language Change, and Crosslinguistic Influence in Direct Object Expression in Catalan–Spanish Bilingualism. Languages, 3(2), 114.Google Scholar
Picoral, A., & Carvalho, A. M. (2020). The Acquisition of Preposition + Article Contractions in L3 Portuguese among Different L1-Speaking Learners: A Variationist Approach. Languages, 5(4), 45.Google Scholar
Puig-Mayenco, E., & Marsden, H. (2018). Polarity-Item Anything in L3 English: Where Does Transfer Come from When the L1 is Catalan and the L2 is Spanish? Second Language Research, 34(4), 487515.Google Scholar
Puig-Mayenco, E., & Rothman, J. (2020). Low Proficiency Does Not Mean Ab initio: A Methodological Footnote for Linguistic Transfer Studies. Language Acquisition, 27(2), 217226.Google Scholar
Puig-Mayenco, E., Miller, D., & Rothman, J. (2018a). Language Dominance and Transfer Selection in L3 Acquisition: Evidence from Sentential Negation and Negative Quantifiers in L3 English. In Cho, J., Iverson, M., Judy, T., Leal, T., & Shimanskaya, E. (Eds.), Meaning and Structure in Second Language Acquisition: In Honor of Roumyana Slabakova. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Puig-Mayenco, E., Cunnings, I., Bayram, F., et al. (2018b). Language Dominance Affects Bilingual Performance and Processing Outcomes in Adulthood. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01199.Google Scholar
Puig-Mayenco, E., González Alonso, J., & Rothman, J. (2020a). A Systematic Review of Transfer Studies in Third Language Acquisition. Second Language Research, 36(1), 3164.Google Scholar
Puig-Mayenco, E., Rothman, J., & Tubau, S. (2020b). Language Dominance in the Previously Acquired Languages Modulates Rate of Third Language (L3) Development over Time: A Longitudinal Study. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 25(5), 16611664.Google Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L. (1999). Accounting for Morphological Variation in Second Language Acquisition: Truncation or Missing Inflection? In Friedemann, M.-A. & Rizzi, L. (Eds.), The Acquisition of Syntax (pp. 202235). London: Longman.Google Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L. (2000). Missing Surface Inflection or Impairment in Second Language Acquisition? Evidence from Tense and Agreement. Second Language Research, 16(2), 103133.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2010). On the Typological Economy of Syntactic Transfer: Word Order and Relative Clause High/Low Attachment Preference in L3 Brazilian Portuguese. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL), 48, 245273.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2011). L3 Syntactic Transfer Selectivity and Typological Determinacy: The Typological Primacy Model. Second Language Research, 27(1), 107127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J. (2015). Linguistic and Cognitive Motivations for the Typological Primacy Model (TPM) of Third Language (L3) Transfer: Timing of Acquisition and Proficiency Considered. Bilingualism, 18(2), 179190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J., & Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2010). What Variables Condition Syntactic Transfer? A Look at the L3 Initial State. Second Language Research, 26, 189218.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., & Halloran, B. (2013). Formal Linguistic Approaches to L3/Ln Acquisition: A Focus on Morphosyntactic Transfer in Adult Multilingualism. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 33, 5167.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., & Iverson, M. (2007). On Parameter Clustering and Resetting the Null-Subject Parameter in L2 Spanish: Implications and Observations. Hispania, 90, 328341.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., & Treffers-Daller, J. (2014). A Prolegomenon to the Construct of the Native Speaker: Heritage Speaker Bilinguals Are Natives Too! Applied Linguistics, 35(1), 9398.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., Cabrelli Amaro, J., & de Bot, K. (2013). Third Language Acquisition. In Herschensohn, J. & Young-Scholten, M. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition (pp. 372393). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., González Alonso, J., & Puig-Mayenco, E. (2019). Third Language Acquisition and Linguistic Transfer. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Santos, H. (2013). Cross-Linguistic Influence in the Acquisition of Portuguese as a Third Language. PhD dissertation. University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B., & Sprouse, R. (1996). L2 Cognitive States and the Full Transfer/Full Access Model. Second Language Research, 12, 4072.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B., & Sprouse, R. (2020). The Full Transfer/Full Access Model and L3 Cognitive States. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11(1), 129.Google Scholar
Siemund, P., & Lechner, S. (2015). Transfer Effects in the Acquisition of English as an Additional Language by Bilingual Children in Germany. In Peukert, Hagen (Ed.), Transfer Effects in Multilingual Language Development (pp. 147160). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Siemund, P., Schröter, S., & Rahbari, S. (2018). Learning English Demonstrative Pronouns on Bilingual Substrate: Evidence from German Heritage Speakers of Russian, Turkish and Vietnamese. In Bonnet, A. & Siemund, P. (Eds.), Language Education in Multilingual Classrooms (pp. 381405). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2017). The Scalpel Model of Third Language Acquisition. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 651666.Google Scholar
Soto-Corominas, A. (2019). Acquisition of Quantified Partitivity in Catalan–Spanish Bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11(6), 873908.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I., & Dimitrakopoulou, M. (2007). The Interpretability Hypothesis: Evidence from Wh-Interrogatives in Second Language Acquisition. Second Language Research, 23(2), 215242.Google Scholar
Villegas, Á. (2014). The Role of L2 English Immersion in the Processing of L1 Spanish Sentence Complement/Relative Clause Ambiguities. PhD dissertation. Pennsylvania State University.Google Scholar
Westergaard, M. (2020). Microvariation in Multilingual Situations: The Importance of Property-by-Property Acquisition. Second Language Research, 37(3), 379407.Google Scholar
Westergaard, M. (2021). The Plausibility of Wholesale vs. Property-by-Property Transfer in L3 Acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11(1), 103108.Google Scholar
Westergaard, M., Mitrofanova, N., Mykhaylyk, R., & Rodina, Y. (2017). Crosslinguistic Influence in the Acquisition of a Third Language: The Linguistic Proximity Model. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 666682.Google Scholar
White, L. (1985). The “Pro-Drop” Parameter in Adult Second Language Acquisition. Language Learning, 35, 4762.Google Scholar
Wrembel, M. (2021). Transfer vs. Dynamic Cross-Linguistic Interactions. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11(1), 109115.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×