Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-rkxrd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T12:31:59.414Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - The Meronomic Model of Cognitive Change and Its Application to Neolithic Çatalhöyük

from Part III - Greater Innovation and Creativity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 February 2020

Ian Hodder
Affiliation:
Stanford University, California
Get access

Summary

There is a natural desire to explain the development of cognitive phenomena that seem particularly characteristic of the human mind. Creativity is a case in point, consciousness another. A recent archaeological project has attempted to understand evidence from the Neolithic site of Çatalhöyük in relation to the emergence of both consciousness and creativity at the dawn of settled life. This project faces the need to settle on a precise understanding of what is meant by the terms in question. But theorists differ as to what this understanding should be. And there are deeper disputes relating to cognition in general. What the term ‘cognitive’ signifies continues to be debated, with some arguing that it has no precise meaning that would differentiate it from terms such as ‘adaptive’ and ‘intelligent’.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Brachman, R. J. (1983). What IS-A Is and Isn’t: An Analysis of Taxonomic Links in Semantic Networks. IEEE Computer, 16, No. 10.Google Scholar
Carnap, R. (1947). Meaning and Necessity: A Study in Semantics and Modal Logic, Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2016). Surfing Uncertainty: Prediction, Action, and the Embodied Mind, Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cruse, D. A. (1979). On the Transitivity of the Part-Whole Relation. Journal of Linguistics, 15 (pp. 2938).Google Scholar
Fiorini, S. R. and Gärdenfors, P. (2014). Representing Part-Whole Relations in Conceptual Spaces. Cognitive Processing, 15, No. 2 (pp. 127142).Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gärdenfors, P. (2004). Conceptual Spaces: The Geometry of Thought (paperback edition), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gebel, H. G. K. (2002). Walls. Loci of Forces. In Gebel, H. G., Hermansen, B. D., and Jensen, C. H. (Eds.), Magic Practices in the Near Eastern Neolithic: Studies in Early Near Eastern Production, Subsistence and Environment 8 (pp. 119132), Berlin: Ex Oriente.Google Scholar
Gebel, H. G. K. (2013). The Neolithic Commodification of Stone. In Borrell, F., Ibanez, J. J., and Molist, M. (Eds.), Stone Tools in Transition: From Hunter-Gatherers to Farming Societies in the Near East 1 (pp. 191206), Barcelona: Universidad Autónoma de Barcelona.Google Scholar
Gerstl, P. and Pribbenow, S. (1995). Midwinters, End Games, and Body Parts: A Classification of Part-Whole Relations. International Journal of Human- Computer Studies, 43 (pp. 865889).Google Scholar
Habel, C., Pribbenow, S. and Simmons, G. (1995). Partonomies and Depictions: A Hybrid Approach. In Glasgow, J., Narayanan, N. H., and Chandrasekaran, B. (Eds.), Diagrammatic Reasoning: Cognitive and Computational Perspectives (pp. 627653), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hodder, I. (2006a). The Leopard’s Tale: Revealing the Mysteries of Çatalhöyük, London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
Hodder, I. (2006b). This Old House. Natural History Magazine.Google Scholar
Hodder, I. (2012). Entangled: An Archaeology of the Relationships between Humans and Things, Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Johansson, I. (2006). Formal Mereology and Ordinary Language – Reply to Varzi. Applied Ontology, 1 (pp. 157161).Google Scholar
Keane, W. (2003). Semiotics and the Social Analysis of Material Things. Language and Communication, 23 (pp. 409425).Google Scholar
Keane, W. (2005). Signs Are Not the Garb of Meaning: On the Social Analysis of Material Things. In Miller, D. (Ed.), Materiality (pp. 182205), Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Machery, E. (2009). Doing without Concepts, Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mellaart, J. (1967). Çatalhöyük: A Neolithic Town in Anatolia, London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1843/1965). On the Logic of Moral Sciences, New York: Bobbs-Merrill.Google Scholar
Mithen, S. (1996). The Prehistory of the Mind: A Search for the Origins of Art, Religion and Science. London: Thames and Hudson.Google Scholar
Pauketat, T. R. (2013). Bundles of/in/as Time. In Robb, I. and Pauketat, T. R. (Eds.), Big Histories, Human Lives. Tackling Problems of Scale in Archaeology (pp. 3556), Santa Fe, NM: SAR Press.Google Scholar
Ross, W. E. (1924). Aristotle’s Metaphysics, Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Szabó, Z. G. (2012). The Case for Compositionality. In Werning, M., Hinzen, W., and Machery, E. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Compositionality (pp. 6480), Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tversky, B. (1989). Parts, Partonomies, and Taxonomies. Developmental Psychology, 25, No. 6 (pp. 983995).Google Scholar
Tversky, B. (2005). On Exploring Parts and Wholes. In Gero, J. S. and Maher, M. L. (Eds.), Computational and Cognitive Models of Creative Design VI (pp. 316), Sydney, Australia: Key Centre of Design Computing and Cognition, University of Sydney.Google Scholar
Winston, M. E., Chaffin, R., and Herrmann, D. (1987). A Taxonomy of Part-Whole Relations. Cognitive Science, 11, No. 4 (pp. 417444).Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×