Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-5nwft Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T15:33:31.259Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

22 - Stereotype Threat and the Cognitive Performance of Older Adults

from Part III - Aging in a Socioemotional Context

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 May 2020

Ayanna K. Thomas
Affiliation:
Tufts University, Massachusetts
Angela Gutchess
Affiliation:
Brandeis University, Massachusetts
Get access

Summary

Because of age-related cognitive declines, people sometimes expect older adults to be incompetent, senile, or extremely forgetful. Research on stereotype threat suggests that these negative expectations can interfere with older adults’ performance on cognitive tests and can cause them to underperform compared to their potential. However, research also shows that not all cognitive domains, and not all older adults, are equally affected by stereotype threat. Stereotype threat effects seem to occur most reliably on cognitive tasks where there are larger deficit expectations and for people who have concerns or negative expectations about aging and cognition. In addition to outlining these task and individual differences, in this chapter we also review potential mechanisms underlying these effects and discuss the applied implications of stereotype threat for how older adults’ memory is assessed in clinical and eyewitness settings.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Aging
A Life Course Perspective
, pp. 400 - 416
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abdou, C. M., Fingerhut, A. W., Jackson, J. S., & Wheaton, F. (2016). Healthcare stereotype threat in older adults in the health and retirement study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 50(2), 191198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2015.07.034Google Scholar
Abrams, D., Crisp, R. J., Marques, S., et al. (2008). Threat inoculation: Experienced and imagined intergenerational contact prevents stereotype threat effects on older people’s math performance. Psychology and Aging, 23(4), 934939. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014293CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abrams, D., Eller, A., & Bryant, J. (2006). An age apart: The effects of intergenerational contact and stereotype threat on performance and intergroup bias. Psychology and Aging, 21(4), 691702. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.4.691Google Scholar
Alquist, J. L., Price, M. M., Hancock, D., Talley, A. E., & Cukrowicz, K. (2019). Exposure to negative stereotypes impairs older adults’ self-control. Self and Identity, 18(2), 217226. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2018.1437069Google Scholar
Andreoletti, C., & Lachman, M. E. (2004). Susceptibility and resilience to memory aging stereotypes: Education matters more than age. Experimental Aging Research, 30(2), 129148. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610730490274167Google Scholar
Armstrong, B., Gallant, S. N., Li, L., Patel, K., & Wong, B. I. (2017). Stereotype threat effects on older adults’ episodic and working memory: A meta-analysis.Gerontologist, 57(Suppl. 2), 193205. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnx170Google Scholar
Barber, S. J. (2017). An examination of age-based stereotype threat about cognitive decline: Implications for stereotype-threat research and theory development. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(1), 6290. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616656345CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barber, S. J., & Mather, M. (2013a). Stereotype threat can both enhance and impair older adults’ memory. Psychological Science, 24(12), 25222529. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613497023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barber, S. J., & Mather, M. (2013b). Stereotype threat can reduce older adults’ memory errors. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(10), 18881895. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.840656Google Scholar
Barber, S. J., & Mather, M. (2014). Stereotype threat in older adults: When and why does it occur and who is most affected? In Verhaeghen, P. & Hertzog, C. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of emotion, social cognition, and problem solving in adulthood (pp. 302320). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barber, S. J., Mather, M., & Gatz, M. (2015). How stereotype threat affects healthy older adults’ performance on clinical assessments of cognitive decline: The key role of regulatory fit. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 70, 891900. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbv009Google Scholar
Barber, S. J., Seliger, J., Yeh, N., & Tan, S. C. (2019). Stereotype threat reduces the positivity of older adults’ recall. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 74(4), 585594. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby026Google Scholar
Bouazzaoui, B., Follenfant, A., Ric, F., et al. (2016). Ageing-related stereotypes in memory: When the beliefs come true. Memory, 24(5), 659668. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2015.1040802CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brubaker, M. S., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2018). The effects of stereotype threat on the associative memory deficit of older adults. Psychology and Aging, 33(1), 1729. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000194Google Scholar
Chasteen, A. L., Bhattacharyya, S., Horhota, M., Tam, R., & Hasher, L. (2005). How feelings of stereotype threat influence older adults’ memory performance. Experimental Aging Research, 31(3), 235260. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610730590948177CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chasteen, A. L., Kang, S. K., & Remedios, J. D. (2012). Aging and stereotype threat: Development, process, and interventions. In Inzlicht, M. & Schmader, T. (Eds.), Stereotype threat: Theory, process, and application (pp. 202216). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Coudin, G., & Alexopoulos, T. (2010). “Help me! I’m old!” How negative aging stereotypes create dependency among older adults. Aging and Mental Health, 14(5), 516523. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199732449.003.0013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craik, F. I., & McDowd, J. M. (1987). Age differences in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(3), 474479. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.13.3.474Google Scholar
Desrichard, O., & Köpetz, C. (2005). A threat in the elder: The impact of task‐instructions, self‐efficacy and performance expectations on memory performance in the elderly. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35(4), 537552. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.249Google Scholar
Eich, T. S., Murayama, K., Castel, A. D., & Knowlton, B. J. (2014). The dynamic effects of age-related stereotype threat on explicit and implicit memory performance in older adults. Social Cognition, 32(6), 559570. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2014.32.6.559Google Scholar
Fernández-Ballesteros, R., Bustillos, A., & Huici, C. (2015). Positive perception of aging and performance in a memory task: Compensating for stereotype threat? Experimental Aging Research, 41(4), 410425. https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2015.1053757Google Scholar
Fresson, M., Dardenne, B., Geurten, M., & Meulemans, T. (2017). The effect of stereotype threat on older people’s clinical cognitive outcomes: Investigating the moderating role of dementia worry. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 31(8), 13061328. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2017.1307456Google Scholar
Gaillard, M., Desmette, D., & Keller, J. (2011). Regulatory focus moderates the influence of age-related stereotypic expectancies on older adults’ test performance and threat-based concerns. Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée/European Review of Applied Psychology, 61(1), 2329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2010.11.001Google Scholar
Gallo, D. A. (2010). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion. Memory and Cognition, 38(7), 833848. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.7.833Google Scholar
Grimm, L. R., Markman, A. B., Maddox, W. T., & Baldwin, G. C. (2009). Stereotype threat reinterpreted as a regulatory mismatch. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(2), 288304. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013463Google Scholar
Haslam, C., Morton, T. A., Haslam, S. A., et al. (2012). “When the age is in, the wit is out”: Age-related self-categorization and deficit expectations reduce performance on clinical tests used in dementia assessment. Psychology and Aging, 27(3), 778784. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027754Google Scholar
Hehman, J. A., & Bugental, D. B. (2013). “Life stage-specific” variations in performance in response to age stereotypes. Developmental Psychology, 49(7), 13961406. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029559Google Scholar
Henkel, L. A. (2013). Do older adults change their eyewitness reports when re-questioned? Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 69(3), 356365. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbt071Google Scholar
Hess, T. M. (2005). Memory and aging in context. Psychological Bulletin, 131(3), 383406. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.3.383Google Scholar
Hess, T. M., Auman, C., Colcombe, S. J., & Rahhal, T. A. (2003). The impact of stereotype threat on age differences in memory performance. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58, 311. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/58.1.P3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, T. M., Emery, L., & Queen, T. L. (2009). Task demands moderate stereotype threat effects on memory performance. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 64(4), 482486. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbp044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, T. M., & Hinson, J. T. (2006). Age-related variation in the influences of aging stereotypes on memory in adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 21(3), 621625. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.621CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, T. M., Hinson, J. T., & Hodges, E. A. (2009). Moderators of and mechanisms underlying stereotype threat effects on older adults’ memory performance. Experimental Aging Research, 35, 153177. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610730802716413Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 146. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60381-0Google Scholar
Horton, S., Baker, J., Pearce, W., & Deakin, J. M. (2010). Immunity to popular stereotypes of aging? Seniors and stereotype threat. Educational Gerontology, 36, 353371. https://doi.org/10.1080/03601270903323976Google Scholar
Jordano, M. L., & Touron, D. R. (2017). Stereotype threat as a trigger of mind-wandering in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 32(3), 307313. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kang, S. K., & Chasteen, A. L. (2009). The moderating role of age-group identification and perceived threat on stereotype threat among older adults. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 69(3), 201220. https://doi.org/10.2190/AG.69.3.cGoogle Scholar
Kensinger, E. A., & Gutchess, A. H. (2017). Cognitive aging in a social and affective context: Advances over the past 50 years. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 72(1), 6170. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbw056Google Scholar
Kliegel, M., Jäger, T., & Phillips, L. H. (2008). Adult age differences in event-based prospective memory: A meta-analysis on the role of focal versus nonfocal cues. Psychology and Aging, 23(1), 203208. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.203Google Scholar
Koen, J. D., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2014). The effects of healthy aging, amnestic mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease on recollection and familiarity: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology Review, 24(3), 332354. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-014-9266-5Google Scholar
Lambert, A. E., Watson, J. M., Stefanucci, J. K., et al. (2016). Stereotype threat impairs older adult driving. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30(1), 2228. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3162Google Scholar
Lamont, R. A., Swift, H. J., & Abrams, D. (2015). A review and meta-analysis of age-based stereotype threat: Negative stereotypes, not facts, do the damage. Psychology and Aging, 30, 180193. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038586Google Scholar
Lemaire, P., Brun, F., & Régner, I. (2018). Negative aging stereotypes disrupt both the selection and execution of strategies in older adults. Gerontology, 64, 373381. https://doi.org/10.1159/000486756CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, P., Zhao, F., Zhang, B., & Dang, Q. (2017). Small change makes a big splash: The role of working self-concept in the effects of stereotype threat on memory. Journal of Psychology, 151(7), 613631. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2017.1372340Google Scholar
Löckenhoff, C. E., De Fruyt, F., Terracciano, A., et al. (2009). Perceptions of aging across 26 cultures and their culture-level associates. Psychology and Aging, 24(4), 941954. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016901Google Scholar
Marquet, M., Missotten, P., Dardenne, B., & Adam, S. (2019). Interactions between stereotype threat, subjective aging, and memory in older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 26(1), 121143. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2017.1413166Google Scholar
Mazerolle, M., Regner, I., Barber, S. J., et al. (2016). Negative aging stereotypes impair performance on brief cognitive tests used to screen for predementia. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 72(6), 932936. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbw083Google Scholar
Mazerolle, M., Regner, I., Morisset, P., Rigalleau, F., & Huguet, P. (2012). Stereotype threat strengthens automatic recall and undermines controlled processes in older adults. Psychological Science, 23(7), 723727. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612437607Google Scholar
Mazerolle, M., Régner, I., Rigalleau, F., & Huguet, P. (2015). Stereotype threat alters the subjective experience of memory. Experimental Psychology, 62(6), 395402. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000303Google Scholar
Montepare, J. M., & Lachman, M. E. (1989). You’re only as old as you feel: Self-perceptions of age, fears of aging, and life satisfaction from adolescence to old age. Psychology and Aging, 4, 7378. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.4.1.73Google Scholar
Old, S. R., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2008). Differential effects of age on item and associative measures of memory: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 23(1), 104118. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.104Google Scholar
Park, D. C., Smith, A. D., Dudley, W. N., & Lafronza, V. N. (1989). Effects of age and a divided attention task presented during encoding and retrieval on memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 11851191. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.15.6.1185Google Scholar
Phibbs, S., & Hooker, K. (2018). An exploration of factors associated with ageist stereotype threat in a medical setting. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 73, 11601165. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbx034Google Scholar
Popham, L. E., & Hess, T. M. (2013). Age differences in the underlying mechanisms of stereotype threat effects. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 70(2), 223232. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbt093Google Scholar
Popham, L. E., & Hess, T. M. (2016). Stereotype threat. In Whitbourne, S. K. (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of adulthood and aging (Vol. 1, pp. 15). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Rahhal, T. A., Hasher, L., & Colcombe, S. J. (2001). Instructional manipulations and age differences in memory: Now you see them, now you don’t. Psychology and Aging, 16(4), 697706. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.16.4.697Google Scholar
Reed, A. E., Chan, L., & Mikels, J. A. (2014). Meta-analysis of the age-related positivity effect: Age differences in preferences for positive over negative information. Psychology and Aging, 29(1), 115. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035194Google Scholar
Reid, L. M., & MacLullich, A. M. (2006). Subjective memory complaints and cognitive impairment in older people. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 22(5–6), 471485. https://doi.org/10.1159/000096295Google Scholar
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(4), 803814. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.21.4.803Google Scholar
Rossi-Arnaud, C., Spataro, P., & Geraci, L. (2018). Effects of stereotype threat and prior task success on older adults’ eyewitness memory. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 7, 422431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.02.001Google Scholar
Schlemmer, M., & Desrichard, O. (2018). Is medical environment detrimental to memory? A test of a white coat effect on older people’s memory performance. Clinical Gerontologist, 41(1), 7781. https://doi.org/10.1080/07317115.2017.1307891CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115(2), 336356. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336Google Scholar
Seibt, B., & Förster, J. (2004). Stereotype threat and performance: How self-stereotypes influence processing by inducing regulatory foci. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(1), 3856. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.38Google Scholar
Sindi, S., Fiocco, A. J., Juster, R. P., Pruessner, J., & Lupien, S. J. (2013). When we test, do we stress? Impact of the testing environment on cortisol secretion and memory performance in older adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(8), 13881396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.004Google Scholar
Smith, A. M., Gallo, D. A., Barber, S. J., Maddox, K. B., & Thomas, A. K. (2017). Stereotypes, warnings, and identity-related variables influence older adults’ susceptibility to associative false memory errors. Gerontologist, 57(Suppl. 2), S206S215. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnx057Google Scholar
Spencer, S. J., Logel, C., & Davies, P. G. (2016). Stereotype threat. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 415437. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-073115-103235Google Scholar
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52(6), 613629. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613Google Scholar
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797811. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797Google Scholar
Swift, H. J., Abrams, D., & Marques, S. (2012). Threat or boost? Social comparison affects older people’s performance differently depending on task domain. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 68(1), 2330. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbs044Google Scholar
Swift, H. J., Lamont, R. A., & Abrams, D. (2012). Are they half as strong as they used to be? An experiment testing whether age-related social comparisons impair older people’s hand grip strength and persistence. BMJ Open, 2(3), 16. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2012-001064Google Scholar
Tan, S. C., & Barber, S. J. (in press). Confucian values as a buffer against age-based stereotype threat for Chinese older adults. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 75(3), 504–512. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby049Google Scholar
Thomas, A. K., & Dubois, S. J. (2011). Reducing the burden of stereotype threat eliminates age differences in memory distortion. Psychological Science, 22(12), 15151517. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611425932Google Scholar
Thomas, A. K., Smith, A. M., & Mazerolle, M. (in press). The unexpected relationship between retrieval demands and memory performance when older adults are faced with age-related stereotypes. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 75(2), 241250. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby031Google Scholar
Verhaeghen, P., Marcoen, A., & Goossens, L. (1993). Facts and fiction about memory aging: A quantitative integration of research findings. Journal of Gerontology, 48(4), 157171. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/48.4.P157Google Scholar
von Hippel, C., Kalokerinos, E. K., & Henry, J. D. (2013). Stereotype threat among older employees: Relationship with job attitudes and turnover intentions. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 1727. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029825Google Scholar
Weiss, D. (2018). On the inevitability of aging: Essentialist beliefs moderate the impact of negative age stereotypes on older adults’ memory performance and physiological reactivity. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 73, 925933. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbw087Google Scholar
Wong, J. T., & Gallo, D. A. (2016). Stereotype threat reduces false recognition when older adults are forewarned. Memory, 24(5), 650658. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2015.1036885Google Scholar
Wong, J. T., & Gallo, D. A. (2019). Activating aging stereotypes increases source recollection confusions in older adults: Effect at encoding but not retrieval. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 74(4), 633641. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbx103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, B., Lin, Y., Gao, Q., et al. (2017). Effects of aging stereotype threat on working self-concepts: An event-related potentials approach. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9, p. 223. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00223Google Scholar
Zuber, S., Ihle, A., Blum, A., Desrichard, O., & Kliegel, M. (2019). The effect of stereotype threat on age differences in prospective memory performance: Differential effects on focal versus nonfocal tasks. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 74(4), 625632. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbx097Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×