Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-5nwft Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-24T16:17:14.415Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

29 - Eating with our eyes: on the color of flavor

from Part VII - Color effects on psychological and biological functioning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2016

Andrew J. Elliot
Affiliation:
University of Rochester, New York
Mark D. Fairchild
Affiliation:
Rochester Institute of Technology, New York
Anna Franklin
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anon. (2001). What’s green. Easy to squirt? Ketchup! USA Today, July 10, p. 2b.Google Scholar
Alley, R. L., and Alley, T. R. (1998). The influence of physical state and color on perceived sweetness. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 132, 561–8.Google ScholarPubMed
Bartoshuk, L. M. (2000). Comparing sensory experiences across individuals: recent psychophysical advances illuminate genetic variation in taste perception. Chemical Senses, 25, 447–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birren, F. (1963). Color and human appetite. Food Technology, 17(May), 45–7.Google Scholar
Blackwell, L. (1995). Visual clues and their effects on odour assessment. Nutrition and Food Science, 5, 24–8.Google Scholar
Bowers, J. S., and Davis, C. J. (2012). Bayesian just-so stories in psychology and neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 389414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brainard, D. H., and Radonjíc, A. (2013). Color constancy. In Werner, J. S. and Chalupa, L. M. (eds.), The New Visual Neurosciences (pp. 545–56). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bruno, N., Martani, M., Corsini, C., and Oleari, C. (2013). The effect of the color red on consuming food does not depend on achromatic (Michelson) contrast and extends to rubbing cream on the skin. Appetite, 71, 307–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calvo, C., Salvador, A., and Fiszman, S. (2001). Influence of color intensity on the perception of color and sweetness in various fruit-flavoured yoghurts. European Food Research and Technology, 213, 99103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cardello, A. V. (1994). Consumer expectations and their role in food acceptance. In MacFie, H. J. H. and Thomson, D. M. H. (eds.), Measurement of Food Preferences (pp. 253–97). London: Blackie Academic and Professional.Google Scholar
Carlsmith, J. M., and Aronson, E. (1963). Some hedonic consequences of the confirmation and disconfirmation of expectancies. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 151–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chan, M. M., and Kane-Martinelli, C. (1997). The effect of color on perceived flavor intensity and acceptance of foods by young adults and elderly adults. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 97, 657–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, C. (1985). Effect of color on judgments of food aroma and food intensity in young and elderly adults. Perception, 14, 755–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clydesdale, F. M. (1993). Color as a factor in food choice. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition, 33, 83101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, N. E. (1934). Equivalence of brightness across modalities. American Journal of Psychology, 46, 117–19.Google Scholar
Cook, W. (2012). Would you eat a ‘gourmet’ burger made with charred bamboo and squid ink? Daily Mail Online, 25 September (www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article-2208321/Burger-King-black-burger-Japan-bamboo-charcoal-squid-ink.html).Google Scholar
Crumpacker, B. (2006). The Sex Life of Food: When Body and Soul Meet to Eat. New York: Thomas Dunne Books.Google Scholar
Davis, R. G. (1981). The role of nonolfactory context cues in odor identification. Perception and Psychophysics, 30, 83–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Den Ouden, H. E. M., Daunizeau, J., Roiser, J., Friston, K. J., and Stephan, K. E. (2010). Striatal prediction error modulates cortical coupling. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 3210–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DuBose, C. N., Cardello, A. V., and Maller, O. (1980). Effects of colorants and flavorants on identification, perceived flavor intensity, and hedonic quality of fruit-flavored beverages and cake. Journal of Food Science, 45, 1393–9, 1415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncker, K. (1939). The influence of past experience upon perceptual properties. American Journal of Psychology, 52, 255–65.Google Scholar
Elliott, A., and Maier, M. A. (2014). Color psychology: effects of perceiving color on psychological functioning in humans. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 4.14.26.Google Scholar
Ernst, M. O. (2006). A Bayesian view on multimodal cue integration. In Knoblich, G., Thornton, I. M., Grosjean, M., and Shiffrar, M. (eds.), Human Body Perception from the Inside Out (pp. 105–31). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ernst, M. O. (2007). Learning to integrate arbitrary signals from vision and touch. Journal of Vision, 7(5), 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ernst, M. O. (2012). Optimal multisensory integration: assumptions and limits. In Stein, B. E. (ed.), The New Handbook of Multisensory Perception (pp. 527–43). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ernst, M. O., and Banks, M. S. (2002). Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature, 415, 429–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ernst, M. O., and Bülthoff, H. H. (2004). Merging the senses into a robust percept. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 162–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, D. (2002). Emotion: The Science of Sentiment. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernández-Vázquez, R., Hewson, L., Fisk, I., Vila, D., Mira, F., Vicario, I. M., and Hort, J. (2014). Color influences sensory perception and liking of orange juice. Flavour, 3(1).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, R. A., Ducheny, K., and Mize, S. J. S. (1989). Strawberry odor, but not red color, enhances the sweetness of sucrose solutions. Chemical Senses, 14, 371–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K. J., and Kiebel, S. J. (2009). Predictive coding under the free-energy principle. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, Biological Sciences, 364, 1211–21.Google ScholarPubMed
Gallace, A., Ngo, M. K., Sulaitis, J., and Spence, C. (2012). Multisensory presence in virtual reality: possibilities and limitations. In Ghinea, G., Andres, F., and Gulliver, S. (eds.), Multiple Sensorial Media Advances and Applications: New Developments in MulSeMedia (pp. 140). Hershey, PA: IGI Global.Google Scholar
Garber, L. L. Jr., Hyatt, E. M., and Starr, R. G. Jr. (2000). The effects of food color on perceived flavor. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 8(4), 5972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garber, L. L. Jr., Hyatt, E. M., and Starr, R. G. Jr. (2001). Placing food color experimentation into a valid consumer context. Journal of Food Products Marketing, 7(3), 324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garber, L. L. Jr., Hyatt, E. M., and Starr, R. G. Jr. (2003a). Measuring consumer response to food products. Food Quality and Preference, 14, 315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garber, L. L. Jr., Hyatt, E. M., and Starr, R. G. Jr. (2003b). Reply to commentaries on: “Placing food color experimentation into a valid consumer context”. Food Quality and Preference, 14, 41–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genschow, O., Reutner, L., and Wanke, M. (2012). The color red reduces snack food and soft drink intake. Appetite, 58, 699702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregson, R. A. M. (1964). Modification of perceived relative intensities of acid tastes by ambient illumination changes. Australian Journal of Psychology, 16, 190–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guéguen, N., and Jacob, C. (2014). Coffee cup color and evaluation of a beverage’s “warmth quality”. Color Research & Application, 39, 7981.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, R. L. (1958). Flavor study approaches at McCormick and Company, Inc. In Little, A. D., Inc. (ed.), Flavor Research and Food Acceptance: A Survey of the Scope of Flavor and Associated Research, Compiled from Papers Presented in a Series of Symposia Given in 1956–1957 (pp. 224–40). New York: Reinhold.Google Scholar
Harrar, V., Piqueras-Fiszman, B., and Spence, C. (2011). There’s no taste in a white bowl. Perception, 40, 880–92.Google Scholar
Harrar, V., and Spence, C. (2013). The taste of cutlery. Flavour, 2(21).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, G. (2011). Colorless food? We blanch. New York Times, April 3, p. 3.Google Scholar
Helbig, H. B., and Ernst, M. O. (2008). Visual-haptic cue weighting is independent of modality-specific attention. Journal of Vision, 8(10), 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutchings, J. B. (1977). The importance of visual appearance of foods to the food processor and the consumer. In Birch, G. G., Brennan, J. G., and Parker, K. J. (eds.), Sensory Properties of Foods (pp. 4557). London: Applied Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Hutchings, J. B. (2003). Expectations and the Food Industry: The Impact of Color and Appearance. New York: Plenum Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, A. (1983). The influence of color on the taste perception of carbonated water preparations. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 21, 145–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imram, N. (1999). The role of visual cues in consumer perception and acceptance of a food product. Nutrition and Food Science, 99, 224–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ISO (2008). Standard 5492: Terms relating to sensory analysis. International Organization for Standardization. Vienna: Austrian Standards Institute.Google Scholar
Johnson, J., and Clydesdale, F. M. (1982). Perceived sweetness and redness in colored sucrose solutions. Journal of Food Science, 47, 747–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. L., Dzendolet, E., and Clydesdale, F. M. (1983). Psychophysical relationships between perceived sweetness and redness in strawberry-flavored beverages. Journal of Food Protection, 46, 21–5, 28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. L., Dzendolet, E., Damon, R., Sawyer, M., and Clydesdale, F. M. (1982). Psychophysical relationships between perceived sweetness and color in cherry-flavored beverages. Journal of Food Protection, 45, 601–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, M., and Love, B. C. (2011). Bayesian fundamentalism or enlightenment? On the explanatory status and theoretical contributions of Bayesian models of cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 169231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanig, J. L. (1955). Mental impact of colors in foods studied. Food Field Reporter, 23, 57.Google Scholar
Kappes, S. M., Schmidt, S. J., and Lee, S.-Y. (2006). Color halo/horns and halo-attribute dumping effects within descriptive analysis of carbonated beverages. Journal of Food Science, 71, S590–5.Google Scholar
Koch, C., and Koch, E. C. (2003). Preconceptions of taste based on color. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 137, 233–42.Google ScholarPubMed
Kostyla, A. S. (1978). The Psychophysical Relationships Between Color and Flavor of Some Fruit Flavored Beverages. Ph.D. thesis, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Kostyla, A. S., and Clydesdale, F. M. (1978). The psychophysical relationships between color and flavor. CRC Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition, 10, 303–19.Google ScholarPubMed
Koza, B. J., Cilmi, A., Dolese, M., and Zellner, D. A. (2005). Color enhances orthonasal olfactory intensity and reduces retronasal olfactory intensity. Chemical Senses, 30, 643–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavin, J. G., and Lawless, H. T. (1998). Effects of color and odor on judgments of sweetness among children and adults. Food Quality and Preference, 9, 283–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levitan, C., Zampini, M., Li, R., and Spence, C. (2008). Assessing the role of color cues and people’s beliefs about color-flavor associations on the discrimination of the flavor of sugar-coated chocolates Chemical Senses, 33, 415–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lim, J., Urban, L., and Green, B. G. (2008). Measures of individual differences in taste and creaminess perception. Chemical Senses, 33, 493501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyman, B. (1989). A Psychology of Food: More Than a Matter of Taste. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
Maga, J. A. (1974). Influence of color on taste thresholds. Chemical Senses and Flavor, 1, 115–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masurovsky, B. I. (1939). How to obtain the right food color. Food Industries, 11, 55–6.Google Scholar
Miller, E. G., and Kahn, B. E. (2005). Shades of meaning: the effect of color and flavor names on consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 8692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moir, H. C. (1936). Some observations on the appreciation of flavour in foodstuffs. Journal of the Society of Chemical Industry: Chemistry and Industry Review, 14, 145–8.Google Scholar
Morrot, G., Brochet, F., and Dubourdieu, D. (2001). The color of odors. Brain and Language, 79, 309–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nath, A. R., and Beauchamp, M. S. (2011). Dynamic changes in superior temporal sulcus connectivity during perception of noisy audiovisual speech. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1704–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberfeld, D., Hecht, H., Allendorf, U., and Wickelmaier, F. (2009). Ambient lighting modifies the flavor of wine. Journal of Sensory Studies, 24, 797832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Mahony, M. (1983). Gustatory responses to nongustatory stimuli. Perception, 12, 627–33.Google ScholarPubMed
Okajima, K., and Spence, C. (2011). Effects of visual food texture on taste perception. i-Perception, 2(8) (http://i-perception.perceptionweb.com/journal/I/article/ic966).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oram, N., Laing, D. G., Hutchinson, I., Owen, J., Rose, G., Freeman, M., and Newell, G. (1995). The influence of flavor and color on drink identification by children and adults. Developmental Psychobiology, 28, 239–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Österbauer, R. A., Matthews, P. M., Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Hansen, P. C., and Calvert, G. A. (2005). Color of scents: chromatic stimuli modulate odor responses in the human brain. Journal of Neurophysiology, 93, 3434–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pangborn, R. M., Berg, H. W., and Hansen, B. (1963). The influence of color on discrimination of sweetness in dry table-wine. American Journal of Psychology, 76, 492–5.Google Scholar
Parr, W. V., White, K. G., and Heatherbell, D. (2003). The nose knows: influence of color on perception of wine aroma. Journal of Wine Research, 14, 79101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Philipsen, D. H., Clydesdale, F. M., Griffin, R. W., and Stern, P. (1995). Consumer age affects response to sensory characteristics of a cherry flavored beverage. Journal of Food Science, 60, 364–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piqueras-Fiszman, B., Alcaide, J., Roura, E., and Spence, C. (2012). Is it the plate or is it the food? Assessing the influence of the color (black or white) and shape of the plate on the perception of the food placed on it. Food Quality and Preference, 24, 205–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piqueras-Fiszman, B., Giboreau, A., and Spence, C. (2013). Assessing the influence of the color/finish of the plate on the perception of the food in a test in a restaurant setting. Flavour, 2(24).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piqueras-Fiszman, B., and Spence, C. (2012a). Sensory incongruity in the food and beverage sector: art, science, and commercialization. Petits Propos Culinaires, 95, 74118.Google Scholar
Piqueras-Fiszman, B., and Spence, C. (2012b). Does the color of the cup influence the consumer’s perception of a hot beverage? Journal of Sensory Studies, 27, 324–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piqueras-Fiszman, B., and Spence, C. (2014). Color, pleasantness, and consumption behaviour within a meal. Appetite, 75, 165–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., and Klein, R. M. (1976). Visual dominance: an information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychological Review, 83, 157–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raudenbush, B., Meyer, B., Eppich, W., Corley, N., and Petterson, S. (2002). Ratings of pleasantness and intensity for beverages served in containers congruent and incongruent with expectancy. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94, 671–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reardon, P., and Bushnell, E. W. (1988). Infants’ sensitivity to arbitrary pairings of color and taste. Infant Behavior and Development, 11, 245–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohm, H., Strobl, M., and Jaros, D. (1997). Butter color affects sensory perception of spreadability. Die Zeitschrift für Lebensmittel-Untersuchung und -Forschung, 205, 108–10.Google Scholar
Roth, H. A., Radle, L. J., Gifford, S. R., and Clydesdale, F. M. (1988). Psychophysical relationships between perceived sweetness and color in lemon- and lime-flavored drinks. Journal of Food Science, 53, 1116–19, 1162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P. (1982). “Taste-smell confusions” and the duality of the olfactory sense. Perception and Psychophysics, 31, 397401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sakai, N. (2011). Tasting with eyes. i-Perception, 2 (8) (http://i-perception.perceptionweb.com/journal/I/article/ic945).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sauvageot, F., and Struillou, A. (1997). Effet d’une modification de la couleur des échantillons et de l’éclairage sur la flaveur de vins évaluée sur une échelle de similarité (Effect of the modification of wine color and lighting conditions on the perceived flavor of wine, as measured by a similarity scale). Science des Aliments, 17, 4567.Google Scholar
Schifferstein, H. N. J. (2001). Effects of product beliefs on product perception and liking. In Frewer, L., Risvik, E., and Schifferstein, H. (eds.), Food, People and Society: A European Perspective on Consumers’ Food Choices (pp. 7396). Berlin: Springer Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shankar, M. U., Levitan, C. A., Prescott, J., and Spence, C. (2009). The influence of color and label information on flavor perception. Chemosensory Perception, 2, 53–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shankar, M. U., Levitan, C., and Spence, C. (2010a). Grape expectations: the role of cognitive influences in color-flavor interactions. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 380–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shankar, M., Simons, C., Levitan, C., Shiv, B., McClure, S., and Spence, C. (2010b). An expectations-based approach to explaining the crossmodal influence of color on odor identification: the influence of temporal and spatial factors. Journal of Sensory Studies, 25, 791803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shankar, M., Simons, C., Shiv, B., Levitan, C., McClure, S., and Spence, C. (2010c). An expectations-based approach to explaining the influence of color on odor identification: the influence of degree of discrepancy. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72, 1981–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shankar, M., Simons, C., Shiv, B., McClure, S., and Spence, C. (2010d). An expectation-based approach to explaining the crossmodal influence of color on odor identification: the influence of expertise. Chemosensory Perception, 3, 167–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skrandies, W., and Reuther, N. (2008). Match and mismatch of taste, odor, and color is reflected by electrical activity in the human brain. Journal of Psychophysiology, 22, 175–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, C. (2010). The color of wine. I. The World of Fine Wine, 28, 122–9.Google Scholar
Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: a tutorial review. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73, 971–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, C. (2012). The development and decline of multisensory flavour perception. In Bremner, A. J., Lewkowicz, D., and Spence, C. (eds.), Multisensory Development (pp. 6387). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, C. (2013). Sound advice. Cocktail Lovers, Winter (6), 1819.Google Scholar
Spence, C., Levitan, C., Shankar, M. U., and Zampini, M. (2010). Does food color influence taste and flavor perception in humans? Chemosensory Perception, 3, 6884.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, C., and Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2012). The multisensory packaging of beverages. In Kontominas, M. G. (ed.), Food Packaging: Procedures, Management and Trends (pp. 187233). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Publishers.Google Scholar
Spence, C., and Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2014). The Perfect Meal: The Multisensory Science of Food and Dining. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, R. J. (2009). The Psychology of Flavour. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, R. J., and Boakes, R. A. (2004). Sweet and sour smells: learned synaesthesia between the senses of taste and smell. In Calvert, G. A., Spence, C., and Stein, B. E. (eds.), The Handbook of Multisensory Processing (pp. 6983). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, R. J., Boakes, R. A., and Prescott, J. (1998). Changes in odor sweetness resulting from implicit learning of a simultaneous odor-sweetness association: an example of learned synaesthesia. Learning and Motivation, 29, 113–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, R. J., Boakes, R. A., and Wilson, J. P. (2000). Counter-conditioning following human odor-taste and color-taste learning. Learning and Motivation, 31, 114–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, R. J., Rich, A., and Russell, A. (2012). The nature and origin of cross-modal associations to odours. Perception, 41, 606–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, P. C., and Goss, E. (2013). Plate shape and color interact to influence taste and quality judgments. Flavour, 2(27).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stillman, J. (1993). Color influences flavor identification in fruit-flavored beverages. Journal of Food Science, 58, 810–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strugnell, C. (1997). Color and its role in sweetness perception. Appetite, 28, 85.Google ScholarPubMed
Teerling, A. (1992). The color of taste. Chemical Senses, 17, 886.Google Scholar
Tepper, B. J. (1993). Effects of a slight color variation on consumer acceptance of orange juice. Journal of Sensory Studies, 8, 145–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tom, G., Barnett, T., Lew, W., and Selmants, J. (1987). Cueing the consumer: the role of salient cues in consumer perception. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 4(2), 23–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Triplett, T. (1994). Consumers show little taste for clear beverages. Marketing News, 28(11), 2, 11.Google Scholar
Trommershäuser, J., Landy, M. S., and Körding, K. P. (eds.) (2011). Sensory Cue Integration. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Urbányi, G. (1982). Investigation into the interaction of different properties in the course of sensory evaluation. I. The effect of color upon the evaluation of taste in fruit and vegetable products. Acta Alimentaria, 11, 233–43.Google Scholar
Van Doorn, G., Wuillemin, D., and Spence, C. (2014). Does the color of the mug influence the taste of the coffee? Flavour, 3, 10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wadhwani, R., and McMahon, D. J. (2012). Color of low-fat cheese influences flavor perception and consumer liking. Journal of Dairy Science, 95, 2336–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wan, X., Velasco, C., Michel, C., Mu, B., Woods, A. T., and Spence, C. (2014). Does the shape of the glass influence the crossmodal association between color and flavour? A cross-cultural comparison. Flavour, 3, 3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wan, X., Zhou, X., Mu, B., Du, D., Velasco, C., Michel, C., and Spence, C. (2014). Crossmodal expectations of tea color based on its flavour. Journal of Sensory Studies, 29, 285–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wansink, B., and Van Ittersum, K. (2012). Fast food restaurant lighting and music can reduce calorie intake and increase satisfaction. Psychological Reports: Human Resources and Marketing, 111(1), 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, E. (2013). We eat with our eyes: flavor perception strongly influenced by food color, says DDW (www.foodnavigator-usa.com/Science/We-eat-with-our-eyes-Flavor-perception-strongly-influenced-by-food-color-says-DDW).Google Scholar
Welch, R. B., and Warren, D. H. (1986). Intersensory interactions. In Boff, K. R., Kaufman, L., and Thomas, J. P. (eds.), Handbook of Perception and Performance, vol. I: Sensory Processes and Perception (pp. 2536). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wheatley, J. (1973). Putting color into marketing. Marketing, October, 24–9, 67.Google Scholar
Wilson, G. D., and Gregson, R. A. M. (1967). Effects of illumination on perceived intensity of acid tastes. Australian Journal of Psychology, 19, 6972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeomans, M., Chambers, L., Blumenthal, H., and Blake, A. (2008). The role of expectancy in sensory and hedonic evaluation: the case of smoked salmon ice-cream. Food Quality and Preference, 19, 565–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zampini, M., Sanabria, D., Phillips, N., and Spence, C. (2007). The multisensory perception of flavor: assessing the influence of color cues on flavor discrimination responses. Food Quality and Preference, 18, 975–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zampini, M., Wantling, E., Phillips, N., and Spence, C. (2008). Multisensory flavor perception: assessing the influence of fruit acids and color cues on the perception of fruit-flavored beverages. Food Quality and Preference, 19, 335–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zellner, D. A. (2013). Color-odor interactions: a review and model. Chemosensory Perception, 6, 155–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zellner, D. A., Bartoli, A. M., and Eckard, R. (1991). Influence of color on odor identification and liking ratings. American Journal of Psychology, 104, 547–61.Google ScholarPubMed
Zellner, D. A., and Durlach, P. (2002). What is refreshing? An investigation of the color and other sensory attributes of refreshing foods and beverages. Appetite, 39, 185–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zellner, D. A., and Durlach, P. (2003). Effect of color on expected and experienced refreshment, intensity, and liking of beverages. American Journal of Psychology, 116, 633–47.Google ScholarPubMed
Zellner, D. A., and Kautz, M. A. (1990). Color affects perceived odor intensity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 391–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Zellner, D. A., and Whitten, L. A. (1999). The effect of color intensity and appropriateness on color-induced odor enhancement. American Journal of Psychology, 112, 585604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×