Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T18:51:00.871Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of interactions between variation in dopaminergic genes, traumatic life events, and anomalous self-experiences on psychosis proneness: Results from a cross-sectional study in a nonclinical sample

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 November 2020

Dorota Frydecka*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Wroclaw Medical University, 50-367Wroclaw, Poland
Kamila Kotowicz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Wroclaw Medical University, 50-367Wroclaw, Poland
Łukasz Gawęda
Affiliation:
Experimental Psychopathology Lab, Institute of Psychology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
Katarzyna Prochwicz
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, 30-060Krakow, Poland
Joanna Kłosowska
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, 30-060Krakow, Poland
Joanna Rymaszewska
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Wroclaw Medical University, 50-367Wroclaw, Poland
Agnieszka Samochowiec
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Szczecin, 71-017Szczecin, Poland
Jerzy Samochowiec
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Pomeranian Medical University, 71-460Szczecin, Poland
Piotr Podwalski
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Pomeranian Medical University, 71-460Szczecin, Poland
Edyta Pawlak-Adamska
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Therapy, Laboratory of Immunopathology, Institute of Immunology and Experimental Therapy, Polish Academy of Sciences, 51-114Wroclaw, Poland
Elżbieta Szmida
Affiliation:
Department of Genetics, Wroclaw Medical University, 50-368Wroclaw, Poland
Andrzej Cechnicki
Affiliation:
Department of Community Psychiatry, Chair of Psychiatry, Medical College Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland
Błażej Misiak
Affiliation:
Department of Genetics, Wroclaw Medical University, 50-368Wroclaw, Poland
*
*Dorota Frydecka, E-mail: dfrydecka@gmail.com

Abstract

Background

There is a growing number of studies showing interactions between genetic polymorphisms associated with dopaminergic neurotransmission and traumatic life events (TLEs) on a risk of psychotic-like experiences (PLEs). Anomalous self-experiences (ASEs) have been associated both with TLEs as well as with PLEs. However, it remains unknown what is the role of ASEs in the complexity of gene–environment interactions on the emergence of PLEs.

Patients and methods

We included 445 young adults—university students from three big cities in Poland. We used the Traumatic Events Checklist to assess TLEs, the Inventory of Psychotic-Like anomalous self-experiences in order to measure ASEs, and the Prodromal Questionnaire (PQ16) to record the level of PLEs. The following gene polymorphisms, related to dopaminergic neurotransmission, were determined: the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) rs4680 polymorphism, the dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) rs6277 polymorphism, and the dopamine transporter 1 (DAT1) rs28363170 polymorphism.

Results

There was a significant effect of the interaction between the DAT1 polymorphism, a severity of ASEs, and a history of TLEs on the level of PLEs. Among the DAT1 10R/10R homozygotes with low level of ASEs, a severity of PLEs was significantly higher in individuals with a history of any TLEs. Higher scores of the PQ16 were associated with a greater severity of ASEs both in the DAT1 9R allele carriers and the DAT1 10R/10R homozygotes.

Conclusion

Our findings imply that genetic liability related to aberrant dopamine transport might impact the association between TLEs and PLEs in subjects with high levels of ASEs.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of European Psychiatry Association

Introduction

Anomalous self-experiences (ASEs) are among the first symptoms that appear in the prodrome, predicting the development of psychosis, and are common in people with schizophrenia [Reference Cicero, Neis, Klaunig and Trask1]. ASEs have a long history in psychopathology research dating back to Bleuler, who claimed that schizophrenia is primarily a disorder of the self [Reference Parnas2]. Current body of research suggests that disordered selfhood may be a central phenotypic feature of schizophrenia [Reference Parnas2,Reference Sass3]. Self-disorders present in different forms at varying magnitudes in the premorbid, prodromal, first episode, chronic, and recovery phases of the disorder [Reference Cicero, Klaunig, Trask and Neis4]. Moreover, they hyperaggregate not only in schizophrenia but also in its spectrum diagnoses, such as schizotypal disorder, nonorganic, and nonaffective psychoses, as well as among first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients [Reference Raballo, Saebye and Parnas5]. Interestingly, ASEs have been shown to be moderately to highly stable over time [Reference Svendsen, Oie, Moller, Nelson, Melle and Haug6,Reference Koren, Tzivoni, Schalit, Adres, Reznik and Apter7].

ASEs are disturbances in the subjective experience of the self. There are various types of ASEs, such as disturbed sense of presence (basic self-awareness), anomalous bodily experiences (perceived morphological change), anomalous cognitive processes (perceptualization of inner speech), self-demarcation (confusion between oneself and other people), and existential reorientation (self-reference), all of which are interrelated [Reference Parnas and Handest8]. It has been postulated that self-disturbances are manifested as diminished self-affection and hyperreflexivity in patients with schizophrenia [Reference Sass3]. Diminished self-affection refers to a disturbance in self-agency or a diminished sense of possession of one’s experiences or disturbance in the perception that one is acting out one’s behavior. Hyperreflexivity refers to excessive attention to self-processes that are normally experienced unconsciously [Reference Cicero, Klaunig, Trask and Neis4].

ASEs have been shown to positively correlate with psychotic-like experiences (PLEs) and self-consciousness while negatively correlate with self-report and self-concept clarity as well as self-esteem [Reference Cicero, Neis, Klaunig and Trask1]. Higher scores are obtained in people with positive schizotypy compared to negative schizotypy as well as in patients with schizophrenia compared with healthy controls [Reference Cicero, Neis, Klaunig and Trask1]. A prospective study on nonclinical help-seeking adolescents has shown that ASEs can serve as a clinical marker of risk for schizophrenia-spectrum disorder [Reference Koren, Tzivoni, Schalit, Adres, Reznik and Apter7]. Another study on nonpsychotic young adults has shown that a combination of high level of PLEs and self-disturbances may capture the highest risk of psychosis in the general population associated with cognitive biases characteristic for psychosis [Reference Gaweda, Pionke, Arciszewska, Prochwicz, Frydecka and Misiak9]. In a clinical sample, ASEs have been shown to predict the onset of schizophrenia-spectrum disorders within 5 years after hospitalization due to a nonpsychotic disorder [Reference Parnas, Raballo, Handest, Jansson, Vollmer-Larsen and Saebye10]. Moreover, a relationship between ASEs and the presence of attenuated positive symptoms, as well as with cognitive or cognitive-perceptive basic symptoms, was found in individuals at clinical high risk (CHR) for psychosis [Reference Raballo, Pappagallo, Dell' Erba, Lo Cascio, Patane and Gebhardt11]. Finally, ASEs were also associated with psychosocial functioning [Reference Nelson, Thompson and Yung12], social functioning [Reference Koren, Reznik, Adres, Scheyer, Apter and Steinberg13], and global functioning [Reference Raballo, Pappagallo, Dell' Erba, Lo Cascio, Patane and Gebhardt11,Reference Comparelli, Corigliano, De Carolis, Pucci, Angelone and Di Pietro14] in CHR samples.

The dopamine hypothesis is the longest standing theory of schizophrenia etiology, and increased striatal dopaminergic activity is the most frequently replicated pathophysiological correlate of psychotic symptoms [Reference Kesby, Eyles, McGrath and Scott15,Reference Nakata, Kanahara and Iyo16]. Studies investigating the way risk factors of psychosis affect the dopamine system, coupled with longitudinal studies during the prodrome, allow for a more refined understanding of what leads to the onset of psychotic disorders. Various studies have suggested that dopaminergic abnormalities exist in CHR individuals and include elevated peripheral dopamine metabolites [Reference Sumiyoshi, Kurachi, Kurokawa, Yotsutsuji, Uehara and Itoh17] or differences in dopaminergic receptor densities [Reference Suridjan, Rusjan, Addington, Wilson, Houle and Mizrahi18]. It has been shown that dopamine synthesis capacity is raised in at-risk individuals [Reference Howes, Montgomery, Asselin, Murray, Valli and Tabraham19,Reference Egerton, Chaddock, Winton-Brown, Bloomfield, Bhattacharyya and Allen20], and it is positively associated with the severity of prodromal symptoms [Reference Howes, Bose, Turkheimer, Valli, Egerton and Valmaggia21]. Moreover, antipsychotic treatment trials have demonstrated efficacy of dopamine blockage in reducing prodromal symptoms severity [Reference McGlashan, Zipursky, Perkins, Addington, Miller and Woods22,Reference Ruhrmann, Bechdolf, Kuhn, Wagner, Schultze-Lutter and Janssen23]. Numerous studies have pointed that aberrant dopamine release might be associated with expression of genes-encoding proteins involved in dopamine synthesis, metabolism, neurotransmission, and signaling [Reference Edwards, Bacanu, Bigdeli, Moscati and Kendler24].

Meta-analytic studies point to the association between traumatic life events (TLEs) and the risk of psychosis [Reference Varese, Smeets, Drukker, Lieverse, Lataster and Viechtbauer25,Reference Matheson, Shepherd, Pinchbeck, Laurens and Carr26] and transition to psychosis among CHR individuals [Reference Peh, Rapisarda and Lee27]. Stress has also been identified as a marker for the occurrence of subclinical psychosis in a large-scale female twin study [Reference Lataster, Myin-Germeys, Derom, Thiery and van Os28]. Moreover, it has been shown that TLEs have an impact on dopaminergic neurotransmission. Greater striatal dopamine release in response to social stress has been observed both in individuals at risk of psychosis [Reference Mizrahi, Kenk, Suridjan, Boileau, George and McKenzie29,Reference Soliman, O'Driscoll, Pruessner, Holahan, Boileau and Gagnon30] and those with schizophrenia [Reference Mizrahi, Addington, Rusjan, Suridjan, Ng and Boileau31]. Interestingly, greater dopamine synthesis capacity [Reference Egerton, Valmaggia, Howes, Day, Chaddock and Allen32] and release [Reference Pruessner, Champagne, Meaney and Dagher33] have been found among adult individuals exposed to childhood adversity.

“Gene–environment studies have demonstrated interactions between genetic and psychosocial risk factors for psychosis [Reference van Os, Rutten and Poulton34]. There are several studies showing how genes associated with dopaminergic neurotransmission mediate the relationship between TLEs, dopamine, and psychosis (for review, see [Reference Howes, McCutcheon, Owen and Murray35]). There are also studies showing the relationship between dopamine transmission and ASEs as well between ASEs and PLEs. Moreover, the impact of TLEs on disruptions of sense of self and agency has been discussed previously [Reference Waite, Knight and Lee36]. However, there are no studies looking at the between TLEs, dopamine, and psychosis proneness while taking into account the severity of ASEs. Therefore, in this study, we aimed to test the effect of interaction between variation in genes playing role in dopaminergic neurotransmission (the COMT rs4680, the DRD2 rs6277, and the DAT1 rs28363170 variable number tandem repeat (VNTR) and TLEs on a risk of PLEs, taking into account the impact of ASEs, in a nonclinical sample.”

Methods

Participants

A detailed description of methodological issues related to our study was published in our previous article [Reference Kotowicz, Frydecka, Gaweda, Prochwicz, Klosowska and Rymaszewska37]. We included 535 undergraduate and postgraduate university students from three big cities in Poland (Wroclaw, Krakow, and Szczecin). A detailed description of sampling procedures was provided in our previous articles [Reference Gaweda, Pionke, Arciszewska, Prochwicz, Frydecka and Misiak9,Reference Kotowicz, Frydecka, Gaweda, Prochwicz, Klosowska and Rymaszewska37,Reference Gaweda, Prochwicz, Adamczyk, Frydecka, Misiak and Kotowicz38]. All participants were of Caucasian origin. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee at Wroclaw Medical University. All participants gave written informed consent.

Measures

The Traumatic Events Checklist

We used the Traumatic Events Checklist to assess TLEs [Reference Nijenhuis, Van der Hart and Kruger39]. It is a self-report questionnaire including 29 items. It captures six types of traumatic events. Emotional abuse is related to being belittled, teased, called names, threatened verbally, or unjustly punished by parents, brothers, and sisters. Emotional neglect is related to being left alone or insufficient affection given by parents. Physical abuse is related to being hit, tortured, or wounded by parents, brothers, or sisters. Threat to life refers to situations of threat to life from another person, such as during a crime. Sexual harassment refers to acts of a sexual nature that do not involve physical contact by parents, brothers, or sisters. Sexual abuse is defined as unwanted sexual acts involving physical contact by parents, brothers, or sisters.

The Inventory of Psychotic-Like Anomalous Self-Experiences

We used the Inventory of Psychotic-Like Anomalous Self-Experiences (IPASE) in order to measure self-disorders. The IPASE is a brief self-report alternative to phenomenological interviews that can be used in the general population, at-risk, and clinical samples to assess minimal self-disturbances [Reference Cicero, Neis, Klaunig and Trask1]. It has been developed based on a comprehensive phenomenological interview for self-disorders—the Examination of Anomalous Self Experience [Reference Parnas, Moller, Kircher, Thalbitzer, Jansson and Handest40]. The IPASE consists of 57 items that can be grouped into five factors [Reference Cicero, Neis, Klaunig and Trask1]. The factor cognition consists of items related to difficulties with thought processes. The second factor, self-awareness, and presence contain items related to loss of basic self or identity and a loss of a connection to the world. The third factor, consciousness, includes items about disturbances in intentionality and difficulty with distinguishing between imagination and reality. The fourth factor, somatization, includes items involving disturbances in bodily experiences, such as feeling like the body was changing shape or difficult to control, as well as thought of not feeling present either psychically or physically within one’s own body. The fifth factor, demarcation/transitivism, consists of items related to a disintegrating boundary between the self and the world or a feeling of nonexistence [Reference Cicero, Neis, Klaunig and Trask1]. The IPASE provides a shorter and more easily administered alternative to phenomenological interviews [Reference Cicero, Neis, Klaunig and Trask1]. The IPASE scores are reliable and valid in a general sample of people with high levels of schizotypy and at risk for the future development of psychosis as well as in individuals with schizophrenia [Reference Cicero, Neis, Klaunig and Trask1].

The Prodromal Questionnaire 16

We used the 16-item Prodromal Questionnaire (PQ16) to screen for PLEs [Reference Ising, Veling, Loewy, Rietveld, Rietdijk and Dragt41]. It is a 16-item self-report questionnaire that consists of nine items of the perceptual abnormalities/hallucinations subscale, five items referring to unusual thought content/delusional ideas/paranoia, and two negative symptoms. The perceptual aberrations are overrepresented since perceptual aberrations are primary psychotic phenomena, and that delusions are explanations of these abnormal sensory experiences. Perceptual aberrations are seen as a first phenomenon leading to psychosis. We excluded items one and seven from data analysis because they do not capture positive PLEs. Our analysis was limited to a number of various PLEs experienced by participants.

Genotyping

DNA samples were obtained via buccal swabs. The single nucleotide polymorphisms: COMT rs4680 (Val158Met) and the DRD2 rs6277 (957C>T) were genotyped with the allelic discrimination technique with the use of validated and predesigned TaqMan®SNP Genotyping Assays (C__25746809_50 and C__11339240_10) according to the manufacturer’s instructions (ThermoFisher Scientific, Inc., USA). DAT1 VNTR rs28363170 polymorphism was genotyped through PCR with pair of primer, where the forward one was labeled with 6-FAM followed by capillary electrophoresis in the presence of the GeneScan™ 600 LIZ® Size Standard on 3500 Genetic Analyzer (Applied Biosystems). Individual genotypes were called according to peak size on GeneMapper® Software version 4.0., being the 9-allele (440 bp) and the 10-allele (480 bp).

Statistics

Agreement of genotypes distribution with the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) was assessed by comparing observed and expected distributions using the χ 2 test. Bivariate comparisons were performed using the Mann–Whitney U test. The Spearman rank correlation coefficients were used to assess correlations. Multiple linear regression was performed to test the effects of variation in dopaminergic genes and a history of any TLEs on the number of PLEs. Main effects were represented by a history of any TLEs, the allele status (COMT: the Met allele carriers vs. the Val/Val homozygotes; DRD2: the C allele carriers vs. the TT homozygotes; DAT1: the 9R allele carriers vs. the 10R/10R homozygotes), and the level of ASEs. The level of ASEs was dichotomized to high level (individuals scored above the mean score of the IPASE) and low level (individuals scored below the mean score of the IPASE). Age and sex were added as covariates. Indeed, the onset of psychosis usually appears in the adolescence or early adulthood [Reference Immonen, Jaaskelainen, Korpela and Miettunen42]. In turn, sex differences might impact various clinical characteristics of individuals at clinical high risk of psychosis [Reference Barajas, Ochoa, Obiols and Lalucat-Jo43]. Firstly, we tested the effects of covariates (the first block of independent variables, model 1). Next, the main effects were implemented (the second block of independent variables, model 2). The third block of independent variables (model 3) included the two-way interactions (allele status × TLEs, allele status × ASEs, and TLEs × ASEs). Finally, we inputted the three-way interaction term (allele status × TLEs × ASEs, model 4). Posthoc analyses were based on bivariate comparisons using the Mann–Whitney U test. Results were considered significant if the p-value was less than 0.05. Statistical analysis was performed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences, version 20 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, Illinois, USA).

Results

General characteristics of participants were presented in Table 1. Distribution of the COMT rs4680 and the DAT1 VNTR 9R/10R genotypes was in agreement with the HWE (COMT: χ 2 = 0.07, p = 0.791; DAT1 9R/10R VNTR: χ 2 = 1.78, p = 0.183). However, the distribution of the DRD2 genotypes did not follow the HWE (χ 2 = 6.31, p = 0.012). As expected, a history of TLEs was associated with significantly higher scores of the IPASE (Table 2). There were significant positive correlations between the IPASE scores and the PQ16 score (Table 3).

Table 1. General characteristics of the sample.

Abbreviations: ASE, anomalous self-experience; COMT, catechol-O-methyltransferase; DAT1, dopamine transporter 1; DRD2, dopamine D2 receptor; PQ-16, Prodromal Questionnaire 16; TEC, Traumatic Events Checklist; TLEs, traumatic life events; SD, standard deviation.

Table 2. The IPASE scores with respect to a history of TLEs.

Abbreviations: IPASE, inventory of psychotic-like anomalous self-experiences; PQ-16, Prodromal Questionnaire 16; TLEs, traumatic life events.

Data expressed as mean ± SD.

p < 0.001 for all comparisons.

Table 3. Correlations between the IPASE scores and the PQ16 score.

Abbreviations: IPASE, inventory of psychotic-like anomalous self-experiences; PQ-16, Prodromal Questionnaire 16.

Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients.

p < 0.001 for all correlations.

Results of multiple linear regression were presented in Table 4. There were significant main effects of age and a severity of ASEs on the PQ16 scores in all models. The effect of a history of TLEs on the PQ16 score was significant only in the model 2 for all tested gene polymorphisms. No significant main effects of gene polymorphisms on the PQ16 scores were found. However, there was a significant effect of the interaction between the DAT1 9R/10R VNTR and a severity of ASEs on the PQ16 score (model 4). Effects of the interaction between a history of TLEs and the level of ASEs on the PQ16 score were significant in the models 3 and 4 for the COMT rs4680 and the DRD2 rs6277 polymorphisms. Similarly, the effects of a three-way interaction (the DAT1 9R/10R allele status × TLEs × ASEs) on the PQ16 score were also significant. The model 4 that included the DAT1 9R/10R allele status explained the largest proportion of variance in the PQ16 score (R 2 = 0.355).

Table 4. Results of multiple linear regression analysis.

Abbreviations: ASEs, anomalous self-experiences; COMT, catechol-O-methyltransferase; DAT1, dopamine transporter 1; TLEs, traumatic life events; DRD2, dopamine D2 receptor.

Significant effects (p < 0.05) were marked with bold characters.

* Significant R 2 changes (p < 0.05).

Results of posthoc analyses were shown in Figure 1 and Supplementary Material. Higher scores of the PQ16 were associated with a greater severity of ASEs both in the DAT1 9R allele carriers and the DAT1 10R/10R homozygotes (p < 0.001,Figure 1 and Supplementary Table 1). In participants with a high level of ASEs, a history of TLEs was associated with significantly higher scores of PQ16 in the DAT1 10R/10R homozygotes (8.36 ± 6.06 vs. 4.47 ± 3.34, p = 0.015) but not in the DAT1 9R allele carriers (Figure 1 and Supplementary Table 2). These results were not significant in the group with low levels of ASEs.

Figure 1. Effects of the interactions between the dopamine transporter 1 (DAT1) 9R/10R polymorphism, a history of traumatic life events (TLEs), and a severity of anomalous self-experiences (ASEs) on the score of the Prodromal Questionnaire 16 (PQ16). (A) Effects of the interaction between the DAT1 9R/10R polymorphism and the level of ASEs on the PQ16 score. (B) Effects of the interaction between the DAT1 9R/10R polymorphism and a history of TLEs on the PQ16 score in individuals with low level of ASEs. (C) Effects of the interaction between the DAT1 9R/10R polymorphism and a history of TLEs on the PQ16 score in individuals with high level of ASEs. Error bars represent 95% confidence interval (CI).

Model 4 for DAT1 explained the largest proportion of variance in the PQ16 score measured by the R 2 statistic (Table 4). Adding a three-way interaction term significantly increased the R 2 statistic. In turn, adding a three-way interaction term to the models with the DRD2 and COMT polymorphisms did not change the R 2 statistic significantly.

Discussion

In our previous articles, we have shown that a path from trauma to psychosis proneness leads via cognitive biases and self-disorders [Reference Gaweda, Prochwicz, Adamczyk, Frydecka, Misiak and Kotowicz38] and that the combination of high level of PLEs together with self-disturbances captures the highest risk of psychosis in the general population [Reference Gaweda, Pionke, Arciszewska, Prochwicz, Frydecka and Misiak9]. Our main finding from genetic studies so far was that among participants with high levels of cognitive biases, there is an interaction between COMT gene polymorphism and a history of TLEs on the severity of PLEs [Reference Kotowicz, Frydecka, Gaweda, Prochwicz, Klosowska and Rymaszewska37]. In our current study, we wanted to see whether not only the level of cognitive biases but also the level of self-disorders influence the interaction between genetic polymorphisms in the genes influencing dopaminergic neurotransmission (COMT, DRD2, and DAT1) and a history of TLEs on the severity of PLEs.

Findings from our study imply that TLEs might increase severity of PLEs only in 10R/10R homozygotes of the DAT1 9R/10R VNTR polymorphism. DAT1 is a protein regulating dopamine synaptic levels by limiting dopamine receptor activation and facilitating neuronal dopamine reuptake. Several DAT1 gene polymorphisms have been described, among these, there is a 40 bp VNTR that most commonly occurs as 9 or 10 tandem repeats (R). There is evidence that this polymorphism is functional, altering gene expression. According to current models, reduced DAT1 expression should amplify phasic DA signals [Reference Cragg and Rice44]. It has been found that healthy individuals with the DAT1 10R/10R genotype show lower striatal transporter binding [Reference Jacobsen, Staley, Zoghbi, Seibyl, Kosten and Innis45], while the DAT1 9R allele carriers present with increased striatal dopamine transporter availability [Reference van Dyck, Malison, Jacobsen, Seibyl, Staley and Laruelle46,Reference van de Giessen, de Win, Tanck, van den Brink, Baas and Booij47]. A recent meta-analysis confirmed that the DAT1 9R/10R VNTR polymorphism regulates dopamine activity in the striatal brain regions independent of the presence of neuropsychiatric illnesses [Reference Faraone, Spencer, Madras, Zhang-James and Biederman48]. Thus, it can be hypothesized that additionally to increased striatal dopamine activity associated with the DAT1 gene polymorphism, exposure to TLEs further increases dopaminergic neurotransmission, leading to increased risk of PLEs. This is in line with schizophrenia risk model which requires two hits in order to generate the clinical phenotype. The first hit is associated with early priming in a genetically predisposed individuals, while the second hit is usually an environmental insult occurring at certain neurodevelopmental stages [Reference Davis, Eyre, Jacka, Dodd, Dean and McEwen49]. Although neither population-based association study [Reference Joober, Toulouse, Benkelfat, Lal, Bloom and Labelle50] nor family-based association study [Reference Georgieva, Dimitrova, Nikolov, Koleva, Tsvetkova and Owen51] showed the significance of the DAT1 9R/10R VNTR polymorphism in the development of schizophrenia, these studies did not control for the effects of TLEs. Partially, the interaction between TLEs and the DAT1 polymorphism could be due to the fact that genetic variants in dopaminergic genes are associated with considerable interindividual variation in the ability to acquire and extinguish conditioned fear reactions [Reference Lonsdorf and Kalisch52]. In particular, it has been shown that higher learning rates during fear extinction are observed in the DAT1 9R-carriers as compared to noncarriers [Reference Raczka, Mechias, Gartmann, Reif, Deckert and Pessiglione53].

There are few studies so far showing that the DAT1 polymorphisms are associated with negative experiences in the childhood and negative parenting. A study on male and female twins showed that men with the 9R/9R genotype report less sexual abuse experiences in comparison to other genotypes, while in women, there was an association between the 9R/9R genotype with less experiences of emotional abuse in comparison to other genotypes [Reference Rehan, Antfolk, Johansson, Aminoff, Sandnabba and Westberg54]. These results suggest that either some experiences of childhood abuse may be genetically influenced, or children carrying the specific genotype have temperamental traits that make them more vulnerable to abuse. Another scenario is that children inherit from their parent the genotype that is associated with certain behavior (e.g., antisocial behavior). On the other hand, the association between maternal DAT1 9R/10R genotype and negative parenting has been found and appeared to be significantly stronger among mothers whose children were highly disruptive and noncompliant during the mother–child interactions, suggesting gene–environment interaction [Reference Lee, Chronis-Tuscano, Keenan, Pelham, Loney and Van Hulle55].

There are also mixed findings from studies showing an interaction between negative experiences and psychiatric symptomatology in nonpsychotic disorders. There is evidence that an interaction between the DAT1 VNTR polymorphism and TLEs has an impact on symptomatology in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) patients. The authors showed that adolescents being homozygous for the 10R of the DAT1 VNTR polymorphism who grew up in greater psychosocial adversity exhibit significantly more ADHD symptoms in comparison to adolescents with other genotypes or who lived in less adverse family environments [Reference Laucht, Skowronek, Becker, Schmidt, Esser and Schulze56]. On the other hand, it has been shown that children with 10R/10R genotype have lower level of conduct problems under the condition of low level of positive maternal emotions than those patients with the 9R/9R and 10R/9R genotypes [Reference Sonuga-Barke, Oades, Psychogiou, Chen, Franke and Buitelaar57]. The DAT1 gene 9R variant has been associated with child negative affect expressed toward the parent during parent–child interactions, and parents of children with the 9R allele have been found to exhibit more hostility and lower guidance/engagement than parents of children without the 9R allele [Reference Hayden, Hanna, Sheikh, Laptook, Kim and Singh58]. Moreover, the DAT1 single nucleotide polymorphism (rs40184) has been shown to moderate the effect of perceived maternal rejection on the onset of major depressive disorder and suicidal ideation in male adolescents [Reference Haeffel, Getchell, Koposov, Yrigollen, Deyoung and Klinteberg59].

Our further analysis revealed that the interaction between TLEs and the DAT1 polymorphism appears only in individuals with high levels of ASEs. Although there are no studies so far that directly associate dopaminergic neurotransmission with ASEs, it has been shown that self-awareness is regulated by dopamine [Reference Lou, Skewes, Thomsen, Overgaard, Lau and Mouridsen60,Reference Joensson, Thomsen, Andersen, Gross, Mouridsen and Sandberg61] via the GABA system through the medial prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex [Reference Lou, Rosenstand, Brooks, Bender, Jakobsen and Blicher62] (for review, see [Reference Lou, Changeux and Rosenstand63]). The current model of ASEs is based on a combination of empirical findings, clinical experience, and phenomenological considerations; however, there is a growing body of research trying to link phenomenology with neurobiology despite technical and conceptual challenges (for review, see [Reference Nelson, Fornito, Harrison, Yucel, Sass and Yung64]). A meta-analytic study showed that the cortical midline system may be responsible for the self-referential processing—basic sense of self—and changes in this region are related to psychotic symptoms as well as they are among the earliest alterations observed in schizophrenia [Reference Northoff, Heinzel, de Greck, Bermpohl, Dobrowolny and Panksepp65]. Aberrant dopamine transmission is thought to disturb the salience of external and internal stimuli, and self-disturbances and psychotic symptoms may arise as patients experience these abnormally salient stimuli [Reference Kapur66]. On the other hand, aberrant salience may result in diminished self-affection and hyperreflexivity, contributing to disturbed basic selfhood [Reference Nelson, Fornito, Harrison, Yucel, Sass and Yung64].

Various limitations of this study need to be taken into consideration. Firstly, our sample was not large, and replication of findings in an independent sample was not performed. Secondly, a cross-sectional study design does not provide insights into causal associations. Finally, the use of a nonclinical sample can also be perceived as potential limitation and does not allow for generalization of findings over patients with psychotic disorders. However, this approach is characterized by a lack of confounding factors related to clinical symptoms, cognitive impairments, comorbidities, and medication effects. Finally, the use of self-reports has been raised as a limitation. However, there is evidence that assessment with self-reports is characterized by high validity and reporting stability.

In summary, our results imply that the effect of interaction between the DAT1 VNTR polymorphism and TLEs on the level of PLEs is limited to individuals with high levels of ASEs. These results provide novel insights into the complexity of gene– environment interactions by showing that simple models might be insufficient in addressing the complexity of how genetic predisposition together with environmental stress might lead to subclinical psychotic experiences. Given that assessment of ASEs provides a unified approach to understand highly diverse symptomatology of schizophrenia, longitudinal and neurobiological studies investigating their basis can unravel new pathways of psychosis development.

Acknowledgment

Authors are deeply grateful to all participants of this study.

Financial Support

This study was supported by the statutory project funded by the Uniwersytet Medyczny im. Piastów Slaskich we Wroclawiu, Wroclaw, Poland (task number: ST.C230.15.074) and the programme provided by the Polish Ministry of Science and Higher Education “Regional Initiative of Excellence” for the years 2019–2022 (002/RID/2018/19).

Conflict of Interest

Authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings will be available in upon request from the corresponding author.

Supplementary Materials

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2020.103.

References

Cicero, DC, Neis, AM, Klaunig, MJ, Trask, CL. The Inventory of Psychotic-Like Anomalous Self-Experiences (IPASE): development and validation. Psychol Assess. 2017;29:1325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parnas, J. A disappearing heritage: the clinical core of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37:11211130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sass, LA. Self-disturbance and schizophrenia: structure, specificity, pathogenesis (current issues, new directions). Schizophr Res. 2014;152:511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicero, DC, Klaunig, MJ, Trask, CL, Neis, AM. Anomalous self-experiences and positive symptoms are independently associated with emotion processing deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2016;176:456461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raballo, A, Saebye, D, Parnas, J. Looking at the schizophrenia spectrum through the prism of self-disorders: an empirical study. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37:344351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svendsen, IH, Oie, MG, Moller, P, Nelson, B, Melle, I, Haug, E. Stability in basic self-disturbances and diagnosis in a first treated psychosis: a 7 year follow-up study. Schizophr Res. 2018;202:274280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koren, D, Tzivoni, Y, Schalit, L, Adres, M, Reznik, N, Apter, A, et al. Basic self-disorders in adolescence predict schizophrenia spectrum disorders in young adulthood: a 7-year follow-up study among non-psychotic help-seeking adolescents. Schizophr Res. 2019;216:97103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parnas, J, Handest, P. Phenomenology of anomalous self-experience in early schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry. 2003;44:121134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaweda, L, Pionke, R, Arciszewska, A, Prochwicz, K, Frydecka, D, Misiak, B, et al. A combination of self-disturbances and psychotic-like experiences. A cluster analysis study on a non clinical sample in Poland. Psychiatry Res. 2019;273:394401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parnas, J, Raballo, A, Handest, P, Jansson, L, Vollmer-Larsen, A, Saebye, D. Self-experience in the early phases of schizophrenia: 5-year follow-up of the Copenhagen Prodromal Study. World Psychiatry. 2011;10:200204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raballo, A, Pappagallo, E, Dell' Erba, A, Lo Cascio, N, Patane, M, Gebhardt, E, et al. Self-disorders and clinical high risk for psychosis: an empirical study in help-seeking youth attending community mental health facilities. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42:926932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, B, Thompson, A, Yung, AR. Basic self-disturbance predicts psychosis onset in the ultra high risk for psychosis “prodromal” population. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:12771287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koren, D, Reznik, N, Adres, M, Scheyer, R, Apter, A, Steinberg, T, et al. Disturbances of basic self and prodromal symptoms among non psychotic help-seeking adolescents. Psychol Med. 2013;43:13651376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Comparelli, A, Corigliano, V, De Carolis, A, Pucci, D, Angelone, M, Di Pietro, S, et al. Anomalous self-experiences and their relationship with symptoms, neuro-cognition, and functioning in at-risk adolescents and young adults. Compr Psychiatry. 2016;65:4449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kesby, JP, Eyles, DW, McGrath, JJ, Scott, JG. Dopamine, psychosis and schizophrenia: the widening gap between basic and clinical neuroscience. Transl Psychiatry. 2018;8:30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakata, Y, Kanahara, N, Iyo, M. Dopamine supersensitivity psychosis in schizophrenia: concepts and implications in clinical practice. J Psychopharmacol. 2017;31:15111518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sumiyoshi, T, Kurachi, M, Kurokawa, K, Yotsutsuji, T, Uehara, T, Itoh, H, et al. Plasma homovanillic acid in the prodromal phase of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47:428433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suridjan, I, Rusjan, P, Addington, J, Wilson, AA, Houle, S, Mizrahi, R. Dopamine D2 and D3 binding in people at clinical high risk for schizophrenia, antipsychotic-naive patients and healthy controls while performing a cognitive task. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2013;38:98106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, OD, Montgomery, AJ, Asselin, MC, Murray, RM, Valli, I, Tabraham, P, et al. Elevated striatal dopamine function linked to prodromal signs of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:1320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egerton, A, Chaddock, CA, Winton-Brown, TT, Bloomfield, MA, Bhattacharyya, S, Allen, P, et al. Presynaptic striatal dopamine dysfunction in people at ultra-high risk for psychosis: findings in a second cohort. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74:106112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, OD, Bose, SK, Turkheimer, F, Valli, I, Egerton, A, Valmaggia, LR, et al. Dopamine synthesis capacity before onset of psychosis: a prospective [18F]-DOPA PET imaging study. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168:13111317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGlashan, TH, Zipursky, RB, Perkins, D, Addington, J, Miller, T, Woods, SW, et al. Randomized, double-blind trial of olanzapine versus placebo in patients prodromally symptomatic for psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:790799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruhrmann, S, Bechdolf, A, Kuhn, KU, Wagner, M, Schultze-Lutter, F, Janssen, B, et al. Acute effects of treatment for prodromal symptoms for people putatively in a late initial prodromal state of psychosis. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 2007;51:s88s95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, AC, Bacanu, SA, Bigdeli, TB, Moscati, A, Kendler, KS. Evaluating the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia in a large-scale genome-wide association study. Schizophr Res. 2016;176:136140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varese, F, Smeets, F, Drukker, M, Lieverse, R, Lataster, T, Viechtbauer, W, et al. Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: a meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective-and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:661671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matheson, SL, Shepherd, AM, Pinchbeck, RM, Laurens, KR, Carr, VJ. Childhood adversity in schizophrenia: a systematic meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2013;43:225238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peh, OH, Rapisarda, A, Lee, J. Childhood adversities in people at Ultra-High Risk (UHR) for psychosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2019;49:10891101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lataster, T, Myin-Germeys, I, Derom, C, Thiery, E, van Os, J. Evidence that self-reported psychotic experiences represent the transitory developmental expression of genetic liability to psychosis in the general population. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;150B:1078–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mizrahi, R, Kenk, M, Suridjan, I, Boileau, I, George, TP, McKenzie, K, et al. Stress-induced dopamine response in subjects at clinical high risk for schizophrenia with and without concurrent cannabis use. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:14791489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soliman, A, O'Driscoll, GA, Pruessner, J, Holahan, AL, Boileau, I, Gagnon, D, et al. Stress-induced dopamine release in humans at risk of psychosis: a [11C]raclopride PET study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:20332041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mizrahi, R, Addington, J, Rusjan, PM, Suridjan, I, Ng, A, Boileau, I, et al. Increased stress-induced dopamine release in psychosis. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:561567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egerton, A, Valmaggia, LR, Howes, OD, Day, F, Chaddock, CA, Allen, P, et al. Adversity in childhood linked to elevated striatal dopamine function in adulthood. Schizophr Res. 2016;176:171176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, JC, Champagne, F, Meaney, MJ, Dagher, A. Dopamine release in response to a psychological stress in humans and its relationship to early life maternal care: a positron emission tomography study using [11C]raclopride. J Neurosci. 2004;24:28252831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Os, J, Rutten, BP, Poulton, R. Gene–environment interactions in schizophrenia: review of epidemiological findings and future directions. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:1066–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, OD, McCutcheon, R, Owen, MJ, Murray, RM. The role of genes, stress, and dopamine in the development of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;81:920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waite, F, Knight, MT, Lee, D. Self-compassion and self-criticism in recovery in psychosis: an interpretative phenomenological analysis study. J Clin Psychol. 2015;71:12011217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotowicz, K, Frydecka, D, Gaweda, L, Prochwicz, K, Klosowska, J, Rymaszewska, J, et al. Effects of traumatic life events, cognitive biases and variation in dopaminergic genes on psychosis proneness. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaweda, L, Prochwicz, K, Adamczyk, P, Frydecka, D, Misiak, B, Kotowicz, K, et al. The role of self-disturbances and cognitive biases in the relationship between traumatic life events and psychosis proneness in a non-clinical sample. Schizophr Res. 2018;193:218224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nijenhuis, ERS, Van der Hart, O, Kruger, K. The psychometric characteristics of the Traumatic Experiences Questionnaire (TEC): First findings among psychiatric outpatients. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy. 2002;9(3):200–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parnas, J, Moller, P, Kircher, T, Thalbitzer, J, Jansson, L, Handest, P, et al. EASE: examination of anomalous self-experience. Psychopathology. 2005;38:236258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ising, HK, Veling, W, Loewy, RL, Rietveld, MW, Rietdijk, J, Dragt, S, et al. The validity of the 16-item version of the Prodromal Questionnaire (PQ-16) to screen for ultra high risk of developing psychosis in the general help-seeking population. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:1288–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Immonen, J, Jaaskelainen, E, Korpela, H, Miettunen, J. Age at onset and the outcomes of schizophrenia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2017;11:453460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barajas, A, Ochoa, S, Obiols, JE, Lalucat-Jo, L. Gender differences in individuals at high-risk of psychosis: a comprehensive literature review. Sci World J. 2015;2015:430735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cragg, SJ, Rice, ME. DAncing past the DAT at a DA synapse. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27:270277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobsen, LK, Staley, JK, Zoghbi, SS, Seibyl, JP, Kosten, TR, Innis, RB, et al. Prediction of dopamine transporter binding availability by genotype: a preliminary report. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:17001703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Dyck, CH, Malison, RT, Jacobsen, LK, Seibyl, JP, Staley, JK, Laruelle, M, et al. Increased dopamine transporter availability associated with the 9-repeat allele of the SLC6A3 gene. J Nucl Med. 2005;46:745–51.Google ScholarPubMed
van de Giessen, E, de Win, MM, Tanck, MW, van den Brink, W, Baas, F, Booij, J. Striatal dopamine transporter availability associated with polymorphisms in the dopamine transporter gene SLC6A3. J Nucl Med. 2009;50:4552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faraone, SV, Spencer, TJ, Madras, BK, Zhang-James, Y, Biederman, J. Functional effects of dopamine transporter gene genotypes on in vivo dopamine transporter functioning: a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19:880889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, J, Eyre, H, Jacka, FN, Dodd, S, Dean, O, McEwen, S, et al. A review of vulnerability and risks for schizophrenia: beyond the two hit hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;65:185194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joober, R, Toulouse, A, Benkelfat, C, Lal, S, Bloom, D, Labelle, A, et al. DRD3 and DAT1 genes in schizophrenia: an association study. J Psychiatr Res. 2000;34:285291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgieva, L, Dimitrova, A, Nikolov, I, Koleva, S, Tsvetkova, R, Owen, MJ, et al. Dopamine Transporter Gene (DAT1) VNTR polymorphism in major psychiatric disorders: family-based association study in the Bulgarian population. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2002;105:396399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lonsdorf, TB, Kalisch, R. A review on experimental and clinical genetic associations studies on fear conditioning, extinction and cognitive-behavioral treatment. Transl Psychiatry. 2011;1:e41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raczka, KA, Mechias, ML, Gartmann, N, Reif, A, Deckert, J, Pessiglione, M, et al. Empirical support for an involvement of the mesostriatal dopamine system in human fear extinction. Transl Psychiatry. 2011;1:e12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rehan, W, Antfolk, J, Johansson, A, Aminoff, M, Sandnabba, NK, Westberg, L, et al. Gene–environment correlation between the Dopamine Transporter Gene (DAT1) polymorphism and childhood experiences of abuse. J Interpers Violence. 2018;33:20592072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, SS, Chronis-Tuscano, A, Keenan, K, Pelham, WE, Loney, J, Van Hulle, CA, et al. Association of maternal dopamine transporter genotype with negative parenting: evidence for gene x environment interaction with child disruptive behavior. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15:548558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laucht, M, Skowronek, MH, Becker, K, Schmidt, MH, Esser, G, Schulze, TG, et al. Interacting effects of the dopamine transporter gene and psychosocial adversity on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms among 15-year-olds from a high-risk community sample. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:585590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sonuga-Barke, EJ, Oades, RD, Psychogiou, L, Chen, W, Franke, B, Buitelaar, J, et al. Dopamine and serotonin transporter genotypes moderate sensitivity to maternal expressed emotion: the case of conduct and emotional problems in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50:10521063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayden, EP, Hanna, B, Sheikh, HI, Laptook, RS, Kim, J, Singh, SM, et al. Child dopamine active transporter 1 genotype and parenting: evidence for evocative gene-environment correlations. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25:163173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haeffel, GJ, Getchell, M, Koposov, RA, Yrigollen, CM, Deyoung, CG, Klinteberg, BA, et al. Association between polymorphisms in the dopamine transporter gene and depression: evidence for a gene–environment interaction in a sample of juvenile detainees. Psychol Sci. 2008;19:6269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lou, HC, Skewes, JC, Thomsen, KR, Overgaard, M, Lau, HC, Mouridsen, K, et al. Dopaminergic stimulation enhances confidence and accuracy in seeing rapidly presented words. J Vis. 2011;11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joensson, M, Thomsen, KR, Andersen, LM, Gross, J, Mouridsen, K, Sandberg, K, et al. Making sense: dopamine activates conscious self-monitoring through medial prefrontal cortex. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:18661877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lou, HC, Rosenstand, A, Brooks, DJ, Bender, D, Jakobsen, S, Blicher, JU, et al. Exogenous dopamine reduces GABA receptor availability in the human brain. Brain Behav. 2016;6:e00484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lou, HC, Changeux, JP, Rosenstand, A. Towards a cognitive neuroscience of self-awareness. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;83:765773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, B, Fornito, A, Harrison, BJ, Yucel, M, Sass, LA, Yung, AR, et al. A disturbed sense of self in the psychosis prodrome: linking phenomenology and neurobiology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33:807817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northoff, G, Heinzel, A, de Greck, M, Bermpohl, F, Dobrowolny, H, Panksepp, J. Self-referential processing in our brain—a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage. 2006;31:440–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kapur, S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. General characteristics of the sample.

Figure 1

Table 2. The IPASE scores with respect to a history of TLEs.

Figure 2

Table 3. Correlations between the IPASE scores and the PQ16 score.

Figure 3

Table 4. Results of multiple linear regression analysis.

Figure 4

Figure 1. Effects of the interactions between the dopamine transporter 1 (DAT1) 9R/10R polymorphism, a history of traumatic life events (TLEs), and a severity of anomalous self-experiences (ASEs) on the score of the Prodromal Questionnaire 16 (PQ16). (A) Effects of the interaction between the DAT1 9R/10R polymorphism and the level of ASEs on the PQ16 score. (B) Effects of the interaction between the DAT1 9R/10R polymorphism and a history of TLEs on the PQ16 score in individuals with low level of ASEs. (C) Effects of the interaction between the DAT1 9R/10R polymorphism and a history of TLEs on the PQ16 score in individuals with high level of ASEs. Error bars represent 95% confidence interval (CI).

Supplementary material: File

Frydecka et al. supplementary material

Table S1

Download Frydecka et al. supplementary material(File)
File 14.5 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.