Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T09:28:46.852Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Language learning and study abroad

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 September 2018

Christina Isabelli-García
Affiliation:
Gonzaga University, Spokane, WA, USAisabelli@gonzaga.edu
Jennifer Bown
Affiliation:
Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, USAjennifer_bown@byu.edu
John L. Plews
Affiliation:
Saint Mary's University, Halifax, NS, CanadaJohn.Plews@smu.ca
Dan P. Dewey
Affiliation:
Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, USAddewey@byu.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The aim of this review is to synthesize empirical studies on undergraduate language learners’ experience abroad during a time period of a year or less. To help provide a framework to this synthesis, we begin our review by tracing the recent evolution of empirical mixed-method research on the learner, identifying problems and characteristics that language learners generally encounter in the study abroad (SA) experience. We take a closer look at variables related to individual difference such as anxiety, motivation, and attitudes to more recent views of learner identity in language learning. We highlight the shift to language learner agency, a topic that merits more discussion in SA literature. We then review how the SA learning environments are treated. This review takes a closer look at research informed by socially grounded theories. Finally, we review the role that SA plays in undergraduate language curricula, where the objectives of the experience are aligned with at-home (AH) curricula, a topic that has not been fully discussed in SA literature. The conclusions offer suggestions for keeping pace with the broader field of applied/educational linguistics.

Type
State-of-the-Art Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

1. Introduction

Generated, perhaps, by the priority of institutions of higher education (HE) worldwide to prepare students to be global citizens in the international community, ‘[t]he global population of students who move to another country to study continues to rise . . . with an annual increase of 10%’ (International Trends in Higher Education 2015: 5). The internationalization of HE has been underway since the Middle Ages in Europe (de Ridder-Symoens Reference de Ridder-Symoens1992), having paused in the eighteenth and nineteenth centuries (de Wit Reference de Wit and Lewin2009) and experiencing a rebirth after the two World Wars. Lewin (Reference Lewin and Lewin2009) traces the ideological history of SA during these time periods and he concludes that the current goals of SA go beyond language learning to the internationalization of students.

According to Knight (Reference Knight, Teferra and Knight2008), ‘“Internationalization” is not a new term. [It] has been used for years in political science and governmental relations, but its popularity in the education sector has really soared only since the early 1980s’ (p. 12). The term Internationalization has existed since the 1970s and ‘was defined in terms of activities such as SA, language studies, institutional agreements, and area students’ (p. 13). Internationalizing campuses is interpreted differently in various geographical areas. The International Association of Universities (IAU Reference Egron-Polak and Hudson2014: 9) states that regional difference exists worldwide with regards to the importance of internationalization and expected benefits:

Students’ increased international awareness is the top-ranked benefit of an international experience abroad in Asia, the Pacific and North America. In Europe and the Middle East, the top-ranked benefit is improved quality of teaching and learning. For African respondents, the top-ranked benefit is strengthened knowledge production capacity, and for Latin American and the Caribbean institutions, the highest-ranked benefit is increased networking of faculty and researchers.

The nature of SA, the desired development outcomes associated with it, and the research itself are shaped by broader realities and sociopolitical agendas. As Kinginger (Reference Kinginger2009) pointed out in her critical review of the research literature on SA, apprehending SA in its entirety is extremely difficult. In fact, even the terminology associated with SA poses challenges to scholars who seek to provide even a broad overview. Coleman (Reference Coleman1997) notes the following terms used in connection with the phenomenon: SA, residence abroad, student mobility, in-country study, overseas language immersion (IM), and academic migration. In addition, King & Raghuram (Reference King and Raghuram2013: 129) pose that the phenomenon of international student migration/mobility can be differentiated among other vectors as well, including the length of SA, the level of study (e.g., undergraduate vs. graduate), the participants themselves, and the experiences. Each of the above-mentioned terms describes particular types of study at foreign institutions, ranging from the short-term SA programs (four to 14 weeks) that are prevalent within the US, to long-term matriculation in a foreign institution, prevalent in countries such as Sri Lanka and Cyprus, where demand for HE outstrips supply (OECD 2009). Most students from Asia and Eastern Europe who choose to study abroad seek degrees in foreign universities, whereas only 2% of postsecondary students in Western Europe enroll in foreign degree programs (Kinginger Reference Kinginger2009). SA in Europe, North America, and Mexico predominantly involves programs of less than one year in duration.

The data provided by UNESCO (2014) on the global flow of tertiary-level students helps to explain three trends in where students choose to go. The first trend points to the reality that mobile students overwhelmingly tend to study in countries where their first language (L1) is one of the official languages, implying little to no need to acquire a foreign language (FL). The next trend is to study in a destination country where English is one of the official languages, predominantly the US, UK, Canada, and Australia. This may be explained by the globalization processes implemented in many secondary and tertiary institutions worldwide. The globalization initiative, according to Haberland & Mortensen (Reference Haberland and Mortensen2012), has ‘led to a situation where English has attained an unprecedented degree of globality’ (p. 1) and may explain the overwhelming presence of English-language education in language education policies and planning for many non-Anglophone countries (Kaplan & Baldauf Reference Kaplan and Baldauf2003). This tendency can be clarified by the fact that the lingua franca of the natural sciences has become English (Lillis & Curry Reference Lillis and Curry2010; Mortensen & Haberland Reference Mortensen and Haberland2012; Söderlundh Reference Söderlundh2012), as compared to the beginning of the twentieth century when the lingua franca was German (Hamel Reference Hamel2013: 327; Kaypak & Ortaçtepe Reference Kaypak and Ortaçtepe2014).

The third trend includes students who choose to study in a country where the target language (non-English) is different from their L1. These students appear to be from countries where the FL education policies, at the secondary and tertiary level, may include a learning objective of acquiring a second language (L2) other than English. Research falling under the first trend will not be addressed in this paper, since the focus is neither on language acquired nor on how various factors might influence language use or acquisition. Yang (Reference Yang2016) recently highlighted that the majority of the SA research (49.2% of what Yang reviewed) falls under the second trend, though not necessarily labeled SA, and is typically reported in journals dedicated to the teaching and learning of English as a second or foreign language (ESL) or in publications addressing HE experiences, psychology, intercultural development, or other areas.

A fourth trend is that of heritage seekers, who study abroad because of an ethnic, (ethno)religious, linguistic, or national connection to a specific ancestral country or region, for example, African American students studying in Ghana, Jewish American students studying in Israel, and Mexican-American students studying in the Spanish-speaking world. See Shively (Reference Shively and Pascual2016) for a review of research on heritage seeking in SA.

In the overview of educational policies regarding the implementation of an L2 within curricular program planning, the policies must be analyzed in the context of globalization. It appears that globalization, instead of being an advantage for promoting L2 learning and therefore promoting student mobility worldwide, in fact promotes English-language acquisition and thereby advantages students in the new global networks in which English has not only become the lingua franca but has also benefited from its value as a ‘form of cultural capital’ (Short et al. Reference Short, Boniche, Kim and Li2001).

Regardless of how internationalization is valued in respective countries, the OECD projects that participation in SA is likely to reach 8 million students per year by 2025, up from 5 million reported in 2014 (ICEF Monitor 2017). This growth is accompanied by a corresponding expansion of studies researching the benefits of studying abroad. The SA experience that is of focus in this review is one that involves language learning during a time period of a year or less, in which the student is enrolled in a host institution taking courses related to the target language and, mostly, in the target language. As will be shown in the review of literature in the following sections, research in this field focuses on what learners do differently during SA and how those actions affect their acquisition of the target language and has shown that not all experiences are equivalent. The dominant tendency has been to focus on second language acquisition (SLA), researching mainly developing bilingual participants in a target language community. Participant characteristics vary widely when we take into account aspects such as affective variables and preprogram proficiency levels. The context within which data are collected and methods are used can vary just as widely, not to mention the numerous aspects of linguistic competency that can be measured. In addition, the outcomes of SA are documented in various domains, including international mobility, ESL, and HE policy, to name a few. SA outcomes are also included within frameworks of intercultural competence development that provide evidence for theoretical models that clarify issues of intercultural sensitivity (see Chen & Starosta Reference Chen and Starosta1997) as well as intercultural transition. According to Zhou et al. (Reference Zhou, Jindal-Snape, Topping and Todman2008), the latter can be categorized into three theories: stress and coping model, culture learning model, and social identification theory. These theories from other fields highlight the complexity of SA research and underscore the challenge inherent in compiling an overview of the available SA literature.

The need for greater understanding of this experience has inspired a growing interest in mixed-methods approaches that go beyond measuring the linguistic development of the language learner to include research on changes in learner identity and agency and student perspectives about language learning that inform development of intercultural/transcultural competence (e.g., MLA 2007; Jackson Reference Jackson2010, Reference Jackson and Kinginger2013; Beaven & Spencer-Oatey Reference Beaven and Spencer-Oatey2016). Looking beyond linguistic gains abroad has also brought attention to a gap between the curriculum of SA programs and those back at home, revealing a need to calibrate traditional classroom goals with those skills needed to negotiate life abroad (Lord & Isabelli-García Reference Lord, Isabelli-García and Lacorte2014). Relatedly, there is considerable interest in the pedagogical enhancement of language learning in SA (Lafford & Isabelli in press).

The aim of this review is to synthesize empirical studies on undergraduate language learners’ experience abroad during a time period of a year or less. To help provide a framework to this synthesis, we begin our review by tracing the recent evolution of empirical mixed-method research on the learner, identifying problems and characteristics that language learners generally encounter in the SA experience. We take a closer look at variables related to individual difference such as anxiety, motivation, and attitudes to more recent views of learner identity in language learning. We highlight the shift to language learner agency, a topic that merits more discussion in SA literature.

We then review how the SA learning environments are treated. Though SA is at times treated as a categorical label, recent studies situate SA as a Social Ecosystem (de Bot, Lowie & Verspoor Reference de Bot, Lowie and Verspoor2007) where research interprets interacting, internal dynamic subsystems to explain the L2 learning process. This review takes a closer look at research informed by socially grounded theories (e.g., Pavlenko & Lantolf Reference Pavlenko, Lantolf and Lantolf2000; Lantolf & Pavlenko Reference Lantolf, Pavlenko and Breen2001; Block Reference Block2003). Finally, we review the role that SA plays in undergraduate language curricula, where the objectives of the experience are aligned with AH curricula, a topic that has not been fully discussed in SA literature. The conclusions offer suggestions for keeping pace with the broader field of applied/educational linguistics.

2. Research design

Research in the field of SA has primarily centered on the student, with earlier methodological approaches exploring the participants’ experiences. These mostly qualitative studies had, and still have, as the goal to ‘listen to participants and build an understanding based on what is heard’ (Creswell Reference Creswell2013: 29). As more research contributed to the conversation on the topic, we saw the application of more quantitative methods where the ‘researcher uses the [previous research] deductively as a framework for the research questions or hypotheses’ (p. 29). By 1989, educators Green, Caracelli & Graham challenged researchers to provide a conceptual framework for the, at the time, recent expansion of ‘methodological repertoire that include the use of both qualitative and quantitative methods’ (p. 255). Since then, mixed-method evaluation has gained a stronghold in SA research designs. Most of the mixed-method designs fall under Creswell & Garrett's (Reference Creswell and Garrett2008) definition of ‘collecting both quantitative and qualitative data (in response to quantitative and qualitative research questions), the merging, linking, or combining the two sources of data, and then conducting research as a single study or as longitudinal projects with multiple phases’ (p. 326).

In the following sections, we address how these three methodological approaches have helped move the field forward.

2.1 Quantitative research

Quantitative research, by definition, involves numbers: scores, statistics, and correlations. Broadly speaking, quantitative methods involve the ‘tallying, manipulation or systematic aggregation of quantities of data’ (Henning Reference Henning1986: 702). Its main purposes are to examine patterns in the data and to determine relationships among variables. Through quantitative analyses, researchers can determine what is typical and how much variation there is in the results, e.g., ‘What is the average change in listening test scores over SA?’ or ‘What is the range of performance on post-SA proficiency measures?’ Quantitative methods are particularly suited to exploring relationships between phenomena, e.g., ‘What is the relationship between preprogram proficiency and development of intercultural sensitivity?’ or to questions testing hypotheses, for example, ‘Does strategy training improve student outcomes on SA?’ Research exploring relationships is referred to as ‘associational,’ ‘correlational,’ or ‘survey’ (Creswell Reference Creswell2013). The goal of associational research is to determine whether a relationship exists among factors and, if so, to measure the strength of the relationship. Researchers undertaking associational studies are not interested in controlling every variable possible to determine causation, but instead they focus on correlation, or co-occurrence of variables.

In contrast to correlational studies, the goal of experimental studies is to establish causation as clearly as possible. In experimental studies, researchers deliberately manipulate variables in order to determine the effect of one variable on another. A true experiment must contain both a control group (receiving no treatment) and a treatment group (exposed to the intervention) to which participants must be randomly assigned (Creswell Reference Creswell2013). However, in SA research, true experiments can be very difficult to arrange. Random assignment of participants to one group or another is nearly impossible due to the expense involved in SA. Furthermore, student responses to being assigned without choosing SA or formal instruction at a home institution (AH) experiences can lead to motivational challenges. Huebner (Reference Huebner and Freed1995) is the only study in this report involving an experimental design, utilizing randomly assigned control (AH) and experimental (SA) groups participating in 9-week programs comparable in classroom hours, pedagogical approach, and so on. Five out of 23 of Huebner's participants dropped out during the study, with motivation being a key factor, leaving only ten abroad and eight AH. Small numbers like these make it difficult to draw conclusions that can be applied beyond the group of participants. While Huebner's SA learners out-gained AH controls on virtually every measure of language gain, these differences were not statistically significant. Huebner writes, ‘With small numbers of students, tests of statistical significance have very low power.’ Therefore, ‘how the results . . . can be generalized to the possible performance of future students is the central question, which sadly cannot adequately be answered by the inferential statistical methods employed’ (pp. 183–184). Due to the challenges of random assignment and creation of comparable control groups of adequate size to draw inferences, most attempts at experimental research in SA are, therefore, best categorized as either ‘pre-experimental’ or ‘quasi-experimental.’

Pre-experimental research is so named because the design involves a treatment given to one group as in an experimental study but does not include a control group (Creswell Reference Creswell2013). The one-group pretest and posttest design that is so common to SA research thus qualifies as ‘pre-experimental.’ Examples of this method include Martinsen's (Reference Martinsen2010) study of the development of learners’ spoken Spanish following a 6-week SA program or Dewaele, Comanaru & Faraco's (Reference Dewaele, Comanaru, Faraco, Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015) study of the effect of SA on the anxiety levels and willingness to communicate of students. Pre-experimental designs are typically thought to be useful for generation of hypotheses that can be tested later with more rigorous methods, but they are often the only option SA researchers have and seldom lead to more carefully controlled follow-up studies.

Quasi-experimental design involves a treatment group and a control group, but the groups are not randomly assigned (Creswell Reference Creswell2013). SA research comparing the linguistic skills of self-selecting students on SA (the treatment) and control students enrolled in formal traditional instruction AH or in domestic IM programs thus represents ‘quasi-experimental’ work. Examples of such studies include Dewey's (Reference Dewey2004) comparison of gains in reading comprehension of learners of Japanese in SA and IM settings. Similarly, Cubillos, Chieffo & Fan (Reference Cubillos, Chieffo and Fan2008) compare listening comprehension results for American learners of Spanish in Spain and Costa Rica with students enrolled in a course of similar length at home, and Juan-Garau, Salazar-Noguera & Prieto-Arranz (Reference Juan-Garau, Salazar-Noguera, Prieto-Arranz and Pérez-Vidal2014) contrast the overall L2 English competence, motivation, and beliefs of comparable AH and SA learners. Taking a different quasi-experimental approach, other studies compare SA students receiving a specific treatment such as homestay placement (Vande Berg, Connor-Linton & Paige Reference Vande Berg, Connor-Linton and Paige2009), work placement (Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura & McManus Reference Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015), or pre-departure orientation (Vande Berg et al. Reference Vande Berg, Connor-Linton and Paige2009) to control students not receiving the treatments. These studies improve on pre-experimental designs in that they can give a stronger indication that the treatment (SA, homestay placement, etc.) is effective. As Larsen-Freeman & Long (Reference Larsen-Freeman and Long2014) note, ‘Quasi-experiments exist as compromises for those interested in studying human behavior in naturally occurring settings in which complete experimental control is difficult, if not impossible’ (p. 22).

There are considerations in quantitative research other than selection or assignment of participants. One such consideration is timing of testing. Mackey & Gass (Reference Mackey and Gass2005) describe the following common timing pattern: pretest/posttest, in which some aspect of a learner's language is tested both before and after SA to determine whether SA resulted in any changes. Much of the SA research employs this method, either within a single group or between SA and AH groups (comparing changes from pre to posttesting over time) for each. The Martinsen (Reference Martinsen2010) pre-experimental study referenced above follows a pretest/posttest approach.

Pretest/posttest is a form of a repeated measures testing design. Repeated measures (also referred to as within-subject) involves testing the same individuals more than once—either testing the same person on multiple occasions (time-series) or administering multiple measures to the same people. Repeated measures can be used in both pre-experimental and quasi-experimental designs. McManus, Mitchell & Tracy-Ventura's (Reference McManus, Mitchell and Tracy-Ventura2014) study, comparing the language gains of students in various placement types, involved both multiple measures of linguistic abilities and multiple occasions over a 23-month period. Similarly, Trenchs-Parera & Juan-Garau (Reference Trenchs-Parera, Juan-Garau and Pérez-Vidal2014) administered a questionnaire on learners’ motivations and beliefs related to language learning four times over the course of learners’ university experience: (1) upon entry to university, (2) after two terms of formal instruction at university and prior to SA, (3) immediately following SA, and (4) 15 months after SA, following 80 hours of additional hours of formal L2 instruction. This longitudinal/time-series study indicated that the initial formal instruction ‘triggered positive changes in self-confidence and intended effort, while SA heightened the development of positive motivational stands, including the reduction of anxiety and the attribution of importance to listening abilities’ (p. 259). These changes held constant following the 15-month post-SA period. The only additional significant change from entry to university to final administration was a drop in the perceived importance of autonomous learning through self-access materials. In short, this study indicates how motivation and beliefs can change in different ways following formal instruction and SA experiences.

Factorial design is a measurement approach in which more than one independent variable and any number of moderating variables (variables that can affect the relationship between independent and dependent variables) are considered. Dewey et al.’s (Reference Dewey, Bown, Baker, Martinsen, Gold and Eggett2014) exploration of factors that influence L2 use during SA employed a factorial design. The researchers examined a variety of variables, including preprogram proficiency, gender, age, and social networks (independent variables), to determine their potential effects on the students’ reported use of the L2 (dependent variable) during SA.

One finding that is apparent in nearly every quantitative study is large amounts of variation (numerically evident in high ranges and standard deviations). Because SA experiences are so varied and complex, quantitative research alone cannot adequately examine them. The research has also consistently shown that individual differences play a prominent role in language learning—or lack thereof—during SA. Quantitative literature has attempted to explicate variables that may affect the differential outcomes associated with SA, but this approach does not always succeed in isolating and explicating variables and can reduce learners’ identities and experiences to an array of variables such as proficiency, motivation, personality, and time on task—measured psychometrically and represented as numbers. Such quantitative studies can give only an incomplete picture of SA, providing rudimentary and at best numerical attention to learners’ motives or dispositions, or to the nature of their experience while abroad. As Kinginger (Reference Kinginger2009: 68) states, ‘In research attempting a maximum level of generalization to whole populations, a recurrent finding is of striking individual differences whose explanation offers only tantalizing glimpses into the nature of SA experiences.’ Coleman (Reference Coleman and Kinginger2013a: 25) agrees, adding, ‘individual trajectories are in fact the essence of recent SA research, in which the focus has shifted from quantitative to qualitative, from product to process, from a search for generalizability to a recognition of complexity and variation.’

Thus, while quantitative studies of SA can answer a number of questions about trends across learners and relationships among factors, qualitative accounts of the SA experience are uniquely positioned to describe the ‘nature of the SA experience.’ In the next section, we provide an overview of qualitative research, its goals, and its methods.

2.2 Qualitative research

A broad definition of qualitative research is ‘any research that uses data that do not indicate ordinal values’ (Nkwi, Nyamongo & Ryan Reference Nkwi, Nyamongo and Ryan2001). Qualitative research is the ‘method of choice when researchers seek to understand processes, events, and relationships in the context of the cultural and social situation’ (Sullivan & Ebrahim Reference Sullivan, Ebrahim, Ebrahim and Sullivan1995: 196).

The advantage of qualitative approaches is that they allow scholars to examine SA in all its complexity, exploring multifarious recursive interactions between students’ motivations, identities (Kinginger Reference Kinginger2004; Isabelli-García Reference Isabelli-García, DuFon and Churchill2006; Kinginger Reference Kinginger2008; Allen Reference Allen2010b), their reception by members of the host culture (Pellegrino Reference Pellegrino2005; Isabelli-García Reference Isabelli-García, DuFon and Churchill2006), their attitudes toward the host culture (Isabelli-García Reference Isabelli-García, DuFon and Churchill2006; Kinginger Reference Kinginger2010), and their evaluations of the SA experience (Menard-Warwick & Palmer Reference Menard-Warwick and Palmer2012). Certain methods of qualitative research also allow researchers to examine the place of SA within the greater ‘ideological environment’ (Kinginger Reference Kinginger2004: 222) of FL learning (Gore Reference Gore2005; Trentman & Diao Reference Trentman and Diao2015).

Qualitative research can be conducted in dozens of ways, some methods with long histories behind them. Among the most prominent in SA research are case study, grounded theory, narrative analysis, discourse analysis, and conversation analysis. Each tradition differs in terms of research purposes, types of data collected, and, to some extent, methods of data collection and analysis. Van Lier (Reference van Lier and Hinkel2005) offers a useful paradigm for understanding differences in qualitative data analysis. He suggests that qualitative methods differ in the amount of ‘intervention’ specified in the research design, as well as in the degree to which the method is concerned with the individual or with groups. By intervention, we will understand the extent to which researchers work with existing data (including observation of naturally occurring interactions) or elicit data via interviews or journals collected specifically for the purposes of research. Ethnography, some forms of conversation analysis, and discourse analysis involve little intervention, focusing instead on collecting or analyzing existing data. Methods like grounded theory, case study, and biography usually elicit data. In terms of the second dimension, ethnography tends to be more concerned with groups, while biography and narrative analysis are frequently, though not exclusively, focused on individuals; and methods like grounded theory and case study can focus either on groups or on individuals.

To van Lier's (Reference van Lier and Hinkel2005) two dimensions, we add a third—the extent to which data are analyzed linguistically or analyzed for content. In the former, researchers use the data obtained from learners to examine evidence of linguistic or pragmatic development, as in discourse and conversation analysis. In the latter, researchers are concerned not with how the message is encoded, but rather with what the individuals are saying, as in much case study research. In fact, when researchers are primarily interested in the message rather than the medium, the language of interviews and journals is frequently, though not always, in the native language (L1), so as to allow learners to adequately express their thoughts and emotions.

When texts are analyzed linguistically, rather than for content, researchers use methods such as discourse analysis or conversation analysis to analyze samples of natural language. Such studies seek to document language socialization, the nature of learner interactions, and linguistic development over time.

Documenting language socialization and learner interactions involves analysis of conversations between learners and their native-speaking interlocutors, examining how relationships between learners and their hosts are constructed in conversation. Some focus on the discourse patterns that learners and their hosts use in their interactions. Wilkinson's (Reference Wilkinson2002) study, for example, showed how learners and their hosts alike rely heavily on classroom roles and discourse structures to manage their interactions. Other studies document the ways in which the hosts’ perception of foreigners, and their beliefs about foreigners, affect the input and corrective feedback that they provide (Siegal Reference Siegal1996; Cook Reference Cook, DuFon and Churchill2006; Iino Reference Iino, DuFon and Churchill2006; Kinginger et al. Reference Kinginger, Lee, Wu and Tan2016; Lee et al. Reference Lee, Wu, Di and Kinginger2017).

The second line of research employs recordings of naturally occurring conversations to trace learners’ language development. Such studies have focused on the development of pragmatic features of language such as requests (Shively Reference Shively2008, Reference Shively2013) and openings and closings of service encounters (Shively Reference Shively2008). Other studies have examined the development of linguistic accuracy (DeKeyser Reference DeKeyser2010) or acquisition of particular discourse features, like the German particle also (Schirm Reference Schirm2015). Thus, close qualitative analysis of learners’ interactions can document the processes by which they learn particular features of the language and their progress over time.

Another area of qualitative research involves what van Lier (Reference van Lier1994) terms action research. In a 1994 article, van Lier defined action research as ‘small-scale intervention in the functioning of the real world and a close examination of the effects of such intervention’ (p. 32). In response to qualitative (and quantitative) studies that show that learners’ expectations of SA are often not met, and that gaining access to communities of practice in the target culture is often difficult, many researchers have recommended training and other interventions that may help learners to make more of their SA experience. Research has documented the value of such interventions as requiring learners to participate in extracurricular activities and service opportunities (Engle & Engle Reference Engle and Engle1999; Kinginger Reference Kinginger and Higgins2011), to conduct ethnographic research projects while in-country (Jackson Reference Jackson, Byram and Feng2006a; Kinginger Reference Kinginger and Higgins2011; Lee Reference Lee2012), and to keep blogs or other journals during their sojourn (Allen Reference Allen and Kinginger2013). Jackson (Reference Jackson, Byram and Feng2006a) and Kinginger (Reference Kinginger and Higgins2011) both highlight the importance of teaching learners the skills of observation and introspection. Belnap et al. (Reference Belnap, Bown, Dewey, Belnap, Steffen, MacIntyre, Gregersen and Mercer2016) emphasize training in self-regulation as well as the importance of providing regular conversation partners for SA students, particularly when homestays are not an option. Findings suggest that these interventions promote social interaction during SA as well as increased intercultural development.

Over the past two decades, qualitative research in SA has become popular, particularly as a means of understanding the complexities of SA contexts. The vast majority of the research seeks to understand the individual variation noted in variables-based research, and to examine the roles played by social and environmental factors as well as by individual differences. Such research has focused on various language learning behaviors (Pellegrino Reference Pellegrino2005; Isabelli-García Reference Isabelli-García, DuFon and Churchill2006; Magnan & Back Reference Magnan and Back2007) and motivations for learning the L2 and studying abroad. Qualitative research to date has clearly indicated that language learning in SA is, as Kinginger (Reference Kinginger and Higgins2011) states, ‘a dialogic and situated affair whose success depends not only on the attributes and intentions of the student but also the ways in which the student is received within his or her host community’ (p. 60).

Though qualitative research methods have painted a richer picture of SA participants and the communities in which they interact, some scholars have noted limitations to their application to SA research. Kinginger (Reference Kinginger2009) notes a tendency to limit research to the perspectives of the participants, without reference to the host community. For example, much of the research on gender-related incidents examines only student reactions to incidents deemed ‘harassment’ without an attempt to understand the host perceptions and the typical gender-related practices of the community. Thus, scholars have suggested that researchers expand their inquiry to consider all of the stakeholders in a SA program, including the teachers, administrators, host families, and others with whom the learners interact, as in Trentman & Diao (Reference Trentman and Diao2015). Another limitation includes the relative lack of longitudinal studies examining the effects of SA on learners’ language or identities after SA. In response, some SA researchers have also turned attention to the long-term effects of SA. In particular, they have become concerned with the effects of SA on the change in learner identity (see Benson et al. Reference Benson, Barkhuizen, Bodycott and Brown2013) and have probed learners’ views of themselves a year or two after the experience. However, this type of research is relatively new, and few have had the time to follow a group of learners for more than a year or so after the SA experience.

2.3 Mixed-methods approach

A fruitful and growing approach to SLA research is the mixed-methods approach, in which researchers combine in-depth qualitative analysis of students’ experiences with quantitative documentation of learning outcomes. Among the studies that successfully blend these two lines of inquiry are Isabelli-García (Reference Isabelli-García, DuFon and Churchill2006, Reference Isabelli-García2010), Kinginger (Reference Kinginger2008), Briggs (Reference Briggs2016), Jackson (Reference Jackson2016), and Tracy-Ventura et al. (Reference Tracy-Ventura, Dewaele, Köylü and McManus2016). These studies evaluate measures of linguistic gain against accounts of learners’ experiences in the host culture, contributing to a fine-grained analysis of individual students’ behavior as an intervening variable between aptitude and initial proficiency and language learning success. The earlier studies used small samples, whereas Briggs (Reference Briggs2016) documents the vocabulary strategies of 241 adult SA learners in two learning contexts and Tracy-Ventura et al. (Reference Tracy-Ventura, Dewaele, Köylü and McManus2016) investigate the personality changes of 58 British undergraduate students who spent their third year abroad in a French- or Spanish-speaking country. And Jackson (Reference Jackson2016) researched the language and intercultural learning of 149 Chinese students who participated in a semester-long exchange program in an English-speaking country.

Other mixed-methods projects include the LANGSNAP research project that documents a range of language domains, connecting progress in L2 to a range of individual, social, and contextual variables. The project tracked a cohort of 56 students majoring in French or Spanish before, during, and after spending their year abroad in France, Spain, or Mexico (McManus, Mitchell & Tracy-Ventura Reference McManus, Mitchell and Tracy-Ventura2014; Mitchell Reference Mitchell2015). Klapper & Rees (Reference Klapper and Rees2012) link cases of ‘quantified’ individual progression with qualitative data. The study involved 57 students in total, with an in-depth focus on 12 who either failed to show progress or who showed substantial progress on various measures of linguistic gain.

Qualitative research is an important component of research on SA phenomena, providing a rich picture of individual variables and of the complexity of language learning in varied SA contexts. Combining quantifiable measures of linguistic or intercultural gains with qualitative descriptions of learners’ experiences, beliefs, and behaviors lends validity to the research. Moreover, there is a need to expand the focus of the research to include a broader range of perspectives.

3. Linguistic constructs and gains: Documenting learning outcomes

DeKeyser (Reference DeKeyser and Pérez-Vidal2014) aptly points out that the field of SA is still far from ‘go[ing] beyond descriptive adequacy to explanatory adequacy, and ultimately . . . predict what we are going to find in the next study’ (p. 319). The SA experience—a complex social ecosystem with an endless number of independent variables—is difficult to control for.

Research carried out on SA has blossomed from a field that focused on isolated linguistic constructs to multiple considerations of the language learner. Research studies focus on a variety of topics, ranging from linguistic performance on a single token such as lexical, morphological, or phonological features to nonlinguistic domains such as fluctuating identity formation that occurs abroad and development of intercultural competency. As is to be expected, although SA participation continues to be viewed as an experience that ‘allows for intensive, regular, contextualized L2-use opportunities in situ. . . . A closer look . . . however, indicates contradictory findings’ (Yang Reference Yang2016: 67).

The aim of this section is not to carry out a review of all SA research in all its transformations. Instead, we focus here on holistic linguistic performance gains and areas where contradictions and omissions are present. These are limited to include empirical studies based on the more frequently researched aspects of SLA in SA: the four modalities of language use and the development of particular aspects of communicative competence. This itself is a challenging task as recent research focuses on multiple aspects of SA, measuring multiple learning outcomes.

3.1 Modalities of language use

3.1.1 Reading

Few scholars have examined the influence of SA on reading development. Churchill (Reference Churchill, DuFon and Churchill2006) and DuFon (Reference DuFon, DuFon and Churchill2006) note that the relative lack of studies on reading reflects a bias both in-program design and in expectations toward the development of speaking and listening skills. Moreover, the unique affordances of SA allow learners to interact with native speakers in a way that classroom learning experiences do not, thus predisposing learners to focus on speaking and listening during their time abroad. Students typically have many opportunities to read the L2 even in their home country, but they have fewer opportunities to speak. Thus, many learners see less value in reading during SA. Allen & Herron's (Reference Allen and Herron2003) study confirmed this bias, finding that students prefer to obtain information by conversing with others rather than by reading. Nevertheless, a number of studies have demonstrated that time spent reading abroad leads to measurable gains in reading comprehension (Brecht, Davidson & Ginsberg Reference Brecht, Davidson, Ginsberg and Freed1995; Lapkin, Hart & Swain Reference Lapkin, Hart, Swain and Freed1995; Kinginger Reference Kinginger2008), as measured by various standardized tests of reading comprehension.

While the studies cited above have looked at the development of reading comprehension, others have examined more closely the development of particular skills in reading abilities. For example, Fraser (Reference Fraser2002) examined the ability of learners to match anaphora and cataphora to their referents, while Dewey (Reference Dewey2004) reported that American learners of Japanese became stronger in vocabulary recognition and text comprehension, along with developing greater confidence in their reading ability. Briggs (Reference Briggs2016) also found that learners made significant gains pre- to post-SA on receptive and productive vocabulary knowledge—knowledge necessary for reading comprehension and writing proficiency development respectively.

Some contradictory evidence does exist, however. For example, Dewey's (Reference Dewey2004) quasi-experimental study comparing the reading gains of learners on SA with learners in an intensive IM environment setting did not find differences between the SA and the IM group on objective measures of reading ability. The SA group did, however, report greater confidence in their reading ability than did the IM students. Dewey also found differences in the types of strategies used by SA students and IM students to comprehend written text. The SA group monitored their understanding of what they were reading more than the IM group, while the IM group more often responded effectively to the text. One might also consider Huebner's experimental (Reference Huebner and Freed1995) study contradictory, since differences between SA and AH learners were not significant in this study. Huebner noted possible differences in motivation between the two groups, with the SA group embracing the introduction of various Japanese writing systems early in their instruction because they had a greater instrumental need to read than did their counterparts at home. Huebner suggests a possible connection between this motivation and the higher (but not significantly different) scores for SA than AH learners.

Whereas much of the focus on reading comprehension in SA has been on learning outcomes, Kline (Reference Kline1998) advocated for a process-oriented approach. In an ethnographic study of the reading habits of learners on an SA program in France, Kline concluded that reading behavior was influenced by a number of factors, including gender, group affiliations, and host family preferences. Kline, however, did not establish a relationship between learners’ reading behaviors and their reading ability.

Reading, thus, represents an understudied domain in the research on SA. Moreover, much of the focus has been on discretely measurable skills, such as reading comprehension, recall, and vocabulary development. There is very little research on student motivations to read, their values and reading preferences, or on their development over time (Kline Reference Kline1998). Moreover, the development of literacy skills while abroad involves more than development of linguistic and pragmatic skills (such as word decoding or identifying textual genres); indeed, learning to read and write requires negotiating new academic cultures and adapting to new forms of literacy (Kinginger Reference Kinginger2009), especially, as Taillefer (Reference Taillefer2005) documents, very different academic literature cultures among European countries and different reading strategies among students from different nationalities.

3.1.2 Writing

Development of writing skills during SA is even less studied than development of reading skills, though some studies indicate that learners report improvement in their writing skills (Meara Reference Meara1994; Adams Reference Adams, DuFon and Churchill2006). Very few studies have used objective measures to document the development of writing in such areas as proficiency, fluency, or quality of essays, and those studies have yielded contradictory results. Freed, So & Lazar (Reference Freed, So and Lazar2003) compared the written fluency of learners on SA with that of learners AH. They found that native speakers (NSs) did not judge the writing of SA students to be any more fluent than that of their AH counterparts, though objective measures demonstrated that the essays of SA learners had grown in length and lexical density. On the other hand, Sasaki (Reference Sasaki2004, Reference Sasaki2007, Reference Sasaki and Manchón2009) found that SA did have a positive effect on participants’ writing, as compared to the writing of students AH. In particular, she found that SA participants developed different strategies for writing and became more motivated to improve their L2 writing than did their AH counterparts. A later follow-up study (Sasaki Reference Sasaki2011) revealed that, in addition to improved writing skills, students who studied abroad formed ‘imagined communities’ with L2 speakers that motivated them to improve their writing in the L2.

Another set of studies related to the SLA project, studying the long-term effects of SA on language learning, suggests that SA may have a positive impact on L2 writing skills, even if learners do not receive formal instruction in L2 writing. Pérez-Vidal & Juan-Garau (Reference Pérez-Vidal and Juan-Garau2009) compared the development of complexity, accuracy, and fluency among SA students following either a 6-month period of instruction AH or a 3-month SA program. They found that SA participants, as opposed to AH participants, made significant progress in fluency measures and lexical diversity. Barquin (Reference Barquin2012, cited in Pérez-Vidal & Barquin Reference Pérez-Vidal, Barquin and Pérez-Vidal2014), likewise, found improvement after SA in fluency, lexical richness, accuracy, syntactic complexity and variety, and cohesion. A later study by Pérez-Vidal & Barquin (Reference Pérez-Vidal, Barquin and Pérez-Vidal2014) considered the development of writing skills among a group of 72 learners over a period of 30 months, which included an SA period preceded and followed by formal instruction AH. The authors found that a 3-month period of SA ‘led to significant gains in [. . .] writing in the domains of fluency, accuracy, and lexical complexity, and that no such gains were found in periods of [f]ormal [i]nstruction either before of after the SA context’ (p. 232). Moreover, they found that the gains made during SA were maintained 15 months after the SA program had ended.

The dearth of literature on the development of writing in SA may reflect a bias toward development of aural/oral skills—at least for SA in FL environments. Much more has been written about the development of writing in L2 situations. Nevertheless, the scant literature on the subject does suggest that SA can lead to some improvements in writing. If nothing else, SA researchers may wish to examine the role of writing as a support skill, since at least one of Kinginger's (Reference Kinginger2008) participants attributed much of his academic progress in France to the activity of writing.

3.1.3 Listening comprehension

As with literacy skills, the literature on listening comprehension in SA is scant. Meara (Reference Meara1994) and Cubillos & Ilvento (Reference Cubillos and Ilvento2012) found that learners feel more confident about their listening skills following SA (with Kaplan's [Reference Kaplan1989] study representing something of an exception: students felt that they had made the most gains in receptive skills, listening and reading) than they do about their literacy skills. This is striking since Ikeguchi (Reference Ikeguchi1996) finds that literacy skills appear more durable after SA than oral/aural skills. The existing research largely confirms their self-assessments: according to the literature, students on SA make significant gains in listening comprehension, particularly when compared to ‘at home learners’ (Dyson Reference Dyson1988; Ginsberg, Robin & Wheeling Reference Ginsberg, Robin and Wheeling1992; Llanes & Muñoz Reference Llanes and Muñoz2009). Other studies using a pre- and postprogram design have also found that learners on SA make significant gains in listening comprehension (Allen & Herron Reference Allen and Herron2003; Kinginger Reference Kinginger2008; Davidson Reference Davidson2010), though Davidson (Reference Davidson2010) and Cubillos & Ilvento (Reference Cubillos and Ilvento2012) suggest that longer-term SA is necessary for greater gains. This finding, however, contradicts that of Llanes & Muñoz (Reference Llanes and Muñoz2009), who report that students on a 3–4-week SA program made significant gains in listening comprehension. A number of other studies using pre- and postprogram designs have found only modest gains in listening comprehension (Huebner Reference Huebner and Freed1995; Tanaka & Ellis Reference Tanaka and Ellis2003). Davidson further found that preprogram listening comprehension was a strong predictor for the development of oral proficiency, while Cubillos et al. (Reference Cubillos, Chieffo and Fan2008) note a potential threshold effect; that is, learners must have reached a certain threshold of listening comprehension in order to make any progress in their comprehension during SA.

Very few studies have compared the gains of students on SA to those in other settings. Huebner's (Reference Huebner and Freed1995) study represents an exception. He compared gains on the Educational Testing Service's Japanese Listening Proficiency Test for students on a 9-week SA program in Japan with learners in an IM program of comparable length and pedagogical approach. In his study, he found that the SA group performed slightly better on the listening test than did the IM group, but the number of participants was small and the numbers were not significant. Cubillos et al. (Reference Cubillos, Chieffo and Fan2008) likewise compared performance on a standardized listening comprehension test by groups of American learners of Spanish on 5-week SA programs in Spain and Costa Rica and by students enrolled in a course of similar length in their home country. The authors found no significant differences between the two groups, though the authors assert that the SA learners differed from the AH learners in their use of strategies. In particular, the SA group tended to use more ‘sophisticated, social, and top-down strategies,’ while the AH learners tended to use less-productive bottom-up strategies.

Whereas most of the above studies have focused on listening comprehension broadly defined, Taguchi (Reference Taguchi2008, Reference Taguchi2011) has examined particular skills associated with listening, including speed of comprehension, lexical access, accuracy of comprehension, and pragmatic comprehension—that is, the ability to understand the intended meanings of particular types of utterances. Her 2008 study examining the lexical access speed and the speed and accuracy with which learners comprehended indirect refusals and opinions noted gains in all areas for SA participants, though their ability to understand indirect opinions in their L2 was not as great as their ability to comprehend indirect refusals. In her 2011 study, Taguchi compared the pragmatic competence of SA learners and AH learners and found that SA learners comprehended certain implicature types more accurately than did their AH counterparts.

Though the primary focus on listening comprehension has been on documenting gains in listening, other studies have attempted to document the types of listening activities in which learners engage (Kaplan Reference Kaplan1989; Freed, Segalowitz & Dewey Reference Freed, Segalowitz and Dewey2004; Kemp Reference Kemp2010; Llanes & Muñoz Reference Llanes and Muñoz2013), usually in the context of examining overall language contact, though Kemp (Reference Kemp2010) focuses on a listening log. Kemp's purpose is to document the ways in which a listening log helped learners to identify comprehension problems, to reflect on their motivation for listening, and the strategies they employed to improve their comprehension abilities.

Kinginger (Reference Kinginger2009) notes a potential problem of ecological validity with the majority of studies listed above, in that they have assessed listening comprehension via multiple-choice tests, which are biased toward academic language and functions and may not reflect the variety of listening in which students engage during SA. Moreover, she notes that the studies have largely ignored the quality of students’ engagement in L2-mediated activities as well as learners’ identities and histories that might affect their listening comprehension. Kemp (Reference Kemp2010) documents the use of a listening log among Erasmus students studying abroad in the UK. She notes that the log helped researchers to better understand learner motivations for listening, and appeared to help learners reflect on the sources of any comprehension difficulties and to develop strategies to aid their comprehension.

3.1.4 Speaking

The most well-researched outcome of SA is speaking gains. Studies within this modality frequently compare outcomes with the control group of AH students, but overall, research to date has not been able to offer a concise holistic conclusion of whether SA is beneficial for oral skills development. Granted, pre- and postprogram speaking measures either show that the student remains at the same level or improves. While most measurement of speaking gains in the SA literature has involved proficiency, the field has also become curious about how specific aspects of oral proficiency change and, therefore, as a natural progression, we have ‘deconstructed’ oral proficiency into more detailed researchable areas. These studies have shown that even if holistic measures of oral proficiency do not show growth over time, atomistic aspects such as fluency (speech rate, length of turns, hesitancy, and self-repetitions), pronunciation, spoken grammatical accuracy, and pragmatics often do (see Section 3.2 for some of these findings).

Global oral proficiency is often measured during SA using instruments such as the ACTFL Oral Proficiency Interview (OPI) (East Reference East2016) that can be face-to-face, telephonic or computer-mediated and is intended to recreate authentic interaction between the student and the test rater. The validity of the OPI has been called into question (Bordón & Liskin-Gasparro Reference Bordón, Liskin-Gasparro and Lacorte2014; Lantolf Reference Lantolf, Spolsky, Inbar-Lourie and Tannebaum2014; East Reference East2016) on several facets, the most notable being the ‘artificiality’ of the interaction between the interlocutor and student (East Reference East2016: 41). A simulated OPI—an alternate method with similar issues—involves the use of prompts either recorded or written in a booklet to elicit speech samples similar to those elicited through the OPI (Vande Berg et al. Reference Vande Berg, Connor-Linton and Paige2009; Hernández Reference Hernández2010). Semi-guided oral interviews, where students interact with each other, as interviewer or interviewee, are also used (Valls-Ferrer & Mora Reference Valls-Ferrer, Mora and Pérez-Vidal2014). Given concerns about the artificiality of such measures as these, some have used recorded natural conversations to understand spoken politeness (Shively Reference Shively2011), accent (Diao Reference Diao2017), and food-related discourse patterns (DuFon Reference DuFon, DuFon and Churchill2006).

Studies that provide helpful conclusions regarding speaking development abroad move away from using the SA context as a categorical label (e.g., Wood Reference Wood2007; Trenchs-Parera Reference Trenchs-Parera2009; Serrano, Tragant & Llanes Reference Serrano, Tragant and Llanes2012; Jochum Reference Jochum2014; Prokhorov & Therkelsen Reference Prokhorov and Therkelsen2015) and more toward a research methodology that measures multiple independent variables of the social ecosystem that SA involves (e.g., Martinsen Reference Martinsen2010; Baker-Smemoe et al. Reference Baker-Smemoe, Dewey, Bown and Martinsen2014; Di Silvio, Donovan & Malone Reference Di Silvio, Donovan and Malone2014; Valls-Ferrer & Mora Reference Valls-Ferrer, Mora and Pérez-Vidal2014). Speaking assessment gains is the dependent variable of the dynamic system the students find themselves in, many times taking into account factors such as preprogram proficiency, differing learner characteristics, and the quantity and quality of out-of-class language contact. In general, speaking proficiency development is as varied as the multiplicity of factors that are found in SA and should be assessed taking those factors into consideration.

3.2 Communicative competence

The term communicative competence in SLA has undergone much discussion. Although other versions of communicative competence exist (e.g., Hymes Reference Hymes, Pride and Holmes1972; Bachman Reference Bachman1990), we use Canale's (Reference Canale, Richards and Schmidt1983) model that consists of the following competencies: grammatical (vocabulary, word formation, sentence formation, pronunciation, spelling, and semantics), sociolinguistic, strategic, and discourse. Of the elements listed in these competencies, we focus here on the SA research trend of studying accuracy development in grammatical and sociolinguistic competence. Moreover, we review those studies that are situated within methods that take into account the social aspect of language use.

SA research encompassing accuracy, defined as ‘the degree of deviancy from a particular norm’ (Housen & Kuiken Reference Housen and Kuiken2009: 3), is normally included within studies of complexity, accuracy, and fluency and ‘cover a wide spectrum of methods in applied linguistics, ranging from holistic and subjective ratings to objective quantitative measures of L2 production. The latter are clearly the preferred method in L2 studies’ (Housen, Kuiken & Vedder Reference Housen, Kuiken, Vedder, Housen, Kuiken and Vedder2012: 8). These authors also note that, although early research in the area of L2 accuracy was more general in nature, a recent trend ‘calls for finer-grained analyses . . . and hence a return to measures targeting more specific subdomains of language and more distinct linguistic features, as a complement to the use of more global measures’ (p. 8). They add that in SLA research, the number of accuracy measures is ‘daunting and partly reflects the lack of consensus on how [they] should be defined as constructs’ (p. 8). Research focusing on accuracy development in SA has typically focused on the number and type of errors in two areas. The first is the SA learners’ development and approximation to NS phonological/phonetic norms, the second is grammatical development toward the prescriptive norm, and the third area discussed in this subsection is development toward target-like interlanguage norms during SA.

3.2.1 Grammatical competence–phonological

As highlighted by Müller (Reference Müller2016), research on the acquisition of L2 pronunciation focuses on differences in pronunciation gains, similar to other aspects of linguistic development abroad, and has found mixed results mostly attributable to individual differences. Studies typically focus on the approximation of participants to the native production of salient phonological features of regional language varieties (e.g., O'Brien Reference O'Brien2003; Knouse Reference Knouse2012; Reynolds-Case Reference Reynolds-Case2013; Avello & Lara Reference Avello, Lara and Pérez-Vidal2014) as well as the learners’ perceptual competence (e.g., Mora Reference Mora and Pérez-Vidal2014; Romanelli, Menegotto & Smyth Reference Romanelli, Menegotto and Smyth2015). In general, such research ‘has shown that learners can adopt the regionally specific features of the language but typically do not use them to the same extent as native speakers’ (George Reference George2014: 98).

Measuring phonological features (usually acoustic analyses realized using Praat [Boersma & Weenink Reference Boersma and Weenink2013] and spectrographic analyses [e.g., Lord Reference Lord2010; Stevens Reference Stevens2011]) either treats the produced variable as an error that is not in line with the target language norm or as an alternation of a target language norm (Salgado Robles Reference Salgado Robles2014: 238). In fact, a trend in SA pronunciation research conjectures that L2 pronunciation errors do not always impede comprehensibility. That is, these errors could be due to a ‘trade-off effect’ (Serrano, Llanes & Tragant Reference Serrano, Llanes and Tragant2016), as learners may focus on other aspects of their oral production, such as proficiency aspects that deal with optimal comprehensibility with native interlocutors outside of the classroom.

Treatment options measuring pronunciation typically involve NSs' rating recordings of participants’ read-aloud tasks using pre-established rubrics, oral narratives based on a series of pictures, and elicited imitation tasks analyzing how successful learners are at replicating native speech norms. However, this nonspontaneous speech, although convenient for data collection, is not true to that used in face-to-face, conversational interactions, as noted in Hardison (Reference Hardison2014), nor do they take into account whether errors or variations impede comprehensibility while abroad. More spontaneous conversations simulating the OPI prove to elicit more natural language (e.g., Fernández-García & Martínez-Arbelaiz Reference Fernández-García and Martínez-Arbelaiz2014; Martinsen, Alvord & Tanner Reference Martinsen, Alvord and Tanner2014). They tend to be less artificial than read-aloud tasks and can capture changes during SA in pronunciation that might be seen in more authentic speech.

The extent to which conclusions can be drawn and compared are limited since the research methods do not ensure that the procedure includes a measure of prior knowledge and maintenance of the features under consideration (George Reference George2014), measuring language contact information abroad (Bongiovanni et al. Reference Bongiovanni, Long, Solon and Willis2015), or learner perceptions of token items (Pope Reference Pope, Sessarego and Tejedo-Herrero2016). Müller (Reference Müller2016: 113) reminds her readers that individual learning factors need to be included in the analyses of future studies, which explains why, to date, it has been ‘difficult to derive common trends from learner cohorts.’

3.2.2 Grammatical competence–word and sentence formation

Grammatical accuracy is typically measured in one of two ways. One method measures accuracy of specific grammatical features in controlled oral production. And the other is via tools that measure accuracy using written surveys or questionnaires. Results from studies employing quasi-experimental designs using an AH control group are just as valuable as within-group pre-experimental studies. The former seek to investigate the effect of SA by comparing with AH learning, whereas the latter tend to provide a descriptive notion of how the social nature of language use comes into play in SA, taking into account learner variation factors.

The focus of grammatical accuracy typically centers on items in the L2 that do not appear in the L1, since dissimilarities may make these items more challenging, causing them to merit more extensive exposure through SA. Generally, development in grammatical accuracy is in line with other aspects of language domains. That is, although some students show development (e.g., Duperron Reference Duperron and Wilkinson2006; Marques-Pascual Reference Marques-Pascual2011; Juan-Garau et al. Reference Juan-Garau, Salazar-Noguera, Prieto-Arranz and Pérez-Vidal2014; Salgado Robles Reference Salgado Robles2014; Grey et al. Reference Grey, Cox, Serafini and Sanz2015), not all do (e.g., DeKeyser Reference DeKeyser2010; Geeslin et al. Reference Geeslin, García-Amaya, Hasler-Barke, Henriksen, Killam, Borgonovo, Español-Echevarría and Prévost2010; Isabelli-García Reference Isabelli-García2010; Wang Reference Wang2010), and quasi-experimental work often indicates no difference in development between SA and AH (e.g., Martínez-Arbelaiz & Pereira Rodríguez Reference Martínez-Arbelaiz and Pereira Rodríguez2008; Arnett Reference Arnett2013).

Methodologies that include NSs as a comparison group tend to have results that are more true to the norm of the target language group (Serrano Reference Serrano2010; Kanwit, Geeslin & Fafulas Reference Kanwit, Geeslin and Fafulas2015). It is arguable whether or not learners’ development (or lack thereof) is explained by different durations abroad. Nevertheless, grammatical accuracy is documented either via measures that elicit oral production (e.g., Juan-Garau Reference Juan-Garau and Pérez-Vidal2014) or written production (Juan-Garau et al. Reference Juan-Garau, Salazar-Noguera, Prieto-Arranz and Pérez-Vidal2014).

Foster & Wigglesworth (Reference Foster, Wigglesworth and van Daele2007) call for research that includes reliable accuracy tools that incorporate a system to measure the level of seriousness of an error. Written data collection measures frequently employ grammaticality judgment tests or written narration of picture stories. Oral data collection measures also include narration of picture stories or semi-guided interviews. Both measures are given at a preprogram and postprogram interval as the norm, at times collecting data from intermediate time points, a repeated measures approach. These multiple collection points provide a nuanced look at the restructuring of acquired knowledge that occurs during a longer stay abroad. In both situations, those methods that base the data collection process on ‘a more communicative assessment task instead of a strictly grammar-driven assessment provided a more detailed and appropriate measure of students’ knowledge’ (Arnett Reference Arnett2013: 710).

3.2.3 Sociolinguistic competence–interlanguage pragmatics

Research in the field of sociolinguistic competence during SA tends to focus on pragmatics—the study of those aspects of language that cannot be considered in isolation from its use, language in its situational context, including the knowledge and beliefs of the speaker and the relationship and interaction between speaker and listener. It is generally known SA has a significant impact on the acquisition of sociolinguistic competence (Rasouli Khorshidi Reference Rasouli Khorshidi2013; Ren Reference Ren2013; Félix-Brasdefer & Hasler-Barker Reference Félix-Brasdefer and Hasler-Barker2015). Within SA, research has been generally carried out in interlanguage pragmatics (ILP) with intercultural pragmatics and cross-cultural pragmatics receiving less focus.

ILP research in SA focuses on L2 learners’ production of speech acts, such as requests (Schauer Reference Schauer2009; Bataller Reference Bataller2010; Shively Reference Shively2011; Bella Reference Bella2012; Nguyen & Basturkmen Reference Nguyen, Basturkmen, Roever and Nguyen2013; Li Reference Li2014; Al-Gahtani & Roever Reference Al-Gahtani and Roever2015), and the extent that request strategies change to target-like norms during SA. Other areas vary widely to include the development of address terms (Schauer Reference Schauer2009; Hassall Reference Hassall2013) and appropriateness (Diao Reference Diao2014; Taguchi Reference Taguchi2015), use of honorifics (Brown Reference Brown and Kinginger2013), apologetic behavior (Warga & Schölmberger Reference Warga and Schölmberger2007), and refusals and opinions (Taguchi Reference Taguchi2008), among others. The quantity of interaction in SA has been shown to play an important role (Taguchi Reference Taguchi2008; Bella Reference Bella2012; Vilar-Beltrán Reference Vilar-Beltrán2014). Nonetheless, development in the production of requests varies during SA (Schauer Reference Schauer2009; Bataller Reference Bataller2010; Iwasaki Reference Iwasaki2010), not unlike the other aspects discussed in the previous pages of this review. Furthermore, on the receptive side, learners can show development on measures of speed but not accuracy (Taguchi Reference Taguchi2008), indicating the importance of assessing development in multiple ways.

Ren (Reference Ren2013: 722) states that ‘it is generally agreed that there is no ideal method for all investigations in ILP’; however, the data collection tool commonly used is the Discourse Completion Task (DCT). A DCT is most often a written survey with situational prompts. That differs from an oral DCT (oral response to a written or oral prompt) and from a role-play, which involves the learner interacting orally with another person in real-time, usually involving multiple turns. The use of DCTs has been shown to be problematic (Schauer Reference Schauer2009; Golato & Golato Reference Golato, Golato and Chapelle2013). Golato (Reference Golato2003), as cited in Félix-Brasdefer & Hasler-Barker (Reference Félix-Brasdefer and Hasler-Barker2015: 78) explain this is because this instrument ‘elicits single-turn responses, has a low degree of content validity, and is simulated and non-interactive’. However, Félix-Brasdefer & Hasler-Barker (Reference Félix-Brasdefer and Hasler-Barker2015: 78) acknowledge that ‘this type of instrument can be reliable for measuring off-line pragmatic knowledge (what learners know) when they have a limited amount of time in which to respond.’ Using natural data, such as audio-recorded interactions (Shively Reference Shively2011), is recommended to complement experimental data. Other experimental tools used to measure ILP include the Language Awareness Interview (Kinginger Reference Kinginger2008) and role-play scenarios (Nguyen & Basturkmen Reference Nguyen, Basturkmen, Roever and Nguyen2013) that are designed to elicit more naturally occurring discourse and to ‘observe how context factors such as power, distance, and imposition affect the speaker's choice of pragmatic strategies’ (Nguyen & Basturkmen Reference Nguyen, Basturkmen, Roever and Nguyen2013: 20–21). As pointed out by Stokoe (Reference Stokoe2013), because the use of language in social situations and in role plays may differ, comparing experimental data at various points in time abroad with natural data is encouraged. However, the challenge with natural data is its collection and comparability. While natural data is highly desirable, another alternative to the written DCT that has been advocated recently (see, e.g., Bardovi-Harlig & Shin Reference Bardovi-Harlig and Shin2014) is to move away from the written DCT and use oral DCTs to improve the validity of pragmatics data collection.

4. The language learner: Evolving views of the learner

This section will trace the recent evolution of research on the learner, from investigations of variables related to individual differences (anxiety, motivation, or attitude) to more recent views of learner identity and agency and student perspectives about language learning.

Just as scholars in SA research have broadened their view of the learning context in SA, so, too, have researchers changed their conceptualizations of the language learner. This shift in perspective can broadly be summarized as moving from a view of learners as homogenous, yet metaphysically independent, processors of linguistic input to a conceptualization of the learner as a ‘whole person’ (Coleman Reference Coleman and Kinginger2013a). This transformation, which largely parallels trends in mainstream SLA, has occurred in stages. The initial stage comprised a cognitive view of the learner as an independent processor of linguistic information. Early on, however, scholars adopted a social psychological model of the language learner, examining individual difference variables and their effects on the SA experience. Block (Reference Block2003) noted that by the end of the twentieth century, a ‘social turn’ in language acquisition occurred, which greatly expanded our conceptualizations of language learning and learners. At this time, the research began to focus on learners as ‘individuals, with intentions, agency, affect, and . . . histories’ (Pavlenko & Lantolf Reference Pavlenko, Lantolf and Lantolf2000: 157). At about the same time, scholars also began to view learners as apprentices or ‘legitimate peripheral participants’ (Lave & Wenger Reference Lave and Wenger1991) trying to become fully fledged members of a particular community. Recently, following Coleman's (Reference Coleman and Kinginger2013a) injunction, researchers are beginning to recognize learners on SA as ‘whole persons’ rather than as simply learners. In this section, we examine the literature that reflects each conceptualization of the language learner. Early outcomes-based literature on SA treated language learners as homogenous processors of linguistic information. Paralleling the undifferentiated view of SA as a ‘context’ of language learning, scholars adopting this view tended to compare students on SA programs to students in other learning environments, especially AH learners and sometimes IM learners. Just as the context was assumed to be largely the same for each learner, and broadly comparable, so, too, were the learners in each program assumed to be similar enough as to invite aggregate data allowing scholars to draw conclusions about the effectiveness of each environment. This view of the language learner persists even in more recent studies of SA. For example, recent research has compared the learning outcomes for students on two types of short-term SA programs and those in domestic residential IM program (Martinsen et al. Reference Martinsen, Baker, Dewey, Bown and Johnson2010), while Llanes & Muñoz (Reference Llanes and Muñoz2013) have studied the difference in writing gains for SA and AH students, and Taguchi (Reference Taguchi2011) compared the pragmatic comprehension of classroom learners and SA learners as well.

Over time the persistent variability in learning outcomes led scholars to examine individual differences among learners, usually casting variables such as age, gender, aptitude, and motivation in terms of ‘predictors’ of language gains (Brecht et al. Reference Brecht, Davidson, Ginsberg and Freed1995; Davidson Reference Davidson2010). Thus, researchers sought to uncover the source of variation in traits that could be measured and isolated, taking a correlational approach. This so-called ‘social psychological’ view of the language learner is still employed in SA research (e.g., Pérez-Vidal Reference Pérez-Vidal, Barquin and Pérez-Vidal2014; Leonard & Shea Reference Leonard and Shea2017).

Studies on predictors of language gain provided some insight into possible variations in learning outcomes, but still took a relatively unnuanced view of language learners. In large measure, language learners were still seen as individual language processors with different abilities within different settings. Kinginger (Reference Kinginger2009: 37) notes that this search for ‘causal variables’ can lead to a view of language learners as balls in a pinball machine, in which the learner is a ‘hapless and unconscious object, identical to all its companion objects, run[ning] a chaotic and meaningless course through an immutable social structure,’ in which, in Schumann & Schumann's (Reference Schumann, Schumann, Brown, Crymes and Yorio1977: 248) words, ‘knobs represent the social, psychological, cognitive and personality variables.’

Despite its continued use in SA research, the social psychological approach to the language learner has been criticized on various grounds. Kinginger (Reference Kinginger2009: 156) points out that the broad array of affective and personality factors treated as ‘causal variables’ are not only dynamic, but ‘even volatile, responding to specific events and practical or emotional circumstances’ that can be ‘reshaped and even stamped out . . . at least temporarily.’ In fact, Pavlenko & Lantolf (Reference Pavlenko, Lantolf and Lantolf2000) argued that the meanings of fixed, apparently ‘biological’ factors such as age, gender, and race are largely dependent on culture, and that these factors may be socially constituted.

Critics of the social psychological approach to learners and learning argued for a view of learners as agents who ‘actively engage in constructing the terms and conditions of their own learning’ (Lantolf & Pavlenko Reference Lantolf, Pavlenko and Breen2001: 145) and who make choices about how they will participate—or not participate––in communities and practices they encounter. Scholars recognized that agency plays a central role in the construction of learner identities, degrees of learner autonomy, motivation, and strategic learner behavior (Toohey & Norton Reference Toohey, Norton, Palfreyman and Smith2003; Ushioda Reference Ushioda and Benson2007; Bown Reference Bown2009; Gao Reference Gao2010). Moreover, researchers adopted a more nuanced view of SA contexts, asserting that SA language learners are received in various ways. Though some may be welcomed and given ample opportunities for involvement, others may find the host community less welcoming, as a result of which, opportunities for participation are limited (Wilkinson Reference Wilkinson1998a). Though quantitative studies of language learners are less suited for exploring much of the complexity of language learning in SA contexts, researchers are attempting to deal with some of that complexity through use of factorial research design, especially for the generation of hypotheses. Scholars have turned their attention to studying factors that might explain learners’ behaviors. Whereas in previous research, L2 use was seen as a predictor of proficiency gain (Spada Reference Spada1986; Yager Reference Yager1998), more recently, Dewey et al. (Reference Dewey, Bown, Baker, Martinsen, Gold and Eggett2014) have examined the factors that contribute to L2 use, including personality, gender, preprogram proficiency, and size of social networks.

As views of the learner and of language learning have changed, many scholars have turned to qualitative research as means of comprehending emic (or insider perspectives), rather than the etic (researcher-focused perspectives), characteristic of the variables-based approach to research. In the mid-1990s, a number of qualitative studies of SA language learning emerged, including Polanyi (Reference Polanyi and Freed1995), Pellegrino (Reference Pellegrino1998, Reference Pellegrino2005), Wilkinson (Reference Wilkinson1998a, Reference Wilkinson1998b, Reference Wilkinson2002), and Talburt & Stewart (Reference Talburt and Stewart1999). These scholars explicitly examined learners’ perspectives on their SA experiences. In the ensuing years, an explosion in the number of qualitative studies has taken place, with many focusing on learners’ competing goals relative to language use and learning (Pellegrino Reference Pellegrino2005), as well as learners’ responses to unmet expectations and additional affective challenges (Allen Reference Allen2010a; Isabelli-García Reference Isabelli-García2010; van Maele, Vassilicos & Borghetti Reference van Maele, Vassilicos and Borghetti2016). These studies present learners as individuals, with intentions and goals and complex histories that often affect their motivation, their language learning behaviors, and their access to interactions (for a thorough review of literature related to identity and SA, see Kinginger Reference Kinginger2013a).

In examining language learners as individuals, qualitative research has also sought to understand the dynamic nature of learners’ motivations for language learning in SA and beyond. Research demonstrates that motives for studying a language are as complex as are the learners themselves; motives can be tied up with learners’ personal histories (Kinginger Reference Kinginger2004), their views of the target culture (Isabelli-García Reference Isabelli-García, DuFon and Churchill2006), their perceptions of the SA experience (Allen Reference Allen2010a), as well as with the discourses surrounding particular languages and cultures (Kinginger Reference Kinginger2004; Trentman & Diao Reference Trentman and Diao2015). Additional research has suggested that imagination plays an important role in sustaining motivation and motivated behavior; in particular, learners envision their ‘ideal selves’ (Dörnyei Reference Dörnyei, Dörnyei and Ushioda2009) interacting in ‘imagined communities’ of L2 speakers (Kinginger Reference Kinginger2004; Anderson Reference Anderson2006; Trentman Reference Trentman2013). Isabelli-García (Reference Isabelli-García, DuFon and Churchill2006) demonstrated the recursive relationship between motivation and participation in social networks, concluding that learners’ motivations ‘must be understood with reference to social relations of power that create the possibilities for language learners to speak’ (p. 254).

While the studies mentioned above hint at the role played by contacts in SA networks, especially teachers and host families, they tend to privilege the perspectives of the learners, who are often not equipped to fully understand the culturally bound meanings associated with particular practices and behaviors. Thus Kinginger (Reference Kinginger2013b) called for scholars to give more attention to the local meanings attached to particular practices and to the host community's perspectives (Stephenson Reference Stephenson1999; Knight & Schmidt-Rinehart Reference Knight and Schmidt-Rinehart2002; Schmidt-Rinehart & Knight Reference Schmidt-Rinehart and Knight2004). Recent research, emerging from a language socialization view of language learning, emphasizes local practices and perspectives (Cook Reference Cook, DuFon and Churchill2006; DuFon Reference DuFon, DuFon and Churchill2006; Kinginger Reference Kinginger, Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015; McMeekin Reference McMeekin2017).

Language socialization conceptualizes language learning as a process of socialization and not merely of acquisition. Learners are not construed as independent processors of linguistic forms, but rather as apprentices, striving to learn not only the practices of the host community but also the meanings attached to those practices by local members. Thus language learning involves more than the accumulation of competence by individuals; instead, it represents one aspect of becoming a person in society (Ochs Reference Ochs and Kramsch2002). Research examining the processes of language socialization in SA has provided important insights into how learners are received by their host cultures and how hosts socialize (or not) learners into particular practices (Cook Reference Cook, DuFon and Churchill2006; DuFon Reference DuFon, DuFon and Churchill2006; Kinginger Reference Kinginger, Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015; McMeekin Reference McMeekin2017).

While trends in SA research employ a broader and more nuanced view of language learners, we must keep in mind that ‘language learner’ may constitute only a very small part of the identity of SA participants (Kinginger Reference Kinginger2009; Coleman Reference Coleman, Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015). In fact, Kinginger (Reference Kinginger2013a) questions the extent to which participants are language learners at all. Even on language-focused SA programs, some learners adapt different stances toward language learning, resulting from their own goals and as a result of interactions with significant others. Indeed, Kinginger (Reference Kinginger2013a) argues that because participants do not privilege their identities as language learners, SA program administrators must be proactive in structuring the environment and in motivating students to learn the target language while abroad (Kinginger Reference Kinginger2010, Reference Kinginger2013a). As a result, a number of scholars have examined the effectiveness of various program interventions to promote interaction and foster self-regulation (Jackson Reference Jackson, Byram and Feng2006a: 132–185; Vande Berg et al. Reference Vande Berg, Connor-Linton and Paige2009; Kinginger Reference Kinginger2010; Allen & Dupuy Reference Allen and Dupuy2012; Di Silvio et al. Reference Di Silvio, Donovan and Malone2014; Belnap et al. Reference Belnap, Bown, Dewey, Belnap, Steffen, MacIntyre, Gregersen and Mercer2016).

An exclusive focus on SA language learning may obscure the most significant elements of the sojourn, including romance, self-discovery, and relationship building (Coleman Reference Coleman and Kinginger2013a). Recent SA research has moved beyond language learning, examining the effects of SA on learners’ identities, including religious identity (Poag & Sperandio Reference Poag and Sperandio2015), sexual identity (Bryant & Soria Reference Bryant and Soria2015), global engagement (Shadowen, Chieffo & Guerra Reference Shadowen, Chieffo and Guerra2015), and vocational identity (Kronholz & Osborn Reference Kronholz and Osborn2016). Whereas most of the previously cited research has focused on the impact of already-formed identities on SA experiences, scholars have begun to examine the impact of experiences of SA on the development of learners’ identities during and after SA (Benson et al. Reference Benson, Barkhuizen, Bodycott and Brown2013). In such studies, SA is viewed as a potentially ‘critical experience’ (Benson et al. Reference Benson, Barkhuizen, Bodycott and Brown2013: 33), in which L2 identity development is likely to be observable in students’ narratives of their experiences. This research agenda offers a broadened view of SA. SA is no longer seen as simply an environment for language learning and use; instead scholars recognize that SA can trigger identity processes and the building of new ways of learning and using languages.

This reversed direction of research highlights the complexity of SA, which can be seen as a social ecosystem in which all parts of the system are in constant flux. In such an ecosystem, any change can have consequences for the participants in the system and for the system in general. That is, ‘[u]nderstanding the study abroad phenomenon requires researchers to take into account the whole person and the whole context’ (Coleman Reference Coleman and Kinginger2013a: 36).

Coleman (Reference Coleman and Kinginger2013a) further notes, however, that, while SA has traditionally been seen as a subfield of applied linguistics, the broader truth is that the subfields of applied linguistics can be applied to SA research. Indeed an overview of recent research demonstrates that SA research is quite varied, addressing subfields of applied linguistics, such as individual differences, sociolinguistics, IM, and literacy. Scholars examine it from a variety of perspectives, fewer and fewer of which might seem to be readily related to language and language learning.

5. Pedagogical implications: Curricula for SA language education

5.1 Five categories of SA curriculum

Curricula, syllabi, pedagogical or teaching and learning approaches, specific course materials, readings or textbooks, course assignments, and formative and summative assessments receive little attention in most L2 SA research. This might be surprising given the potential of such to influence the linguistic, communicative, and intercultural learning outcomes that this research measures or explores. Perhaps because most research studies on SA are primarily interested in out-of-class opportunities for language use, studies measuring students’ L2 proficiencies usually provide only minimal description of the curricular program. They provide specific information related to the language research scope and design rather than a commitment to explore pedagogy and programming. Research or scholarly reports that solely or primarily focus on curriculum and pedagogy for SA, or the articulation of SA with domestic curricula, are rare.

More research on curricular issues is needed because, as Engle & Engle (Reference Engle and Engle1999: 43) maintain, ‘curriculum and extracurriculum are intimately linked.’ If instruction is given by a foreign instructor or offered by a foreign partner institution, the cultural norms of the host location will surely already be present in the classroom too—for either positive or negative effect (Brecht & Robinson Reference Brecht, Robinson and Freed1995; Polanyi Reference Polanyi and Freed1995; Bacon Reference Bacon2002; Kinginger Reference Kinginger2004; Pellegrino Reference Pellegrino2005; Churchill Reference Churchill, DuFon and Churchill2006). Nonetheless, the general issue for the field has been not knowing what students are doing in the target language while on SA (Clyne & Rizvi Reference Clyne, Rizvi, Davis and Olsen1998; Adams Reference Adams, DuFon and Churchill2006; Kinginger Reference Kinginger2009 Reference Kinginger2010; cf. Ranta & Meckelborg Reference Ranta and Meckelborg2013). Thoughtful curriculum design ensures that learners are engaging constructively with the SA IM environment. Plews, Breckenridge & Cambre (Reference Plews, Breckenridge and Cambre2010: 18) criticize the curriculum of mere exposure for leaving potential learning ‘to chance and individual initiative,’ and many scholars have called for more effective program structure or curricular interventions (e.g., Bertocchini & Costanzo Reference Bertocchini and Costanzo1996; Brierley & Coleman Reference Brierley and Coleman1997; Engle & Engle Reference Engle and Engle1999, Reference Engle, Engle, Grünzweig and Rinehart2002; Wilkinson Reference Wilkinson2000, Reference Wilkinson and Lavine2001; Byram, Nichols & Stevens Reference Byram, Nichols and Stevens2001; Paige et al. Reference Paige, Cohen, Kappler, Chi and Lassegard2002; Vande Berg Reference Vande Berg2007; Jackson Reference Jackson2008, Reference Jackson2010; Brewer & Cunningham Reference Brewer, Cunningham, Brewer and Cunningham2009; Vande Berg et al. Reference Vande Berg, Connor-Linton and Paige2009; Vande Berg, Paige & Lou Reference Vande Berg, Paige and Lou2012; Bown, Dewey & Belnap Reference Bown, Dewey, Belnap, Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015). After all, students do not acquire L2 skills by their mere presence in that unfiltered environment; rather, they learn ‘through individually constructing and through their membership in various cultural communities, co-constructing the world through the very act of perceiving it’ (Vande Berg et al. Reference Vande Berg, Paige and Lou2012: xv). Brewer & Cunningham (Reference Brewer, Cunningham, Brewer and Cunningham2009) identify factors pertinent to curriculum and pedagogy that impede such processing perception and successful SA: when participants remain too much in the company of co-nationals; when excursions are touristic rather than academically engaging; when students’ language skills are insufficient for effective communication; when students lack knowledge of the host culture; when they are unwilling to suspend judgment or take risks or are unprepared to work through disorienting experiences; when programs do not give students opportunities to reflect on and make sense of experiences for themselves, or make new sense of themselves.

A laissez-faire attitude to students’ out-of-class experience of IM in the L2 environment along with either importing domestic L2 class instruction into the host environment or sourcing classes offered by a foreign partner institution might exemplify one curriculum approach to SA. Beyond this, there are four ways in which SA researchers and program designers have attended to the experience of the SA environment also as curriculum and pedagogy. Thus, in the second curriculum category, there is the intentional use of the immersion environment as an extracurricular addition to a standard classroom course (Tang & Choi Reference Tang and Choi2004; Brubaker Reference Brubaker2007; Cohen & Shively Reference Cohen and Shively2007; Di Silvio et al. Reference Di Silvio, Donovan, Malone, Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015). Third, there is the explicit integration of course content and IM environment (Tschirner Reference Tschirner2007; Plews Reference Plews2013). Fourth, SA is understood as a process involving the three stages of moving from one place to another and back again (Lewis & Niesenbaum Reference Lewis and Niesenbaum2005; Jackson Reference Jackson, Byram and Feng2006a, Reference Jackson2006b, Reference Jackson2008, Reference Jackson2010). Finally, SA is largely or fully articulated with the domestic degree as a central component and means of achieving the overall curriculum goals (Brockington & Wiedenhoeft Reference Brockington, Wiedenhoeft and Lewin2009; Bathurst & La Brack Reference Bathurst, La Brack, Vande Berg, Paige and Lou2012).

5.2 Intentional addition

By way of intentional addition to a standard course, some SA programs add features to help learners take advantage of the immersion environment. A variety of interventions are discussed in the literature, including homestay, cultural excursions, and tandem partnerships. Di Silvio et al.’s (Reference Di Silvio, Donovan, Malone, Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015) study of homestay interaction provides a rare example of an intervention in this area. Host family members received one hour of training—including understanding their role, typical conversation contexts, and talking prompts—to help encourage meaningful interaction with American students of Mandarin, Russian, and Spanish. Brubaker's (Reference Brubaker2007) investigation of a short-term SA program for upper-level German students highlights the need for curricular interventions to help learners engage with the culture. She notes that ‘while culture-specific learning was certainly addressed in and out of class, the concept and process of culture learning was not explicitly presented or discussed, as is the case with most programs’ (p. 121). As a result, the students in her study did not possess a theoretical frame or develop the vocabulary necessary to express their thoughts, feelings, experiences, and observations, and most exhibited a passive attitude toward learning about culture. Thus Brubaker suggests exercises such as an iceberg inventory, cultural and personal differentiation, and other activities described by Paige et al.’s (Reference Paige, Cohen, Kappler, Chi and Lassegard2002) Maximizing study abroad to help students focus explicitly on the L2 culture (see also Goldoni Reference Goldoni2007). Such exercises are designed to assist intercultural and language learning but do not necessarily require immediate L2 use; however, Cohen et al. (Reference Cohen, Paige, Shively, Emert and Hoff2005) report statistically significant language gains in students from assigned weekly readings from the Paige et al. (Reference Paige, Cohen, Kappler, Chi and Lassegard2002) guide.

In SA programs designed specifically for pre- and in-service teachers, immersive cultural experiences include ‘visiting schools’ (Badia Reference Badia1994: 137) or ‘teacher contact with schools in the host country’ (Roskvist et al. Reference Roskvist, Harvey, Corder and Stacey2013: 7). For example, Tang & Choi (Reference Tang and Choi2004) describe an English IM program for Hong Kong pre-service teachers in Canada and Australia that includes fieldwork in schools. Participants were required to conduct ‘classroom observation, curriculum and lesson planning as well as individual or team teaching with the support of mentor teachers in the schools they are placed’ (p. 53), though this specialist experience was not formally linked to the IM language class or to the ongoing course of study AH. Nonetheless, Tang & Choi claim the international experience led to increased language competence and personal growth. Plews et al. (Reference Plews, Breckenridge and Cambre2010, Reference Plews, Breckenridge, Cambre and Fernandes2014) investigate a residence abroad program for in-service Mexican teachers of English that required them to assist Spanish classes at school and university in Canada. The researchers find that homestay for teachers specifically with host-country teachers or education administrators strengthened professional and disciplinary knowledge and potential access to professional networks.

Engle & Engle (Reference Engle and Engle1999) take a more proactive stance with the extracurricular IM context by recommending mandatory activities that aim to integrate students into the local culture and encourage reflection on the home culture. In addition to the regular academic course and exploring local culture independently or with fellow students, their program in France requires American students to participate in a weekly conversation exchange with a tandem partner (see also, e.g., Badstübner & Ecke Reference Badstübner and Ecke2009; Wilkinson Reference Wilkinson, Doiz, Lasagabaster and Sierra2012; see Bown et al. Reference Bown, Dewey, Belnap, Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015, for hired conversation partners), pursue a personal interest or hobby by joining a local club or organization (see also, e.g., Fraser Reference Fraser2002; Schmidt-Rinehart & Knight Reference Schmidt-Rinehart and Knight2004), and contribute community service (see also, e.g., Kiely & Nielson Reference Kiely and Nielson2003; Lewis & Niesenbaum Reference Lewis and Niesenbaum2005; Parker & Dautoff Reference Parker and Dautoff2007). Engle & Engle (Reference Engle and Engle1999) claim that these interventions are enriching since they help students break out of the co-national group passively consuming the SA destination and instead engage with it personally. The success of these activities depends on the foreign tandem partners’ self-interest in connecting with the SA students, the students’ linguistic preparation (especially for pursuing their personal interest or hobby), student mentoring, and the fact that the students had selected the program precisely because it offered such mandatory components (for a recent review of the program, see Engle & Engle Reference Engle, Engle, Vande Berg, Paige and Lou2012). Vande Berg et al. (Reference Vande Berg, Connor-Linton and Paige2009) found that the cultural mentoring in their program positively affected language proficiency and intercultural development.

Another intervention includes journaling, which facilitates reflections on homestay experiences and informal interactions with local people and culture. Plews & Misfeldt (Reference Plews and Misfeldt2016) indicate that most student journals discussed in SA research are introduced as part of the research study design to gather data on intercultural awareness and not as a pedagogical intervention (in the target language) intended to enhance students’ language acquisition or linguistic awareness. In fact, Paige et al. (Reference Paige, Cohen, Kappler, Chi and Lassegard2002), who describe procedures for effective use of journaling, view this activity primarily as a tool for culture learning rather than language learning. However, in a small number of studies, journals are used as pedagogical interventions to facilitate language learning. For example, Bown et al. (Reference Bown, Dewey, Belnap, Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015) describe the use of ‘weekly speaking journals’ in which learners reflected on their interactions in the previous week and planned ways to improve their interactions in the coming week. Nevertheless, in this study, students wrote their journals in their L1. In a program described by Rollmann (Reference Rollmann, Lorey, Plews and Rieger2007), students wrote in their L2 to record their daily experiences and list new vocabulary. However, they were allowed to choose between the L1 or the L2 to record their impressions of the culture and to discuss problems.

Journals can also be used to help learners reflect on cultural differences, especially if, as Paige et al. (Reference Paige, Cohen, Kappler, Chi and Lassegard2002) suggest, they are not viewed as mere records of itinerary. Instead, the authors propose structuring journal assignments according to four categories: impressions (of people, places, things, ideas, etc.), descriptive (a picture of an event, etc.), narrative (stories of experiences), and expressive (evaluations). Ingram (Reference Ingram2005) and Raschio (Reference Raschio2001) describe programs in which students kept journals to record and interpret differences between their home country and the host country. In Bridges’ (Reference Bridges2007) study, journals allowed pre-service teachers studying abroad in Australia to gauge progress in intercultural and interpersonal development, confidence in English, and awareness of personal language proficiency, and to raise critical awareness of language use, pedagogy and classroom practice, and both Hong Kong and Australian culture.

Blogging represents a contemporary form of journaling. Lee (Reference Lee2011) analyzes the use of personal blogs, a class blog, and project blogs by Americans in Spain to increase engagement with the target culture and interactions with Spanish speakers out of class. She concludes that the interaction with Spaniards and the collaboration required by the blogs increased students’ learner autonomy, sense of belonging, and ability to compare and exchange ideas about the two cultures, which led to intercultural competence development.

5.3 Explicit integration

The third category concerns programming that formally combines language and culture course content and assignments with engagement with the IM environment and community. Particular teaching and learning approaches are central to this endeavor, as is the intention to avoid remaining ‘touristic’ (Lewis & Niesenbaum Reference Lewis and Niesenbaum2005: 258). Tschirner's (Reference Tschirner2007) study of an intensive IM for pre- and in-service American teachers of German included curriculum or program structures such as knowledge about the development of oral proficiency, intensive language instruction, as well as course projects designed to increase interaction with locals and homestays and/or tandem partners. Thus, in addition to three hours of daily intensive language practice through task- and genre-based instruction, participants in this program had to work for a further three hours daily on small-group cultural research projects for in-course presentations.

Similar to Tschirner's project-based course component, Plews (Reference Plews2013) used task-based language teaching in an intensive advanced German language and culture IM course to raise students’ sense of personal, emotional, and creative ownership of the target language. All class tasks (e.g., drawing family trees, visiting a Turkish restaurant, mapping the local Turkish-German community, investigating the rules of romantic dating, designing local flyers) and the themes of the capstone task (individual ethnographic studies) were related to a contemporary novel read in the course. Misfeldt (Reference Misfeldt, Plews and Schmenk2013) used the reading of a novel and drama pedagogy in an upper-intermediate course in the same program to encourage students to engage with Germans and German culture, and to build vocabulary through personal experience, discussing challenging topics with each other and their hosts, and access different kinds of intercultural knowledge and self-knowledge.

Ethnography is emerging as an especially popular and effective approach for integrating the experience of IM into the SA curriculum (Roberts et al. Reference Roberts, Byram, Baro, Jordan and Street2001). In preparing ethnographic assignments, students are taught techniques for participant observation, making field notes, keeping a reflective journal, attentive interviewing, and processing and analyzing data. The presentation of the ethnographic study is fundamental to completing the process. Jackson (Reference Jackson, Byram and Feng2006a, Reference Jackson2006b, Reference Jackson2008, Reference Jackson2010) describes ethnographic research as a central component of a short-term SA. She chooses this approach since ‘ethnographic projects require sojourners to move beyond the safety of their group and venture into unfamiliar territory,’ ultimately using unscripted language for purposeful communication (Reference Jackson2006b: 93). Participants in Thompson's (Reference Thompson2002) investigation of American teachers of Spanish studying in Spain gained considerable conversational interaction with L1 Spanish speakers by conducting ethnographic interviews on topics of their own choice. These interviews helped learners refresh their linguistic knowledge, acquire cultural knowledge, and improve their attitudes toward Spaniards.

Brockington & Wiedenhoeft (Reference Brockington, Wiedenhoeft and Lewin2009) report on two projects that enrich students’ language, offer meaningful interaction with L2 speakers, involve learners in their own cultural learning process, increase their knowledge of other cultures, and enable self-reflection. The two projects include the Earlham College ethnographic project, undertaken as part of a field studies course or internship abroad, and Kalamazoo College's ethnographic-like project, the Integrative Cultural Research Project (ICRP), reworked from the former program. Whereas the Earlham College project tries to shape students’ worldview by having them critically compare their local placement with home, the ICRP engages with local communities in order to acquire an ‘understanding [of] how and where the activity at the project site is located in a larger cultural context’ (p. 126). Here, the L2 is used to increase the participants’ firsthand experience and understanding of the everyday lives of local people not solely for the participants’ sake, but for the sake of the hosts. At stake for the participants is achieving meaningful interaction with local people and improving analytical skills. In this project, students learn and use the ethical transformative method of DIVE: ‘Describe in value neutral terms. Interpret what is happening within the local context. Verify your interpretation with a local person. Evaluate how well it seems to be working within the local context’ (p. 126). Brockington & Wiedenhoeft underline that description and reflection are key to the success of ICRP since they help students come to terms with the external observations and challenges and internal feelings of dissonance presented by the SA context and thereby make sense of their surroundings. But it is unclear whether the description and reflection are written in the L2.

5.4 Three-stage process

The fourth way of attending to the immersion environment as curriculum and pedagogy conceives the L2 SA experience as part of a learning process or cycle of (two or) three stages that begins with guided preparation before departure, optimally includes specified engagement beyond the classroom during the sojourn, and preferably continues with a reflective and interpretive project drawing on SA experiences after returning to the home campus. This thinking underlies the culture- and language-learning strategies in Paige et al.’s (Reference Paige, Cohen, Kappler, Chi and Lassegard2002) Maximizing study abroad, in which activities for intercultural learning and each language skill are subdivided for pre-departure, in-country, and post-study-abroad. This category concerns a smaller group of reported programs for ‘typical study abroad programs’ and ‘do not include pre- or post-sojourn elements or specially designed components during the study abroad’ (Jackson Reference Jackson2006b: 94). In these examples, the pre-departure phase involves more than a general travel orientation offered by the home institution, ranging from linking to course content already discussed on campus (Lewis & Niesenbaum Reference Lewis and Niesenbaum2005) to a preceding course with directed readings and discussion of expectations in addition to previewing the itinerary and other practical information (Ingram Reference Ingram2005; Wolf Reference Wolf2007; Schwieter & Kunert Reference Schwieter, Kunert, Chamness Miller, Watze and Mantero2012), web-based courses (Rollmann Reference Rollmann, Lorey, Plews and Rieger2007), chat or other online exchanges (Godwin-Jones Reference Godwin-Jones2016), or an intercultural communication course and the development of research skills required for ethnographic work (Jackson Reference Jackson, Byram and Feng2006a, Reference Jackson2006b, Reference Jackson2008, Reference Jackson2009, Reference Jackson2010; Lou & Bosley Reference Lou, Bosley, Vande Berg, Paige and Lou2012). The short-term German program described by Rollmann (Reference Rollmann, Lorey, Plews and Rieger2007) comprises three courses that begin online 12 weeks prior to the SA component. These courses are domestic courses revised to focus on practical vocabulary building for visiting Germany, the local history and culture of the destination city, and conversation practice (to enable students to describe themselves and where they come from) and grammar review, and are completed overseas in combination with a language class for international students and optional excursions offered by the partner institution. Dewaele et al. (Reference Dewaele, Comanaru, Faraco, Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015) prove that a 2–4-week on-site pre-sessional course had a positive impact on participants’ affect and willingness to communicate by preparing them for French social and academic life.

The in- and post-sojourn components of the three-stage SA programs often require journals or the collection of material and data for ethnographic study that are then reviewed and analyzed for the purpose of term papers, class presentations, or ongoing work on the student's sense of linguistic and (inter-)cultural self (Jackson Reference Jackson, Byram and Feng2006a, Reference Jackson2006b; Wolf Reference Wolf2007). However, even when a contextualized pre-departure and in-program curriculum is provided, post-sojourn activities can still be lacking or concern informal choices such as enrolling in further language courses, recalling the SA experience for outreach presentations or independent study credits, attending another SA program, and opting for a major in languages (e.g., Ingram Reference Ingram2005; Rollmann Reference Rollmann, Lorey, Plews and Rieger2007; Wolf Reference Wolf2007). Wilson et al. (Reference Wilson, Brain, Brown, Gaind, Radan and Redmond2016) highlight the need to go beyond the typical institutional, optional reintegration session to realize the full potential of SA as experiential learning. They suggest requiring a critically reflective project that is researched while abroad, developed AH, and presented to fellow returnees, newly arrived international students, and non-SA students on the home campus. Digital technology offers possibilities for extending the participants’ reflections and insights for the benefit of future participants (Rodríguez Reference Rodríguez2010; Hampton Reference Hampton, Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015; Godwin-Jones Reference Godwin-Jones2016). Also, Hampton (Reference Hampton2016) describes a novel UK Routes into Languages project in which British undergraduate students of French volunteer to collect realia with linguistic and cultural value during their year abroad to enhance language education at UK secondary schools after their return home. Her data show that the social engagement required when acting as cultural intermediaries enhanced the participants’ experience.

One of the most extensively analyzed examples of the three-stage SA curriculum process is provided by Jackson's (Reference Jackson, Byram and Feng2006a, Reference Jackson2006b, Reference Jackson2008, Reference Jackson2009, Reference Jackson2010) work on a 5-week ‘Special English Stream’ program, which was designed to enhance the linguistic, communicative, cultural, intellectual, and interpersonal knowledge and skills of Hong Kong English majors. The program goals were based on Byram's (Reference Byram1997) model of intercultural communicative competence and addressed ethnocentrism, cultural world views, native-speaker communication, and the critical evaluation of both host and home cultures. The initial stage included a literature seminar with readings related to the cultural itinerary and a linguistics seminar in which students developed ethnographic research techniques and were required to conduct a ‘small scale ethnographic project to explore their own cultural world’ (Jackson Reference Jackson, Byram and Feng2006a: 139). The next stage included homestay, courses on English literature, language, and current affairs, a cultural program, informal interaction with other international students, and a field journal to record impressions of intercultural encounters and communication. The subsequent stage involved meetings with the researcher-director to analyze critical incidents described in the students’ diary entries and ethnographic reports. Jackson (Reference Jackson, Byram and Feng2006a, Reference Jackson2006b) finds that language gains varied among the students but that those with positive attitudes and experiences invested more in building relationships and initiated communication more often, and thereby became more confident in speaking English. However, Jackson's (Reference Jackson2009) continued examination cautions that SA is no guarantee of intercultural competence and L2 acquisition, that the two do not necessarily develop in parallel, and that some students might gain an advanced level in the L2 but remain ethnocentric. Nonetheless, she maintains that ‘well-planned pre-sojourn preparation, adequate socio-emotional support during the program, and post-sojourn debriefings can prompt and sustain deeper levels of language and intercultural learning’ (Jackson Reference Jackson2009: 14).

5.5 Full articulation

In the preceding category, courses or tasks within the domestic curriculum serve the SA program's linguistic and intercultural learning goals. In the fifth category of understanding the IM environment as curriculum and pedagogy, SA is fully articulated with and integrated into the domestic curriculum, as the central learning opportunity directly serving the overall curriculum goals. Examples include the aforementioned Earlham and Kalamazoo programs, whose ethnographic studies are discussed by Brockington & Wiedenhoeft (Reference Brockington, Wiedenhoeft and Lewin2009), as well as the SA experience in the School of International Studies at the University of the Pacific described by Bathurst & La Brack (Reference Bathurst, La Brack, Vande Berg, Paige and Lou2012). Streitwieser (Reference Streitwieser and Lewin2009) proposes that research-oriented undergraduate SA shape the 4-year North American degree in order to graduate culturally and globally responsible future professionals (see also Brockington & Wiedenhoeft Reference Brockington, Wiedenhoeft and Lewin2009: 131). The characteristics of such programming would include pre-departure preparation aimed at training students to develop research proposals based on their personal interests and with culturally appropriate research designs for collecting and analyzing data abroad to be approved by the home institution, on-site adviser guidance in relation to being sensitive to the cultural and interactional norms of the foreign context, the ability to conduct research in an L2, and accountability through the thoughtful presentation and use of the research upon return to the home campus (see Streitwieser Reference Streitwieser and Lewin2009: 402–407). Such an extensive integration of SA into the curriculum requires broad institutional structures, including an internationalized curriculum per se, scholarships reserved for SA, a variety of types of SA, and subject courses taught in the target language (see Wanner Reference Wanner and Lewin2009). For these reasons, Wanner (Reference Wanner and Lewin2009) advocates a ‘realistic’ rather than a ‘maximal model,’ hoping that short-term programs will motivate students to incorporate language in their curriculum choices and undertake long-term SA. One example of the realistic model of L2 SA articulated with the domestic curriculum is the integration of Spanish and biology for cultural and environmental conservation in Costa Rica reported by Gorka & Niesenbaum (Reference Gorka and Niesenbaum2001).

6. Conclusions

The future of SA research of students participating in programs in non-English-speaking countries may parallel the decline of FL study in the US (Goldberg, Looney & Lusin Reference Goldberg, Looney and Lusin2015). That is, the trend that globalization is leading to the dominance of English-language proficiency goals in educational policies due to its status as the lingua franca may be reducing the urgency of language acquisition of non-English languages. Despite the decline of FL study in the US, US-based mobile learners are the ones that are most represented in non-English-speaking countries.

This trend may, therefore, explain why the great majority of SA projects involve US-based learners of commonly taught languages. Despite this trend, UNESCO has reported new trends in global student mobility. For example,

Australia and Japan, traditional destinations in East Asia and the Pacific, are rivaled by newcomers China, Malaysia, the Republic of Korea, Singapore and New Zealand, which hosted 7% of the global share of mobile students in 2013. In the Arab States, Egypt, Saudi Arabia and the United Arab Emirates are making efforts to recruit students from abroad. These three countries hosted 4% of the global share of mobile students. (UNESCO Institute of Statistics 2014)

The current research on various aspects of the SA process in emerging education hubs includes China (e.g., Han Reference Han and Lee2008; Mu Reference Mu and Lee2008; Du Reference Du2013; Taguchi, Li & Xiao Reference Taguchi, Li and Xiao2013), Malaysia (e.g., Baharun, Awang & Padlee Reference Baharun, Awang and Padlee2011; Knight & Morshidi Reference Knight and Morshidi2011; Zamberi Ahmad & Buchanan Reference Zamberi Ahmad and Buchanan2017), Republic of Korea (e.g., Kim Reference Kim and Kondo-Brown2006; Beausoleil Reference Beausoleil2008; Brown Reference Brown and Kinginger2013; Jon, Lee & Byun Reference Jon, Lee and Byun2014), Singapore (e.g., Olds Reference Olds2007; Collins et al. Reference Collins, Sidhu, Lewis and Yeoh2014), Egypt (e.g., Dewey et al. Reference Dewey, Ring, Gardner and Belnap2013; Trentman Reference Trentman2013; Mercer Reference Mercer2015), Saudi Arabia, and the UAE (e.g., Paris, Nyaupane & Teye Reference Paris, Nyaupane and Teye2014; Zamberi Ahmad & Buchanan Reference Zamberi Ahmad and Buchanan2017). The majority of the studies, however, focus on the reasons that international students choose to study in these regions. It is recommended that more researchers try to focus on SA experiences in these destinations.

The nascent field of student mobility research has, up to now, focused primarily on the student in the SA setting. Some methodologies use SA as a categorical label and others, more recently, as a social ecosystem (de Bot, Lowie & Verspoor Reference de Bot, Lowie and Verspoor2007). SA used as a categorical label treats the experience abroad usually in opposition to AH or domestic IM. That is, it is used as the main factor to explain L2 acquisition. Taguchi (Reference Taguchi2016) states that ‘by design, these [study abroad as a black box/categorical label] studies are not concerned with what actually goes on in a SA context. Features of SA—exposure, social contact, and cultural experiences—are all consolidated into this physical context . . . without any internal components available for inspection’ (p. 5). Nonetheless, the notion of individual differences has resided in statistical analyses and results but has not been addressed more broadly. On the other hand, SA is treated as a social ecosystem that is dynamic, where interacting factors shape L2 acquisition. Researchers in this category take interacting, internal, dynamic subsystems into account to explain the L2 learning process. The complex, dynamic systems theory encapsulates this approach and ‘allows us to merge the social and the cognitive aspects of SLA and shows how their interaction can lead to development’ (de Bot et al. Reference de Bot, Lowie and Verspoor2007: 19). The dynamic systems approach takes into account the effect of internal resources—‘the capacity to learn, time to learn, internal informational resources such as conceptual knowledge, and motivational resources’ (de Bot et al. Reference de Bot, Lowie and Verspoor2007: 11)—to explain the L2 learning process as well as studying the role of input via situation practice and via extended, dense social networks (e.g., Isabelli-García Reference Isabelli-García, DuFon and Churchill2006; Kurata Reference Kurata2011; Dewey, Bown & Egget Reference Dewey, Bown and Eggett2012; Dewey et al. Reference Dewey, Ring, Gardner and Belnap2013; Shiri Reference Shiri2015). Using qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods, research is confirming that these students are complex agents. They show ‘that learner beliefs are dynamic . . . implying that language learner beliefs are bi-directionally related to the perceptions of study abroad sojourns’ (Kaypak & Ortaçtepe Reference Kaypak and Ortaçtepe2014: 364).

King & Raghuram (Reference King and Raghuram2013) now call for a need to ‘decenter the student as the object of study and instead recognise the multiple players’ (p. 134) invested in and gaining from student mobility. They suggest the need for detailed ethnographic research ‘to document their complex lives in the academic, social, cultural, and economic realms’ (p. 135). This approach could include biographical research (see Roberts [Reference Roberts2002] for more information), be multi-sited (such as the LANGSNAP project [2013]), and follow the students back home to continue post-SA research.

King & Raghuram (Reference King and Raghuram2013) also invite researchers to delve into the available survey data and other datasets and carry out statistical analyses, improve theorization of student mobility (e.g., Coleman Reference Coleman2013b, Reference Coleman, Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015; Xiao & Wray Reference Xiao and Wray2016), and include gendered analysis to explain the dynamics of SA experiences (e.g., Trentman Reference Trentman, Mitchell, Tracy-Ventura and McManus2015; King & Sondhi Reference King and Sondhi2016). Jensen & Howard (Reference Jensen, Howard, Roberts, Vedder and Hulstijn2014: 32) highlight that ‘while SA has proven to be highly beneficial in certain areas, such as fluency, sociolinguistic competence and lexical development, it appears to have a less positive effect on grammatical development compared to the classroom setting.’ However, learners may evidence development if they spend longer amounts of time abroad, and it is imperative that studies include the effects of longer lengths of time on grammatical development (e.g., Nishida & Isabelli Reference Nishida, Isabelli and Eddington2005; Davidson Reference Davidson2010; Serrano Reference Serrano2010; Llanes Reference Llanes and Muñoz2012; Pérez-Vidal et al. Reference Pérez-Vidal, Juan-Garau, Mora, Valls-Ferrer and Muñoz2012; Serrano, Tragant & Llanes Reference Serrano, Tragant and Llanes2012; Jensen & Howard Reference Jensen, Howard, Roberts, Vedder and Hulstijn2014) and differences in speed of processing (Taguchi Reference Taguchi2015, Reference Taguchi2016). Other suggestions are that future studies should try to collect their data at several points in time (Serrano Reference Serrano2010; Stokoe Reference Stokoe2013).

In addition, SA programs vary widely in their framework, which may explain the inconclusive findings in SLA research in SA (Llanes Reference Llanes2011; DeKeyser Reference DeKeyser and Pérez-Vidal2014; Sanz Reference Sanz and Pérez-Vidal2014). For example, Grey et al. (Reference Grey, Cox, Serafini and Sanz2015: 138) note the ways a program can differ, including ‘the duration of the program, students’ proficiency in the target language upon entry, the type and context of course work, the type of student housing, and the nature and extent of opportunities for guided/structured cultural interaction, reflection, and experiential learning, among other areas (see Engle & Engle Reference Engle and Engle1999).’ The publication of the Boren Report (Mason, Powers & Donnelly Reference Mason, Powers and Donnelly2015)—focusing on non-English cognate language acquisition in SA contexts—‘acknowledged that curricular and affective variables constitute key elements, and researchers have admitted that existing on foreign soil does not necessarily guarantee active engagement with speakers of the language and members of the local community’ (Miano, Bernhardt & Brates Reference Miano, Bernhardt and Brates2016: 289).

Regardless, research focusing on linguistic constructs and gains in SA has recently become much more complex. Spoelman & Verspoor (Reference Spoelman and Verspoor2010: 547) observe that ‘we now know from recent studies that many aspects of language development are nonlinear and . . . multivariate and dynamic’ and that each subcomponent may interact with other subcomponents and exhibit its own developmental dynamics (Larsen-Freeman Reference Larsen-Freeman2006; Norris & Ortega Reference Norris and Ortega2009). In order to meet the challenges of investigating the intricate interaction between elements, Larsen-Freeman (Reference Larsen-Freeman2009), therefore, calls for ‘more longitudinal and nonlinear research, in which difference and variation occupy a central role, and for a broader conceptual framework, such as that offered by dynamic or complex systems theory (Larsen-Freeman & Cameron Reference Larsen-Freeman and Cameron2008; Verspoor, de Bot & Lowie Reference Verspoor, de Bot and Lowie2011)’ (Housen, Kuiken & Vedder Reference Housen, Kuiken, Vedder, Housen, Kuiken and Vedder2012: 8).

A variety of SA program frameworks apply both to the out-of-classroom IM on SA as well as to the in-class and course approaches, assignments, goals, and expectations of so-called ‘island’ programs or the host culture education system. Unlike research trends, the student does not necessarily distinguish these two domains when processing language and culture, but rather draws on both and integrates the potential learning of each. Thus, to neglect one or the other in the SA research would be like reading only the even pages of a book. Somehow one might put together much of the story without the odd pages, but there will always be important missing elements that contribute to the story. While careful study of a particular even or odd page may be necessary at times, the whole story cannot be conceptualized without both. Although it may be possible to understand a key element of SA by focusing more on the classroom or more on the IM experience, both contribute to the story and are therefore necessary to understand the entire experience and outcomes.

As the preceding sections have shown, SA can be highly productive for language learning. However, there are many aspects of language learning in this setting that are not yet fully understood, and research in this area should keep pace with the broader field of applied linguistics, invoking new theories and agendas and applying new methodologies.

Questions arising

  1. 1. How can the SA research community better collaborate, share data and collectively come to a clearer understanding of commonalities and variation in SA experiences?

  2. 2. Given that research has largely ignored the perspectives of members of the host culture, how can scholars design research to give voice to the hosts?

  3. 3. What role does literacy play in learners’ SA experiences? How does SA influence the development of literacy and other abilities not typically valued in SA?

  4. 4. How do learners’ various identities shape their experiences on SA?

  5. 5. Are there innovative means of assessment that might be more appropriate for capturing SA linguistic gains than currently used global assessments (such as the OPI)?

  6. 6. Are there innovative means of assessment that might be more appropriate for specific research purposes or settings other than currently used methods of assessing language gains, intercultural competence development, and other outcomes?

  7. 7. What role does learner self-regulation play in shaping an individual's experience abroad?

  8. 8. What influence will continually evolving technologies have on the SA experience in the future?

  9. 9. How will SA research knowledge change if/once SA curricula, teaching approaches, and learning materials or activities are taken more into account by researchers?

  10. 10. How can we better explore the relationship between in-class teaching and learning while on SA and out-of-class experience and engagement?

Christina Isabelli-García, Professor and Chair of the Department of Modern Languages and Literature at Gonzaga University, received a Ph.D. in Ibero-Romance Philology and Linguistics from the University of Texas at Austin. She teaches courses in linguistics, SLA, and language pedagogy. She has also taught at Illinois Wesleyan University and Middlebury College. Her research interests relate to SLA in various learning situations, identifying processes of forming social networks abroad and how they function as contexts for language learning.

Jennifer Bown, Associate Professor of Russian at Brigham Young University, received her Ph.D. from The Ohio State University in Slavic linguistics. Her research interests include self-regulated language learning, particularly in non-traditional contexts such as SA, distance learning, individualized instruction, and FL housing, with a particular interest in the role of affective and social factors in language learning. Her articles have appeared in Modern Language Journal, Critical Issues in Language Studies, and Language Learning, among others.

John L. Plews is Professor of Modern Languages (German) at Saint Mary's University, Halifax, Canada. He is also the Director of the Canadian Summer School in Germany and President of the Canadian Association of University Teachers of German. He earned a Ph.D. in German Literatures, Languages, & Linguistics and a Ph.D. in Secondary Education, both from the University of Alberta. He researches L2 curriculum and teaching, second language learner identities, and SA for language learners and language teachers.

Dan P. Dewey, Professor of Linguistics at Brigham Young University, received a Ph.D. in SLA from Carnegie Mellon University. His research focuses primarily on how people use an L2 outside of the classroom and includes work on study abroad, internships abroad, FL housing, and computer-assisted learning, etc. He also studies language assessment, self-assessment, and motivation and SLA.

References

Adams, R. (2006). Language learning strategies in the study abroad context. In DuFon, M. A. & Churchill, E. (eds.), 259–292.Google Scholar
Al-Gahtani, S. & Roever, C. (2015). The development of requests by L2 learners of Modern Standard Arabic: A longitudinal and cross-sectional study. Foreign Language Annals 48.4, 570583.Google Scholar
Allen, H. W. (2010a). Interactive contact during study abroad as linguistic affordance: Myth or reality? Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad 19, 126.Google Scholar
Allen, H. W. (2010b). Language-learning motivation during short-term study abroad: An activity theory perspective. Foreign Language Annals 43.1, 2749.Google Scholar
Allen, H. W. (2013). Self-regulatory strategies of foreign language learners. In Kinginger, C. (ed.), 47–83.Google Scholar
Allen, H. W. & Dupuy, B. (2012). Study abroad, foreign language use, and the Communities Standard. Foreign Language Annals 45.4, 468493.Google Scholar
Allen, H. W. & Herron, C. (2003). A mixed-methodology investigation of the linguistic and affective outcomes of summer study abroad. Foreign Language Annals 36.3, 370385.Google Scholar
Anderson, B. (2006). Imagined communities: Reflections on the origin and spread of nationalism. London: Verso Books.Google Scholar
Arnett, C. (2013). Syntactic gains in short-term study abroad. Foreign Language Annals 46.4, 705712.Google Scholar
Avello, P. & Lara, R. (2014). Phonological development in L2 speech production during study abroad programmes differing in length of stay. In Pérez-Vidal, C. (ed.), 137–166.Google Scholar
Bachman, L. F. (1990). Fundamental considerations in language testing. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bacon, S. (2002). Learning the rules: Language development and cultural adjustment during study abroad. Foreign Language Annals 35.6, 637646.Google Scholar
Badia, A. (1994). Teaching FLES methodology abroad. Hispania 77.1, 134137.Google Scholar
Badstübner, T. & Ecke, P. (2009). Student expectations, motivations, target language use, and perceived learning progress in a summer study abroad program in Germany. Unterrichtspraxis 42.1, 4149.Google Scholar
Baharun, R., Awang, Z. & Padlee, S. (2011). International students’ choice criteria for selection of higher learning in Malaysian private universities. African Journal of Business Management 5.12, 47044714.Google Scholar
Baker-Smemoe, W., Dewey, D. P., Bown, J. & Martinsen, R. A. (2014). Variables affecting L2 gains during study abroad. Foreign Language Annals 47.3, 464486.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. & Shin, S. Y. (2014). Expanding traditional testing measures with tasks from L2 pragmatics research. Iranian Journal of Language Testing 4.1, 2649.Google Scholar
Barquin, E. (2012). Writing development in a study abroad context. Ph.D. dissertation, Universitat Pompeu Fabra.Google Scholar
Bataller, R. (2010). Making a request for a service in Spanish: Pragmatic development in the study abroad setting. Foreign Language Annals 43.1, 160175.Google Scholar
Bathurst, L. & La Brack, B. (2012). Shifting the locus of intercultural learning: Intervening prior to and after student experiences abroad. In Vande Berg, M., Paige, R. M. & Lou, K. H. (eds.), 261283.Google Scholar
Beausoleil, A. (2008). Understanding heritage and ethnic identity development through study abroad: The case of South Korea. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Santa Barbara.Google Scholar
Beaven, A. & Spencer-Oatey, H. (2016). Cultural adaptation in different facets of life and the impact of language: A case study of personal adjustment patterns during study abroad. Language and Intercultural Communication 16.3, 349367.Google Scholar
Bella, S. (2012). Pragmatic development in a foreign language: A study of Greek FL requests. Journal of Pragmatics 44, 19171947.Google Scholar
Belnap, R. K., Bown, J., Dewey, D. P., Belnap, L. P. & Steffen, P. R. (2016). Project perseverance: Helping learners become self-regulating learners. In MacIntyre, P. D., Gregersen, T. & Mercer, S. (eds.), Positive psychology in SLA. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, 282301.Google Scholar
Benson, P., Barkhuizen, G. P., Bodycott, P. & Brown, J. (2013). Second language identity in narratives of study abroad. London: Palgrave MacMillan.Google Scholar
Bertocchini, P. & Costanzo, E. (1996, July). L'Europe et l'autoformation des enseignants de langues. Français dans le Monde 282, 6266.Google Scholar
Block, D. (2003). The social turn in SLA. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Boersma, P. & Weenink, D. (2013). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.3.41) [Computer program]. www.praat.org.Google Scholar
Bongiovanni, S., Long, A. Y., Solon, M. & Willis, E. W. (2015). The effect of short-term study abroad on second language Spanish phonetic development. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics 8.2, 243283.Google Scholar
Bordón, T. & Liskin-Gasparro, J. E. (2014). The assessment and evaluation of Spanish. In Lacorte, M. (ed.), The Routledge handbook of Hispanic applied linguistics. New York: Routledge, 258274.Google Scholar
Bown, J. (2009). Self-regulatory strategies and agency in self-instructed language learning: A situated view. Modern Language Journal 93.4, 570583.Google Scholar
Bown, J., Dewey, D. P. & Belnap, R. K. (2015). Student interactions during study abroad in Jordan. In Mitchell, R., Tracy-Ventura, N. & McManus, K. (eds.), 199–222.Google Scholar
Brecht, R. & Robinson, J. L. (1995). The value of formal instruction in study abroad: Student reactions in context. In Freed, B. (ed.), 317–334.Google Scholar
Brecht, R., Davidson, D. & Ginsberg, R. B. (1995). Predictors of foreign language gain during study abroad. In Freed, B. (ed.), 37–66.Google Scholar
Brewer, E. & Cunningham, K. (2009). Capturing study abroad's transformative potential. In Brewer, E. & Cunningham, K. (eds.), Integrating study abroad into the curriculum. Sterling, VA: Stylus Publishing, 117.Google Scholar
Bridges, S. (2007). Learner perceptions of a professional development immersion course. Prospect 22.2, 3960.Google Scholar
Brierley, B. & Coleman, J. (1997). Supporting language students during their year abroad. Linguist 36.1, 24.Google Scholar
Briggs, J. G. (2016). A mixed-methods study of vocabulary-related strategic behaviour in informal L2 contact. Study Abroad Research in Second Language Acquisition and International Education 1.1, 6187.Google Scholar
Brockington, J. L. & Wiedenhoeft, M. D. (2009). The liberal arts and global citizenship: Fostering intercultural engagement through integrative experiences and structured reflection. In Lewin, R. (ed.), 117–132.Google Scholar
Brown, L. (2013). Identity and honorifics use in Korean study abroad. In Kinginger, C. (ed.), 268–298.Google Scholar
Brubaker, C. (2007). Six weeks in the Eifel: A case for culture learning during short-term study abroad. Unterrichtspraxis 40.2, 118123.Google Scholar
Bryant, K. M. & Soria, K. M. (2015). College students’ sexual orientation, gender identity, and participation in study abroad. Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad 25, 95106.Google Scholar
Byram, M. (1997). Teaching and assessing intercultural communicative competence.Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Byram, M., Nichols, A. & Stevens, D. (eds.) (2001). Developing intercultural competence in practice. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Canale, M. (1983). From communicative competence to communicative language pedagogy. In Richards, J. C. & Schmidt, R. W. (eds.), Language and communication. London: Longman, 227.Google Scholar
Chen, G. M. & Starosta, W. J. (1997). A review of the concept of intercultural sensitivity. Human Communication 1.1, 116.Google Scholar
Churchill, E. (2006). Variability in the study abroad classroom and learner competence. In DuFon, M. A. & Churchill, E. (eds.), 203–227.Google Scholar
Clyne, F. & Rizvi, F. (1998). Outcomes of student exchange. In Davis, D. & Olsen, A. (eds.), Outcomes of international education. Sydney: IDP Education Australia, 3549.Google Scholar
Cohen, A. D. & Shively, R. L. (2007). Acquisition of requests and apologies in Spanish and French: Impact of study abroad and strategy-building intervention. Modern Language Journal 91.2, 189212.Google Scholar
Cohen, A. D., Paige, R. M., Shively, R. L., Emert, H. & Hoff, J. (2005). Maximizing study abroad through language and culture strategies: Research on students, study abroad program professionals, and language instructors. Minneapolis: CARLA.Google Scholar
Coleman, J. A. (1997). Residence abroad within language study. Language Teaching 30, 120.Google Scholar
Coleman, J. A. (2013a). Research whole people and whole lives. In Kinginger, C. (ed.), 17–44.Google Scholar
Coleman, J. A. (2013b). Dymplexity: New theories, new contexts and new labels for mobile students. Journal of Multicultural Discourses 8, 2028.Google Scholar
Coleman, J. A. (2015). Social circles during residence abroad: What students do, and who with. In Mitchell, R., Tracy-Ventura, N. & McManus, K. (eds.), 33–54.Google Scholar
Collins, F. L., Sidhu, R., Lewis, N. & Yeoh, B. S. A. (2014). Mobility and desire: International students and Asian regionalism in aspirational Singapore. Discourse: Studies in the Cultural Politics of Education 35.5, 661676.Google Scholar
Cook, H. M. (2006). Joint construction of folk beliefs by JFL learners and Japanese host families. In DuFon, M. & Churchill, E. (eds.), 120–150.Google Scholar
Creswell, J. W. (2013). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods approaches (4th edn.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Creswell, J. W. & Garrett, A. L. (2008). The ‘movement’ of mixed methods research and the role of educators. South African Journal of Education 28.3, 321333.Google Scholar
Cubillos, J. H., Chieffo, L. & Fan, C. (2008). The impact of short-term study abroad programs on L2 listening comprehension skills. Foreign Language Annals 41.1, 157186.Google Scholar
Cubillos, J. H. & Ilvento, T. (2012). The impact of study abroad on students’ self-efficacy perceptions. Foreign Language Annals 45.4, 494511.Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (2010). Study abroad: When, how long, and with what results? New data from the Russian front. Foreign Language Annals 43.1, 626.Google Scholar
de Bot, K., Lowie, W. & Verspoor, M. (2007). A dynamic systems theory approach to second language acquisition. Bilingualism Language and Cognition 10.1, 721.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2010). Monitoring processes in Spanish as a second language during a study abroad program. Foreign Language Annals 43.1, 8092.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2014). Research on language development during study abroad: Methodological consideration and future perspectives. In Pérez-Vidal, C. (ed.), 313–326.Google Scholar
de Ridder-Symoens, H. (1992). A history of the university in Europe vol. 1: Universities in the Middle Ages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dewaele, J. M., Comanaru, R. S. & Faraco, M. (2015). The affective benefits of a pre-sessional course at the start of study abroad. In Mitchell, R., Tracy-Ventura, N. & McManus, K. (eds.), 95–114.Google Scholar
Dewey, D. P. (2004). A comparison of reading development by learners of Japanese in intensive domestic immersion and study abroad contexts. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 26.2, 303327.Google Scholar
Dewey, D. P., Bown, J., Baker, W., Martinsen, R. A., Gold, C. & Eggett, D. (2014). Language use in six study abroad programs: An exploratory analysis of possible predictors. Language Learning 64.1, 3671.Google Scholar
Dewey, D., Bown, J. & Eggett, D. (2012). Japanese language proficiency, social networking, and language use during study abroad: Learners’ perspectives. Canadian Modern Language Journal 68.2, 111137.Google Scholar
Dewey, D., Ring, S., Gardner, D. & Belnap, R. K. (2013). Social network formation and development during study abroad in the Middle East. System 41.2, 269282.Google Scholar
de Wit, H. (2009). Global citizenship and study abroad: A European comparative perspective. In Lewin, R. (ed.), 212–229.Google Scholar
Diao, W. (2014). Peer socialization into gendered L2 Mandarin practices in a study abroad context: Talk in the dorm. Applied Linguistics 35.1, 123.Google Scholar
Diao, W. (2017). Between the standard and non-standard: Accent and identity among transnational Mandarin speakers studying abroad in China. System https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2017.09.013.Google Scholar
Di Silvio, F., Donovan, A. & Malone, M. E. (2014). The effect of study abroad homestay placements: Participant perspectives and oral proficiency gains. Foreign Language Annals 47.1, 168188.Google Scholar
Di Silvio, F., Donovan, A. & Malone, M. E. (2015). Promoting oral proficiency gains in study abroad homestay placements. In Mitchell, R., Tracy-Ventura, N. & McManus, K. (eds.), 75–94.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2009). The L2 motivational self system. In Dörnyei, Z. & Ushioda, E. (eds.), Motivation, language identity and the L2 self. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, 942.Google Scholar
Du, H. (2013). The development of Chinese fluency during study abroad in China. Modern Language Journal 97.1, 131143.Google Scholar
DuFon, M. A. (2006). The socialization of taste during study abroad in Indonesia. In DuFon, M. A. & Churchill, E. (eds.), 91–119.Google Scholar
Duperron, L. (2006). Study abroad and the second language acquisition of tense and aspect in French: Is longer better? In Wilkinson, S. (ed.), Insights from study abroad for language programs. Boston: Thomson Heinle, 4571.Google Scholar
Dyson, P. (1988). The effect on linguistic competence of the year spent abroad by students studying French, German, and Spanish at the degree level. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
East, M. (2016). Assessing spoken proficiency: What are the issues? Educational Linguistics 26, 2550.Google Scholar
Engle, J. & Engle, L. (1999). Program intervention in the process of cultural integration: The example of French practicum. Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad 5.2, 3959.Google Scholar
Engle, J. & Engle, L. (2002). Neither international nor educative: Study abroad in the time of globalization. In Grünzweig, W. & Rinehart, N. (eds.), Rockin’ in Red Square: Critical approaches to international education in the time of cyberculture. Münster: Lit Verlag, 2540.Google Scholar
Engle, J. & Engle, L. (2012). Beyond immersion. The American University Center of Provence experiment in holistic intervention. In Vande Berg, M., Paige, R. M. & Lou, K. H. (eds.), 284–307.Google Scholar
Félix-Brasdefer, J. S. & Hasler-Barker, M. (2015). Complimenting in Spanish in a short-term study abroad context. System 48, 7585.Google Scholar
Fernández-García, M. & Martínez-Arbelaiz, A. (2014). Native speaker-non-native speaker study abroad conversations: Do they provide feedback and opportunities for pushed output? System 42, 93104.Google Scholar
Foster, P. & Wigglesworth, G. (2007). Capturing accuracy in second language performance: Exploring the reliability of global and local measures. In van Daele, S. et al. (eds.), Complexity, accuracy and fluency in second language use, learning and teaching. Brussels: Contactforum, 67.Google Scholar
Fraser, C. C. (2002). Study abroad: An attempt to measure the gains. German as a Foreign Language Journal 1, 4565.Google Scholar
Freed, B. (ed.) (1995). Second language acquisition in a study abroad context. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Freed, B., Segalowitz, N. & Dewey, D. (2004). Contexts of learning and second language fluency in French: Comparing regular classrooms, study abroad, and intensive domestic programs. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 26.2, 275301.Google Scholar
Freed, B., So, S. & Lazar, N. A. (2003). Language learning abroad: How do gains in written fluency compare with gains in oral fluency in French as a second language? ADFL Bulletin 34.3, 3440.Google Scholar
Gao, X. (2010). Strategic language learning: The roles of agency and context (vol. 49).Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Geeslin, K. L., García-Amaya, L., Hasler-Barke, M., Henriksen, N. & Killam, J. (2010). The SLA of direct object pronouns in a study abroad immersion environment where use is variable. In Borgonovo, C., Español-Echevarría, M. & Prévost, P. (eds.), Selected proceedings of the 12th Hispanic linguistics symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, 246259.Google Scholar
George, A. (2014). Study abroad in Central Spain: The development of regional phonological features. Foreign Language Annals 47.1, 97114.Google Scholar
Ginsberg, R. B., Robin, R. M. & Wheeling, P. R. (1992). Listening comprehension before and after study abroad. Washington, DC: National Foreign Language Center.Google Scholar
Godwin-Jones, R. (2016). Integrating technology into study abroad. Language Learning & Technology 20.1, 120.Google Scholar
Golato, A. (2003). Studying compliment responses: A comparison of DCTs and recordings of naturally occurring talk. Applied linguistics 24.1, 90121.Google Scholar
Golato, A. & Golato, P. (2013). Speech acts. In Chapelle, A. C. (ed.), vol. 9. http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781405198431.wbeal1100/full.Google Scholar
Goldberg, D., Looney, D. & Lusin, N. (2015). Enrollments in languages other than English in United States institutions of higher education, Fall 2013. MLA web published. https://apps.mla.org/pdf/2013_enrollment_survey.pdf.Google Scholar
Goldoni, F. (2007). The ethnography of study abroad: What is study abroad as a cultural event? EMIGRA Working Papers 8, 118.Google Scholar
Gore, J. E. (2005). Dominant beliefs and alternative voices: Discourse, belief, and gender in American study abroad. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gorka, B. & Niesenbaum, R. (2001). Beyond the language requirement: Interdisciplinary short-term study-abroad programs in Spanish. Hispania 84.1, 100109.Google Scholar
Green, J. C., Caracelli, V. J. & Graham, W. F. (1989). Toward a conceptual framework for mixed-method evaluation designs. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis 11.3, 255274.Google Scholar
Grey, S., Cox, J. G., Serafini, E. J. & Sanz, C. (2015). The role of individual differences in the study abroad context: Cognitive capacity and language development during short-term intensive language exposure. Modern Language Journal 99.1, 137157.Google Scholar
Haberland, H. & Mortensen, J. (2012). Language variety, language hierarchy and language choice in the international university. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 216, 16.Google Scholar
Hamel, R. E. (2013). The field of science and higher education between the monopoly of English and plurilingualism: Elements for a language policy in Latin America. Trabalhos em lingüística aplicada 52.2, 321384.Google Scholar
Hampton, C. (2015). Meeting in the virtual middle: Blending online human resources to generate a year abroad community. In Mitchell, R., Tracy-Ventura, N. & McManus, K. (eds.), 223–240.Google Scholar
Hampton, C. (2016). Cultural discovery as a post-year abroad agent of change for UK modern language students. Comparative and International Education / Éducation Comparée et Internationale 45.2, Article 3. http://ir.lib.uwo.ca/cie-eci/vol45/iss2/3.Google Scholar
Han, B. (2008). Speak the language, understand the culture, cross the barrier: Combining internship experience with language learning in China. In Lee, C. K. et al. (eds.), Chinese as foreign/second language in the study abroad context. Beijing: Peking University Press, 187202.Google Scholar
Hardison, D. M. (2014). Changes in second-language learners’ oral skills and socio-affective profiles following study abroad: A mixed methods approach. The Canadian Modern Language Review 70.4, 416444.Google Scholar
Hassall, T. (2013). Pragmatic development during short-term study abroad: The case of address terms in Indonesian. Journal of Pragmatics 55, 117.Google Scholar
Henning, G. (1986). Quantitative methods in language acquisition research. TESOL Quarterly 20.4, 701708.Google Scholar
Hernández, T. A. (2010). The relationship among motivation, interaction, and the development of second language oral proficiency in a study‐abroad context. Modern Language Journal 94.4, 600617.Google Scholar
Housen, A. & Kuiken, F. (2009). Complexity, accuracy, and fluency in second language acquisition. Applied Linguistics 30.4, 113.Google Scholar
Housen, A., Kuiken, F. & Vedder, I. (2012). Complexity, accuracy and fluency: Definitions, measurement and research. In Housen, A., Kuiken, F. & Vedder, I. (eds.), Dimensions of L2 performance and proficiency: Complexity, accuracy and fluency in SLA. Philadelphia: John Benjamins, 120.Google Scholar
Huebner, T. (1995). The effects of overseas language programs: Report on a case study of an intensive Japanese course. In Freed, B. (ed.), 171–193.Google Scholar
Hymes, D. H. (1972). On communicative competence. In Pride, J. B. & Holmes, J. (eds.), Sociolinguistics. Harmondsworth: Penguin, 269293.Google Scholar
IAU (International Association of Universities) (2014). Internationalization of higher education: Growing expectations, fundamental values. Egron-Polak, E. & Hudson, R. (eds.), IAU 4th Global Survey. www.iau-aiu.net/content/iau-global-surveys.Google Scholar
ICEF (International Consultants for Education and Fairs) Monitor (2017). OECD charts a slowing of international mobility growth. http://monitor.icef.com/2017/09/oecd-charts-slowing-international-mobility-growth/.Google Scholar
Iino, M. (2006). Norms of interaction in a Japanese homestay setting: Toward two-way flow of linguistic and cultural resources. In DuFon, M. & Churchill, E. (eds.), 151–173.Google Scholar
Ikeguchi, C. B. (1996). Self assessment and ESL competence of Japanese returnees. Tsukuba Women's University. Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan. http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED399798.pdf.Google Scholar
Ingram, M. (2005). Recasting the foreign language requirement through study abroad: A cultural immersion program in Avignon, France. Foreign Language Annals 38.2, 211222.Google Scholar
International Trends in Higher Education (2015). University of Oxford. www.ox.ac.uk/sites/files/oxford/International%20Trends%20in%20Higher%20Education%202015.pdf.Google Scholar
Isabelli-García, C. (2006). Study abroad social networks, motivation, and attitudes: Implications for SLA. In DuFon, M. & Churchill, E. (eds.), 231–258.Google Scholar
Isabelli-García, C. (2010). Acquisition of Spanish gender agreement in two learning contexts: Study Abroad and at home. Foreign Language Annals 43.2, 289303.Google Scholar
Iwasaki, N. (2010). Style shifts among Japanese learners before and after study abroad in Japan: Becoming active social agents in Japanese. Applied Linguistics 31, 4571.Google Scholar
Jackson, J. (2006a). Ethnographic pedagogy and evaluation in short-term study abroad. In Byram, M. & Feng, A. (eds.), Living and studying abroad: Research and practice (vol. 12). Bristol: Multilingual Matters, 132185.Google Scholar
Jackson, J. (2006b). Ethnographic preparation for short-term study and residence in the target culture. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 30, 7798.Google Scholar
Jackson, J. (2008). Language, identity and study abroad: Sociocultural perspectives. London: Equinox.Google Scholar
Jackson, J. (2009). Intercultural learning on short-term sojourns. Intercultural Education 20, 5971.Google Scholar
Jackson, J. (2010). Intercultural journeys: From study abroad to residence abroad. London: Palgrave MacMillan.Google Scholar
Jackson, J. (2013). The transformation of ‘a frog in the well’: A path to a more intercultural, global mindset. In Kinginger, C. (ed.), 179–204.Google Scholar
Jackson, J. (2016). The language use, attitude, and motivation of Chinese students prior to a semester-long sojourn in an English-speaking environment. Study Abroad Research in Second Language Acquisition and International Education 1.1, 433.Google Scholar
Jensen, J. & Howard, M. (2014). The effects of time in the development of complexity and accuracy during study abroad. In Roberts, L., Vedder, I. & Hulstijn, J. H. (eds.), EUROSLA Yearbook 14. Philadelphia: John Benjamins, 3164.Google Scholar
Jochum, C. J. (2014). Measuring the effects of a semester abroad on students’ oral proficiency gains: A comparison of at-home and study abroad. The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad 24, 93104.Google Scholar
Jon, J. E., Lee, J. J. & Byun, K. (2014). The emergence of a regional hub: Comparing international student choices and experiences in South Korea. Higher Education 67.5, 691710.Google Scholar
Juan-Garau, M. (2014). Oral accuracy growth after formal instruction and study abroad. In Pérez-Vidal, C. (ed.), 87–110.Google Scholar
Juan-Garau, M., Salazar-Noguera, J. & Prieto-Arranz, J. I. (2014). English L2 learners’ lexico-grammatical and motivational development at home and abroad. In Pérez-Vidal, C. (ed.), 259– 283.Google Scholar
Kanwit, M., Geeslin, K. L. & Fafulas, S. (2015). Study abroad and the SLA of variable structures: A look at the present perfect, the copula contrast, and the present progressive in Mexico and Spain. Probus: International Journal of Latin and Romance Linguistics 27.2, 307348.Google Scholar
Kaplan, M. A. (1989). French in the community: A survey of language use abroad. The French Review 63.2, 290299.Google Scholar
Kaplan, R. B. & Baldauf, R. B. (2003). Language and language-in-education planning in the Pacific Basin. Boston: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Kaypak, E. & Ortaçtepe, D. (2014). Language learner beliefs and study abroad: A study on English as a lingua franca (ELF). System 42, 355367.Google Scholar
Kemp, J. (2010). The listening log: Motivating autonomous learning. ELT Journal 64.4, 385395.Google Scholar
Kiely, R. & Nielson, D. (2003). International service learning: The importance of partnerships. Community College Journal 73, 3941.Google Scholar
Kim, E. J. (2006). Heritage language maintenance by Korean-American college students. In Kondo-Brown, K. (ed.), Heritage language development: Focus on East Asian immigrants. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins, 175208.Google Scholar
King, R. & Sondhi, G. (2016). Gendering international student migration: A comparison of UK and Indian students’ motivations and experiences of studying abroad. Working Paper No. 84. University of Sussex. www.academia.edu/download/42370314/mwp84.pdf.Google Scholar
King, R. & Raghuram, P. (2013). International student migration: Mapping the field and new research agendas. Population, Space and Place 19.2, 127137.Google Scholar
Kinginger, C. (2004). Alice doesn't live here anymore: Foreign language learning and identity reconstruction. Negotiation of Identities in Multilingual Contexts 21.2, 219242.Google Scholar
Kinginger, C. (2008). Language learning in study abroad: Case studies of Americans in France. Modern Language Journal 92, 1124.Google Scholar
Kinginger, C. (2009). Language learning and study abroad: A critical reading of research. Basingstoke: Palgrave MacMillan.Google Scholar
Kinginger, C. (2010). American students abroad: Negotiation of difference? Language Teaching 43.2, 216227.Google Scholar
Kinginger, C. (2011). National identity and language learning abroad: American students in the post-9/11 era. In Higgins, C. (ed.), Identity formation in globalizing contexts: Language learning in the new millennium. Boston: Walter de Gruyter, 147166.Google Scholar
Kinginger, C. (2013a). Identity and language learning in study abroad. Foreign Language Annals 46.3, 339358.Google Scholar
Kinginger, C. (ed.) (2013b). Social and cultural aspects of language learning in study abroad (vol. 37). Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kinginger, C. (2015). Language socialization in the homestay: American high school students in China. In Mitchell, R., Tracy-Ventura, N. & McManus, K. (eds.), 53–74.Google Scholar
Kinginger, C., Lee, S. H., Wu, Q. & Tan, D. (2016). Contextualized language practices as sites for learning: Mealtime talk in short-term Chinese homestays. Applied Linguistics 37.5, 716740.Google Scholar
Klapper, J. & Rees, J. (2012). University residence abroad for foreign language students: Analysing the linguistic benefits. Language Learning Journal 40.3, 335358.Google Scholar
Kline, R. R. (1998). Literacy and language learning in a study abroad context. Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad 4.2, 139165.Google Scholar
Knight, J. (2008). The internationalization of higher education: Complexities and realities. In Teferra, D. & Knight, J. (eds.), Higher education in Africa: The international dimension. Oxford: African Books Collective, 143.Google Scholar
Knight, S. M. & Schmidt-Rinehart, B. C. (2002). Enhancing the home stay: Study abroad from the host family's perspective. Foreign Language Annals 35.2, 190201.Google Scholar
Knight, J. & Morshidi, S. (2011). The complexities and challenges of regional education hubs: Focus on Malaysia. Higher Education 62.5, 593606.Google Scholar
Knouse, S. M. (2012). The acquisition of dialectal phonemes in the study abroad context: The case of the Castilian theta. Foreign Language Annals 45.4, 512542.Google Scholar
Kronholz, J. F. & Osborn, D. S. (2016). The impact of study abroad experiences on vocational identity among college students. Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad 26, 7084.Google Scholar
Kurata, N. (2011). Foreign language learning and use: Interaction in informal social networks. New York: Continuum.Google Scholar
Lafford, B. & Isabelli, C. (in press). Programas de estudios en el extranjero. In J. Muñoz-Basols, Gironzetti, E. & Lacorte, M. (eds.), The Routledge handbook of Spanish language teaching: Metodologías, contextos y recursos para la enseñanza del español L2. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
LANGSNAP project (2013). Social networks, target language interaction and second language acquisition during the year abroad: A longitudinal study. University of Southampton. http://langsnap.soton.ac.uk/project.html.Google Scholar
Lantolf, J. P. (2014). Integrational linguistics and L2 proficiency. In Spolsky, B., Inbar-Lourie, O. & Tannebaum, M. (eds.), Challenges for language education and policy: Making space for people. New York: Routledge, 309322.Google Scholar
Lantolf, J. P. & Pavlenko, A. (2001). Second language activity theory: Understanding second language learners as people. In Breen, M. P. (ed.), Learner contributions to language learning: New directions in research. New York: Pearson, 141158.Google Scholar
Lapkin, S., Hart, D. & Swain, M. (1995). A Canadian interprovincial exchange: Evaluating the linguistic impact of a three-month stay in Quebec. In Freed, B. (ed.), 67–94.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2006). The emergence of complexity, fluency, and accuracy in the oral and written production of five Chinese learners of English. Applied Linguistics 27, 590619.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2009). Adjusting expectations: The study of complexity, accuracy, and fluency in second language acquisition. Applied Linguistics 30.4, 579589.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D. & Cameron, L. (2008). Complex systems and applied linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D. & Long, M. (2014). An introduction to second language acquisition research. New York: Longman.Google Scholar
Lave, J. & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, L. (2011). Blogging: Promoting learner autonomy and intercultural competence through study abroad. Language Learning & Technology 15.3, 87109.Google Scholar
Lee, L. (2012). Engaging study abroad students in intercultural learning through blogging and ethnographic interviews. Foreign Language Annals 45.1, 721.Google Scholar
Lee, S. H., Wu, Q., Di, C. & Kinginger, C. (2017). Learning to eat politely at the Chinese homestay dinner table: Two contrasting case studies. Foreign Language Annals 50.1, 135158.Google Scholar
Leonard, K. R. & Shea, C. E. (2017). L2 Speaking development during study abroad: Fluency, accuracy, complexity, and underlying cognitive factors. Modern Language Journal 101.1, 179193.Google Scholar
Lewin, R. (2009). Introduction: The quest for global citizenship through study abroad, xiii–xxii. In Lewin, R. (ed.), The handbook of practice and research in study abroad: Higher education and the question for global citizenship. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lewis, T. L. & Niesenbaum, R. A. (2005). Extending the stay: Using community-based research and service learning to enhance short-term study abroad. Journal of Studies in International Education 9, 251264.Google Scholar
Li, S. (2014). The effects of different levels of linguistic proficiency on the development of L2 Chinese request production during study abroad. System 45, 103116.Google Scholar
Lillis, T. & Curry, M. J. (2010). Academic writing in a global context: The politics and practices of publishing in English. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Llanes, À. (2011). The many faces of study abroad: An update on the research on L2 gains emerged during a study abroad experience. International Journal of Multilingualism 8.3, 189215.Google Scholar
Llanes, À. (2012). The impact of study abroad and age on second language accuracy development. In Muñoz, C. (ed.), 193–210.Google Scholar
Llanes, À. & Muñoz, C. (2009). A short stay abroad: Does it make a difference? System 37.3, 353365.Google Scholar
Llanes, À. & Muñoz, C. (2013). Age effects in a study abroad context: Children and adults studying abroad and at home. Language Learning 63.1, 6390.Google Scholar
Lord, G. (2010). The combined effects of immersion and instruction on second language pronunciation. Foreign Language Annals 43.3, 488503.Google Scholar
Lord, G. & Isabelli-García, C. (2014). Program articulation and management. In Lacorte, M. (ed.), The Routledge handbook of Hispanic applied linguistics. New York: Routledge, 150167.Google Scholar
Lou, K. H. & Bosley, G. B. (2012). Facilitating intercultural learning abroad. The intentional, targeted intervention model. In Vande Berg, M., Paige, R. M. & Lou, K. H. (eds.), 335–359.Google Scholar
Mackey, A. & Gass, S. (2005). Second language research: Methodology and design. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Magnan, S. S. & Back, M. (2007). Social interaction and linguistic gain during study abroad. Foreign Language Annals 40.1, 4361.Google Scholar
Marques-Pascual, L. (2011). Study abroad, previous language experience and Spanish L2 development. Foreign Language Annals 44, 565582.Google Scholar
Martínez-Arbelaiz, A. & Pereira Rodríguez, I. (2008). La adquisición del pretérito imperfecto en situación de inmersión. Spanish in Context 5.2, 161181.Google Scholar
Martinsen, R. A. (2010). Short-term study abroad: Predicting changes in oral skills. Foreign Language Annals 43.3, 504530.Google Scholar
Martinsen, R. A., Alvord, S. M. & Tanner, J. (2014). Perceived foreign accent: Extended stays abroad, level of instruction, and motivation. Foreign Language Annals 47.1, 6678.Google Scholar
Martinsen, R. A., Baker, W., Dewey, D. P., Bown, J. & Johnson, C. (2010). Exploring diverse settings for language acquisition and use: Comparing study abroad, service learning abroad, and foreign language housing. Applied Language Learning 20.1/2, 4569.Google Scholar
Mason, L., Powers, C. & Donnelly, S. (2015). The Boren awards: A report of oral language proficiency gains during academic study abroad. New York: Institute of International Education. www.iie.org/Research-and-Publications/Publications-and-Reports/IIE-Bookstore/The-Boren-Awards-A-Report-Of-Oral-Language-Proficiency-Gains#.V-LpU5MrJGM.Google Scholar
McManus, K., Mitchell, R. & Tracy-Ventura, N. (2014). Understanding insertion and integration in a study abroad context: The case of English-speaking sojourners in France. Revue Française de Linguistique Appliquée 19.2, 97116.Google Scholar
McMeekin, A. (2017). L2 learners of Japanese: Socialization of private assessments in a host family setting. Canadian Journal of Applied Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique appliquée 19.2, 107127.Google Scholar
Meara, P. (1994). The year abroad and its effects. Language Learning Journal 10.1, 3238.Google Scholar
Menard-Warwick, J. & Palmer, D. (2012). Bilingual development in study-abroad journal narratives: Three case studies from a short-term program in Mexico. Multilingua 31.4, 381412.Google Scholar
Mercer, C. (2015). Finding freedom abroad: Working with conservative Christian students in study abroad programs. Teaching Theology & Religion 18.1, 8187.Google Scholar
Miano, A. A., Bernhardt, E. B. & Brates, V. (2016). Exploring the effects of a short-term Spanish immersion program in a postsecondary setting. Foreign Language Annals 49.2, 287301.Google Scholar
Misfeldt, K. F. (2013). Pedagogies of affect and lived place: Reading Der Vorleser on a short-term intensive immersion. In Plews, J. L. & Schmenk, B. (eds.), Traditions and transitions: Curricula for German studies. Waterloo, ON: Wilfrid Laurier University Press, 191208.Google Scholar
Mitchell, R. (2015). The development of social relations during residence abroad. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 9.1, 2233.Google Scholar
Mitchell, R., Tracy-Ventura, N. & McManus, K. (eds.) (2015). Social interaction, identity and language learning during residence abroad. EUROSLA Monographs Series 4. Amsterdam: European Second Language Association.Google Scholar
Mora, J. C. (2014). The role of onset level on L2 perceptual phonological development after formal instruction and study abroad. In Pérez-Vidal, C. (ed.), 167–194.Google Scholar
Mortensen, J. & Haberland, H. (2012). English—the new Latin of academia? Danish universities as a case. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 216, 175197.Google Scholar
Mu, J. Z. (2008). Enforcing the language pledge: A mission impossible? In Lee, C. K. et al. (eds.), Chinese as foreign/second language in the study abroad context. Beijing: Peking University Press, 918.Google Scholar
Müller, M. (2016). Listening to learners’ voices: Qualitative aspects of pronunciation learning during study abroad. Journal of Second Language Pronunciation 2.1, 108142.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C. (ed.), Intensive exposure experiences in second language learning. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Nguyen, T. T. M. & Basturkmen, H. (2013). Requesting in Vietnamese as a second language. In Roever, C. & Nguyen, H. T. (eds.), Pragmatics of Vietnamese as a native and target language. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawai‘i, National Foreign Language Resource Center, 1375.Google Scholar
Nishida, C. & Isabelli, C. (2005). Development of the Spanish subjunctive in a nine-month study-abroad setting. In Eddington, D. (ed.), Selected proceedings of the 6th conference on the acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as first and second languages. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, 7891.Google Scholar
Nkwi, P. N., Nyamongo, I. K. & Ryan, G. W. (2001). Field research into socio-cultural issues: Methodological guidelines. Yaounde, Cameroon, Africa: International Center for Applied Social Sciences, Research, and Training.Google Scholar
Norris, J. M. & Ortega, L. (2009). Toward an organic approach to investigating CAF in instructed SLA: The case of complexity. Applied Linguistics 30.4, 555578.Google Scholar
O'Brien, M. G. (2003). Longitudinal development of second language German vowels. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin, Madison.Google Scholar
Ochs, E. (2002). Becoming a speaker of culture. In Kramsch, C. J. (ed.), Language acquisition and language socialization: Ecological perspectives. London: Continuum, 99120.Google Scholar
Olds, K. (2007). Global assemblage: Singapore, foreign universities, and the construction of a ‘global education hub.’ World Development 35, 959975.Google Scholar
OECD (Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development) (2009). Education at a glance 2009: OECD indicators. OECD www.oecd.org/education/skills-beyond-school/43636332.pdf.Google Scholar
Paige, R. M., Cohen, A. D., Kappler, B., Chi, J. C. & Lassegard, J. P. (2002). Maximizing study abroad. A students’ guide to strategies for language and culture learning and use. Minneapolis, MN: Center for Advanced Research on Language Acquisition.Google Scholar
Paris, C. M., Nyaupane, G. P. & Teye, V. (2014). Expectations, outcomes and attitude change of study abroad students. Annals of Tourism Research 48, 275277.Google Scholar
Parker, B. & Dautoff, D. A. (2007). Service-learning and study abroad: Synergistic learning opportunities. Michigan Journal of Community Service Learning 13.2, 4053.Google Scholar
Pavlenko, A. & Lantolf, J. P. (2000). Second language learning as participation and the (re)construction of selves. In Lantolf, J. P. (ed.), Sociocultural theory and second language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 155178.Google Scholar
Pellegrino, V. A. (1998). Student perspectives on language learning in a study abroad context. Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad 4.2, 91120.Google Scholar
Pellegrino, V. A. (2005). Study abroad and second language use: Constructing the self. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pérez-Vidal, C. & Juan-Garau, M. (2009). The effects of study abroad on written performance. EUROSLA Yearbook, 9.1, 269295.Google Scholar
Pérez-Vidal, C., Juan-Garau, M., Mora, J. C. & Valls-Ferrer, M. (2012). Oral and written development in formal instruction and study abroad: Differential effects of learning context. In Muñoz, C. (ed.), 193–212.Google Scholar
Pérez-Vidal, C. & Barquin, E. (2014). Comparing progress in academic writing after formal instruction and study abroad. In Pérez-Vidal, C. (ed.), 217–234.Google Scholar
Plews, J. L. (2013). Taking study abroad to task: Issues and choices in syllabus design and enactment. 5th Biennial International Conference on Task Based Language Teaching, Banff, AB, Canada, Oct. 3–5.Google Scholar
Plews, J. L., Breckenridge, Y. & Cambre, M. C. (2010). Mexican English teachers’ experiences of international professional development in Canada: A narrative analysis. e-FLT 7.1, 520. http://eflt.nus.edu.sg/v7n12010/plews.pdf.Google Scholar
Plews, J. L., Breckenridge, Y., Cambre, M. C. & Fernandes, G. (2014). Mexican English teachers’ experiences of international professional development in Canada: A narrative sequel. e-FLT 11.1, 5275. http://e-flt.nus.edu.sg/v11n12014/plews.pdf.Google Scholar
Plews, J. L. & Misfeldt, K. (2016). Reviewing language learning journals in study abroad, or engaging students’ language awareness. 13th International Conference of the Association of Language Awareness, Vienna, Austria, July 19–22.Google Scholar
Poag, T. & Sperandio, J. (2015). Changing minds: The impact of study abroad components on students’ changes in their religious faith. Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad 26, 144166.Google Scholar
Polanyi, L. (1995). Language learning and living abroad: Stories from the field. In Freed, B. F. (ed.), 271–292.Google Scholar
Pope, J. (2016). The role of social networks in the acquisition of a dialectal feature during study abroad. In Sessarego, S. & Tejedo-Herrero, F. (eds.), Spanish language and sociolinguistic analysis. Philadephia: John Benjamins, 177196.Google Scholar
Prokhorov, A. & Therkelsen, J. (2015). Visualizing St. Petersburg: Using documentary production in a short-term study abroad program to enhance oral proficiency, media literacy, and research skills. Journal of Film and Video 67.3/4, 112124.Google Scholar
Ranta, L. & Meckelborg, A. (2013). How much exposure to English do international graduate students really get? Measuring language use in a naturalistic setting. Canadian Modern Language Review 69.1, 133.Google Scholar
Raschio, R. A. (2001). Integrative activities for the study-abroad setting. Hispania 94, 534541.Google Scholar
Rasouli Khorshidi, H. (2013). Study abroad and interlanguage pragmatic development in request and apology speech acts among Iranian learners. English Language Teaching 6.5, 6270.Google Scholar
Ren, W. (2013). The effect of study abroad on the pragmatic development of the internal modification of refusals. Pragmatics 23.4, 715741.Google Scholar
Reynolds-Case, A. (2013). The value of short-term study abroad: An increase in students’ pragmatic competence. Foreign Language Annals 46.2, 311322.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. (2002). Biographical research. Philadelphia, PA: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, C., Byram, M., Baro, A., Jordan, S. & Street, B. V. (2001). Language learners as ethnographers. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Rodríguez, K. (2010). Digital storytelling in study abroad: Toward a counter-catalogic experience. Seminar.net. International Journal of Media, Technology and Lifelong Learning 6.2, 219233.Google Scholar
Rollmann, M. (2007). Three German web courses with a study abroad component. In Lorey, C., Plews, J. L. & Rieger, C. L. (eds.), Interkulturelle Kompetenzen im Fremdsprachenunterricht. Intercultural literacies and German in the classroom. Festschrift für Manfred Prokop zum 65sten Geburtstag. Tübingen: Narr Verlag, 161181.Google Scholar
Romanelli, S., Menegotto, A. C. & Smyth, R. (2015). Stress perception: Effects of training and a study abroad program for L1 English late learners of Spanish. Journal of Second Language Pronunciation 1.2, 181210.Google Scholar
Roskvist, A., Harvey, S., Corder, D. & Stacey, K. (2013). ‘To improve language, you have to mix’: Perceptions of language learning in an overseas immersion environment. The Language Learning Journal 42.3, 321333.Google Scholar
Salgado Robles, F. (2014). Los efectos del aprendizaje-servicio en la adquisición de la variación regional por aprendices de español en un contexto de inmersión: El caso del leísmo vallisoletano. Revista Electrónica de Lingüística Aplicada 13.1, 233258.Google Scholar
Sanz, C. (2014). Contributions of study abroad research to our understanding of SLA processes and outcomes: The SALA project, an appraisal. In Pérez-Vidal, C. (ed.), 1–13.Google Scholar
Sasaki, M. (2004). A multiple-data analysis of the 3.5-year development of EFL student writers. Language Learning 54.3, 525582.Google Scholar
Sasaki, M. (2007). Effects of study-abroad experiences on EFL writers: A multiple-data analysis. Modern Language Journal 91.4, 602620.Google Scholar
Sasaki, M. (2009). Changes in English as a Foreign Language students’ writing over 3.5 years: A sociocognitive account. In Manchón, R. M. (ed.), Writing in foreign language contexts: Learning, teaching, and research. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, 4976.Google Scholar
Sasaki, M. (2011). Effects of varying lengths of study-abroad experiences on Japanese EFL students’ L2 writing ability and motivation: A longitudinal study. TESOL Quarterly: A Journal for Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages and of Standard English as a Second Dialect 45.1, 81105.Google Scholar
Schauer, G. (2009). Interlanguage pragmatic development: The study abroad context. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Schirm, R. S. (2015). The interactional proficiency of advanced learners of German. Paper presented at The Culture of Study Abroad for Second Languages conference, Halifax, Canada.Google Scholar
Schmidt-Rinehart, B. C. & Knight, S. M. (2004). The homestay component of study abroad: Three perspectives. Foreign Language Annals 37.2, 254262.Google Scholar
Schumann, F. & Schumann, J. (1977). Diary of a language learner: An introspective study of second language learning. In Brown, H., Crymes, R. & Yorio, C. (eds.), Teaching and learning: Trends in research and practice. Selected papers from the 1977 TESOL convention. Washington, DC: TESOL Georgetown University, 241249.Google Scholar
Schwieter, J. W. & Kunert, S. J. (2012). Short-term study abroad and cultural sessions: Issues of L2 development, identity, and socialization. In Chamness Miller, P., Watze, J. & Mantero, M. (eds.), Readings in language studies, vol. 3: Critical language studies focusing on identity. New York: International Society for Language Studies, Inc., 587604.Google Scholar
Serrano, S. (2010). Learning languages in study abroad and at home contexts: A critical review of comparative studies. Porta Linguarum 13, 149163.Google Scholar
Serrano, S., Llanes, Á. & Tragant, E. (2016). Examining L2 development in two short-term intensive programs for teenagers: Study abroad vs. ‘at home.’ System 57, 4354.Google Scholar
Serrano, S., Tragant, E. & Llanes, Á. (2012). A longitudinal analysis of the effects of one year abroad. Canadian Modern Language Review 68.2, 138163.Google Scholar
Shadowen, N. L., Chieffo, L. P. & Guerra, N. G. (2015). The Global Engagement Measurement Scale (GEMS): A new scale for assessing the impact of education abroad and campus internationalization. Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad 26, 231247.Google Scholar
Shiri, S. (2015). The homestay in intensive language study abroad: Social networks, language socialization, and developing intercultural competence. Foreign Language Annals 48.1, 525.Google Scholar
Shively, R. L. (2008). L2 acquisition of [β], [ð], and [ɣ] in Spanish: Impact of experience, linguistic environment, and learner variables. Southwest Journal of Linguistics 27.2, 79115.Google Scholar
Shively, R. L. (2011). L2 pragmatic development in study abroad: A longitudinal study of Spanish service encounters. Journal of Pragmatics 43, 18181835.Google Scholar
Shively, R. L. (2013). Out-of-class interaction during study abroad: Service encounters in Spain. Spanish in Context 10.1, 5391.Google Scholar
Shively, R. L. (2016). Heritage language learning in study abroad: Motivations, identity work, and language development. In Pascual, D. (ed.), Advances in Spanish as a heritage language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 259280.Google Scholar
Short, J. R., Boniche, A., Kim, Y. & Li, P. L. (2001). Cultural globalization, global English, and geography journals. The Professional Geographer 53.1, 111.Google Scholar
Siegal, M. (1996). The role of learner subjectivity in second language sociolinguistic competency: Western women learning Japanese. Applied Linguistics 17.3, 356382.Google Scholar
Söderlundh, H. (2012). Global policies and local norms: Sociolinguistic awareness and language choice at an international university. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 216, 87109.Google Scholar
Spada, N. (1986). The interaction between type of contact and type of instruction: Some effects on the L2 proficiency of adult learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 8.2, 181199.Google Scholar
Spoelman, M. & Verspoor, M. (2010). Dynamic patterns in development of accuracy and complexity: A longitudinal case study in the acquisition of Finnish. Applied Linguistics 31.4, 532553.Google Scholar
Stephenson, S. (1999). Two cultures under one roof: Study abroad as a transformational experience impact upon exchange students and host nationals in Santiago Chile. Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad 5, 138.Google Scholar
Stevens, J. J. (2011). Vowel duration in second language Spanish vowels: Study abroad versus at-home learners. Arizona Working Papers in SLA & Teaching 18, 77104.Google Scholar
Stokoe, E. (2013). The (in)authenticity of simulated talk: Comparing role-played and actual interaction and the implications for communication training. Research on Language and Social Interaction 46, 165185.Google Scholar
Streitwieser, B. T. (2009). Undergraduate research during study abroad. Scope, meaning, and potential. In Lewin, R. (ed.), 399–419.Google Scholar
Sullivan, K. R. & Ebrahim, G. J. (1995). Qualitative field research. In Ebrahim, G. J. & Sullivan, K. R. (eds.), Mother and child health: Research methods. London: Book Aid, 196211.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. (2008). Cognition, language contact, and the development of pragmatic comprehension in a study-abroad context. Language Learning 58.1, 3371.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. (2011). The effect of L2 proficiency and study-abroad experience on pragmatic comprehension. Language Learning 61.3, 904939.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. (2015). Developing interactional competence in a Japanese study abroad context. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. (2016). Learning contexts and pragmatics learning: Problems and opportunities of the study abroad research. Language Teaching 49, 114. www.cambridge.org/core/journals/language-teaching/article/contexts-and-pragmatics-learning-problems-and-opportunities-of-the-study-abroad-research/5E74C0FE21AB63ED6739EA8F4995C779.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N., Li, S. & Xiao, F. (2013). Production of formulaic expressions in L2 Chinese: A developmental investigation in a study abroad context. Chinese as a Second Language Research Journal 2.1, 2358.Google Scholar
Taillefer, G. F. (2005). Foreign language reading and study abroad: Cross-cultural and cross-linguistic questions. Modern Language Journal 89.4, 503528.Google Scholar
Talburt, S. & Stewart, M. A. (1999). What's the subject of study abroad? Race, gender, and living culture. Modern Language Journal 83.2, 163175.Google Scholar
Tanaka, K. & Ellis, R. (2003). Study abroad, language proficiency, and learner beliefs about language learning. JALT Journal 25.1, 6385.Google Scholar
Tang, S. Y. F. & Choi, P. L. (2004). The development of personal, intercultural, and professional competence in international field experience in initial teacher education. Asia Pacific Education Review 5.1, 5063.Google Scholar
Thompson, G. (2002). Teachers studying abroad: An analysis of changes in linguistic and cultural knowledge, attitudes toward the Spanish culture and the effects of ethnographic interviews. Texas Papers in Foreign Language Education 53–75.Google Scholar
Toohey, K. & Norton, B. (2003). Learner autonomy as agency in sociocultural settings. In Palfreyman, D. & Smith, R. (eds.), Learner autonomy across cultures. Basingstoke: Palgrave MacMillan, 5872.Google Scholar
Tracy-Ventura, N., Dewaele, J. M., Köylü, Z. & McManus, K. (2016). Personality changes after the ‘year abroad’? A mixed-methods study. Study Abroad Research in Second Language Acquisition and International Education 1.1, 107127.Google Scholar
Trenchs-Parera, M. (2009). Effects of formal instruction and stay abroad on the acquisition of native-like oral fluency. The Canadian Modern Language Review 62.3, 365393.Google Scholar
Trenchs-Parera, M. & Juan-Garau, M. (2014). A longitudinal study of learners’ motivation and beliefs in at home and study abroad contexts. In Pérez-Vidal, C. (ed.), 259–282.Google Scholar
Trentman, E. (2013). Arabic and English during study abroad in Cairo, Egypt: Issues of access and use. Modern Language Journal 97.2, 457473.Google Scholar
Trentman, E. (2015). Negotiating gendered identities and access to social networks during study abroad in Egypt. In Mitchell, R., Tracy-Ventura, N. & McManus, K. (eds.), 263–280.Google Scholar
Trentman, E. & Diao, W. (2015). The American gaze East: Cultural discourses of learning Mandarin and Arabic overseas. Presented at The Culture of Study Abroad for Second Languages conference, Halifax, Canada.Google Scholar
Tschirner, E. (2007). The development of oral proficiency in a four-week intensive immersion program in Germany. Unterrichtspraxis 40.2, 111117.Google Scholar
UNESCO Institute of Statistics (2014). Global flow of tertiary-level students. www.uis.unesco.org/Education/Pages/international-student-flow-viz.aspx.Google Scholar
Ushioda, E. (2007). Motivation, autonomy and sociocultural theory. In Benson, P. (ed.), Learner autonomy 8: Teacher and learner perspectives. Dublin: Authentik, 524.Google Scholar
Valls-Ferrer, M. & Mora, J. C. (2014). L2 fluency development in formal instruction and study abroad: the role of initial fluency level and language contact. In Pérez-Vidal, C. (ed.), 111–136.Google Scholar
Vande Berg, M. (2007). Intervening in the learning of U.S. students abroad. Journal of Studies in International Education 11.3/4, 392399.Google Scholar
Vande Berg, M., Connor-Linton, J. & Paige, M. (2009). The Georgetown consortium project: Interventions for student learning abroad. Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad 18, l–75.Google Scholar
Vande Berg, M., Paige, R. M. & Lou, K. H. (eds.) (2012). Student learning abroad: What our students are learning, what they're not, and what we can do about it. Sterling, VA: Stylus Publishing, xi–xvi.Google Scholar
van Lier, L. (1994). Action research. Sintagma: Revista De Lingüística 6, 3137.Google Scholar
van Lier, L. (2005). Case study. In Hinkel, E. (ed.), Handbook of research in second language teaching and learning. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 195208.Google Scholar
van Maele, J., Vassilicos, B. & Borghetti, C. (2016). Mobile students’ appraisals of keys to a successful study abroad experience: Hints from the IEREST project. Language and Intercultural Communication 16.3, 384481.Google Scholar
Verspoor, M., de Bot, K. & Lowie, W. (2011). A dynamic approach to second language development: Methods and techniques. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Vilar-Beltrán, E. (2014). Length of stay abroad: Effects of time on the speech act of requesting. International Journal of English Studies 14, 7996.Google Scholar
Wang, C. (2010). Toward a second language socialization perspective: Issues in study abroad research. Foreign Language Annals 43.1, 5063.Google Scholar
Wanner, D. (2009). Study abroad and language. From maximal to realistic models. In Lewin, R. (ed.), 81–98.Google Scholar
Warga, M. & Schölmberger, U. (2007). The acquisition of French apologetic behavior in a study abroad context. Intercultural Pragmatics 4.2, 221251.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, R. (2012). English-medium instruction at a Dutch university: Challenges and pitfalls. In Doiz, A., Lasagabaster, D. & Sierra, J. M. (eds.), English-medium instruction at university worldwide: Challenges and ways forward. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, 326.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, S. (1998a). On the nature of immersion during study abroad: Some participant perspectives. Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad 4.2, 121138.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, S. (1998b). Study abroad from the participants’ perspective: A challenge to common beliefs. Foreign Language Annals 31.1, 2339.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, S. (2000). Emerging questions about study abroad. ADFL Bulletin 32.1, 3641.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, S. (2001). Beyond classroom boundaries: The changing nature of study abroad. In Lavine, R. Z. (ed.), Beyond the boundaries: Changing contexts in language learning. Boston: McGraw-Hill, 81105.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, S. (2002). The omnipresent classroom during summer study abroad: American students in conversation with their French hosts. Modern Language Journal 86.2, 157173.Google Scholar
Wilson, J., Brain, R., Brown, E., Gaind, L., Radan, K. & Redmond, J. (2016). Interdisciplinary study abroad as experiential learning. Comparative and International Education / Éducation Comparée et Internationale 45.2, Article 4. http://ir.lib.uwo.ca/cie-eci/vol45/iss2/4.Google Scholar
Wolf, G. H. (2007). Studying abroad in Berlin and achieving cultural competence using English. Unterrichtspraxis 40.2, 143146.Google Scholar
Wood, D. (2007). Mastering the English formula: Fluency development of Japanese learners in a study abroad context. JALT Journal 29.2, 209230.Google Scholar
Xiao, L. & Wray, E. (2016). Theorizing one learner's perceived affective experiences and performances from a dynamic perspective. Journal of Psychology & the Behavioral Sciences 2.1, 6083.Google Scholar
Yager, K. (1998). Learning Spanish in Mexico: The effect of informal contact and student attitudes on language gain. Hispania 81.4, 898913.Google Scholar
Yang, J. S. (2016). The effectiveness of study-abroad on second language learning: A meta-analysis. Canadian Modern Language Review 72.1, 6694.Google Scholar
Zamberi Ahmad, S. & Buchanan, F. R. (2017). Motivation factors in students’ decision to study at international branch campuses in Malaysia. Studies in Higher Education 42.4, 651668. https://doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2015.1067604.Google Scholar
Zhou, Y., Jindal-Snape, D., Topping, K. & Todman, J. (2008). Theoretical models of cultural shock and adaptation in international students in higher education. Studies in Higher Education 33.1, 6375.Google Scholar