Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-pftt2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T13:15:50.532Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of childhood trauma in psychopathy and response inhibition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 January 2022

Stacey A. Bedwell*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, Kings College London, London, UK
Charlotte Hickman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Birmingham City University, Birmingham, UK
*
Corresponding author: Stacey A. Bedwell, email: Stacey.bedwell@kcl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Childhood trauma is linked to impairments in executive function and working memory, thought to underly psychological disorders including depression and posttraumatic stress disorder. Research demonstrates that childhood trauma can partially mediate posttraumatic stress disorder in those with executive function deficits. Despite a link with executive function deficit, psychopathy as a consequence of trauma is yet to be studied in this context. The present study investigates the possibility of a relationship between childhood trauma, psychopathic traits, and response inhibition. Eighty participants were tasked to completed the Childhood Traumatic Events Scale (Pennebaker & Susman, 2013), Levenson’s Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (Levenson et al., 1995), and Flanker task of response inhibition (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974). Scores of trauma exposure, psychopathic traits, and reaction times in the Flanker task were measured. Regression analysis revealed no significance for trauma exposure in predicting psychopathic traits (p = .201) and response inhibition (p = .183), indicating that childhood trauma does not strongly predict susceptibility to psychopathic traits or response inhibition deficits. These findings form an important basis on which to build a further understanding of the consequences of childhood trauma exposure, specifically in terms of understanding how specific cognitive functions may be influenced and providing a clearer understanding of how psychopathic traits develop.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Research acknowledges that exposure to trauma during childhood may lead to long-term neurological (De Bellis, Reference De Bellis2001), cognitive (De Bellis, Reference De Bellis2001; Twamley et al., Reference Twamley, Allard, Thorp, Norman, Cissell, Berardi and Stein2009), and psychological impairments (Op den Kelder et al., Reference Op den Kelder, Ensink, Overbeek, Maric and Lindauer2017). A commonly studied potential consequence of childhood trauma exposure is posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD); A wide range of studies note the increased risk of developing PTSD as a result of childhood trauma exposure (Alisic et al., Reference Alisic, Zalta, Van Wesel, Larsen, Hafstad, Hassanpour and Smid2014; Malarbi et al., Reference Malarbi, Abu-Rayya, Muscara and Stargatt2017; Op den Kelder et al., Reference Op den Kelder, Ensink, Overbeek, Maric and Lindauer2017). Research in this area is dominant in aspects of treatment, comorbidity, and prevalence rates. One study found around a third of children exposed to traumatic childhoods to develop PTSD in later life (Alisic et al., Reference Alisic, Jongmans, van Wesel and Kleber2011). Similarly, meta-analyses report 16% of all trauma-exposed children develop PTSD (Alisic et al., Reference Alisic, Zalta, Van Wesel, Larsen, Hafstad, Hassanpour and Smid2014). One of the most cited studies in this area reports young people exposed to complex trauma in childhood were more likely to display cognitive deficits, leading to higher levels of posttraumatic symptoms in later life (Op den Kelder et al., Reference Op den Kelder, Ensink, Overbeek, Maric and Lindauer2017).

Psychopathic traits

Though psychopathy itself is not a common psychological disorder; <1% in the general population (Coid et al., Reference Coid, Yang, Ullrich, Roberts and Hare2009; Tuvblad et al., Reference Tuvblad, Wang, Bezdjian, Raine and Baker2016), psychopathic traits are common (Coid et al., Reference Coid, Yang, Ullrich, Roberts and Hare2009). Psychopathic traits include a lack of emotional sensitivity and empathy, impulsiveness, superficial charm and insensitivity to punishing consequences (O’Donnell & Hetrick, Reference O’Donnell and Hetrick2016). Childhood adversity including physical neglect and family disruption are strongly associated with high levels of psychopathic traits (Marshall & Cooke, Reference Marshall and Cooke1999; Piquero et al., Reference Piquero, Farrington, Fontaine, Vincent, Coid and Ullrich2012). Similarly, childhood sexual abuse, emotional abuse, physical abuse, and neglect are associated with higher prevalence of psychopathic traits (Craparo et al., Reference Craparo, Schimmenti and Caretti2013; Graham et al., Reference Graham, Kimonis, Wasserman and Kline2012) emotional abuse and neglect. Existing research evidence suggests that exposure to trauma through various abuse in childhood may play a relevant role in the development of psychopathic traits. However, research to date has relied heavily on institutionalized samples, where psychopathy is over-represented at 7%–8% (Coid et al., Reference Coid, Yang, Ullrich, Roberts and Hare2009). This does not provide a clear understanding of the prevalence of this relationship in the general population.

Response inhibition

The evidence base shows that trauma has profound effects on cognitive functioning, particularly executive functions (Bucker et al., Reference Bucker, Kapczinski, Posy, Cereser, Szobot, Yatham, Kapczinski and Kauer-SantʼAnna2012), working memory (Castaneda et al., Reference Castaneda, Tuulio-Henriksson, Marttunen, Suvisaari and Lönnqvist2008), and emotional regulation (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Nishimi, Gomez, Powers and Bradle2018; Pechtel & Pizzagalli, Reference Pechtel and Pizzagalli2011; Zou et al., Reference Zou, Meng, Ma, Deng, Du, Wang, Chen and Hu2013). However, the sampling frame for a number of existing studies is a major limitation of the research. Where effects were found between executive functions and childhood trauma experience, the effects are often partly mediated by either depressive (Kaczmarczyk et al., Reference Kaczmarczyk, Wingenfeld, Kuehl, Otte and Hinkelmann2018), posttraumatic (Malarbi et al., Reference Malarbi, Abu-Rayya, Muscara and Stargatt2017; Op den Kelder et al., Reference Op den Kelder, Ensink, Overbeek, Maric and Lindauer2017), or schizophrenic (Li et al., Reference Li, Bo, Zhang, Zheng, Wang, Li and Wang2017) diagnoses. As a result, although there is evidence for a relationship, this may be more complex than that of a linear one and needs to be explored further.

Although there is evidence for a general role of trauma in executive function, the specific role of trauma in response inhibition remains to be fully understood. Response inhibition refers to a suppression of actions or behaviors that are inappropriate in a given context, or that interfere with goal direction (Mostofsky & Simmonds, Reference Mostofsky and Simmonds2008). Support for an association derives from multilevel meta-analyses utilizing 322 effect sizes and 55 studies in the area that found an overall small to medium effect size (Op den Kelder et al., Reference Op den Kelder, Ensink, Overbeek, Maric and Lindauer2017), and this was corroborated by Ji and Wang (Reference Ji and Wang2018), who also claimed adverse childhood experiences had a possibility rate of 56% in predicting inhibition ability. A recent study supported a strong association between childhood traumatic experience and response inhibition to aggressive behavior (Madole et al., Reference Madole, Johnson and Carver2019). These findings imply that trauma-exposed children have lower levels of executive functions that may play a role in later psychological functioning when considered in terms of aggression Further research is required to establish the presence of this role in response inhibition outside of aggressive behaviors.

A possible link between childhood trauma exposure, susceptibility to psychopathic traits, and response inhibition deficits is yet to be investigated thoroughly. Some research suggests an interaction between these variables. One body of research showed executive functioning deficits to mediate PTSD diagnoses in children exposed to trauma (Op den Kelder et al., Reference Op den Kelder, Ensink, Overbeek, Maric and Lindauer2017) and in schizophrenic diagnoses (Li et al., Reference Li, Bo, Zhang, Zheng, Wang, Li and Wang2017). Deficits in these functions have also been linked to trauma-exposed children with conduct disorder diagnoses, which is often regarded as a precursor to psychopathy (Fairchild et al., Reference Fairchild, van Goozen, Stollery, Aitken, Savage, Moore and Goodyer2009). A recent study by Madole et al. (Reference Madole, Johnson and Carver2019) found childhood traumas and poor response inhibition to account for 16% variance in aggression displayed in early adulthood. This could suggest that there is a role for mediation between variables in relation to childhood trauma exposure and response inhibition that should be investigated. Early childhood traumatic experiences may adversely impact an individual’s cognitive systems, particularly in relation to emotional and inhibition control, which in turn may pose a risk factor for the development of a range of psychopathological disorders.

Present study

Response inhibition as a type of executive functioning is yet to be investigated using nonclinical samples. In addition, the current replication crisis in psychology (Camerer et al., Reference Camerer, Dreber, Holzmeister, Ho, Huber, Johannesson and Altmejd2018) puts into perspective that the small number of significant effects and small effect sizes reported must be corroborated with further research to ensure their validity. Therefore, the present study aimed to establish if there are links between trauma experiences during childhood, response inhibition performance, and psychopathic traits. Specifically, it was hypothesized based on previous evidence from the literature that H1. Those who report higher levels of childhood trauma will score higher on measures of psychopathic traits and H2. Those who report higher levels of childhood trauma will score lower on measures of response inhibition.

Method

Design

The present study employed a within-subjects quasi-experimental design whereby all participants completed the Flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, Reference Eriksen and Eriksen1974), Levenson’s Self-Psychopathy Scale (Levenson et al., Reference Levenson, Kiehl and Fitzpatrick1995), and Childhood Traumatic Experience Scale (Pennebaker & Susman, Reference Pennebaker and Susman2013) on the Gorilla experimental platform. Childhood experience, specifically in relation to traumatic events as described in the CTES took the role of the independent variable in the study. Participants’ level of response inhibition was also measured.

Participants

Eighty participants from a psychology student population at a UK University were recruited through a volunteer sample. Participants were awarded with four research credits upon completion of the study. Those who had suffered a medically diagnosed traumatic brain injury, diagnosed with a traumatic psychological disorder, or were going through a traumatic experience at the time of research were discouraged from participation due to the sensitive nature of the study and possibility of confounding variables in relation to cognitive functioning (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Godfrey, Rosenfeld and Catroppa2012; Dikmen et al., Reference Dikmen, Corrigan, Levin, Machamer, Stiers and Weisskopf2009).

Materials

Childhood Traumatic Events Scale

Pennebaker and Susman’s (Reference Pennebaker and Susman2013) Childhood Traumatic Events Scale was completed to establish the frequency, type, and participants’ level of confiding of traumas experienced in childhood for the purpose of comparison and data analysis. The Childhood Traumatic Events Scale consists of six questions related to six types of childhood trauma: death of a very close friend or family, major upheaval between parents, sexual abuse, violence, illness or injury, and any other major upheaval significantly impacting life or personality. Examples of questions included “Prior to the age of 17, did you experience a death of a very close friend or family member? If yes, how old were you?” Participants’ responses were recorded using a seven-point Likert scale which ranged from 1 = not at all traumatic to 7 = extremely traumatic. The CTES has been validated by a range of studies in the field (Marcusson-Clavertz et al., Reference Marcusson-Clavertz, Gušić, Bengtsson, Jacobsen and Cardeña2017; Miu et al., Reference Miu, Bîlc, Bunea and Szentágotai-Tătar2017; Wamser-Nanney et al., Reference Wamser-Nanney, Howell, Schwartz and Hasselle2018).

Levenson’s Self-Report Psychopathy Scale

The Levenson’s Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (LSRP) measures perception of their personality in relation to psychopathic traits (Levenson et al., Reference Levenson, Kiehl and Fitzpatrick1995). The LSRP contained 16 items related to primary psychopathy, whilst the remaining 10 items assessed secondary psychopathy (Levenson et al., Reference Levenson, Kiehl and Fitzpatrick1995). This self-report method contained 26 questions related to psychopathic traits scored on a five-point Likert scale which scored using strongly disagree, disagree, neutral, agree, and strongly agree. Examples of question included were “Before I do anything, I carefully consider the possible consequences” and “I enjoy manipulating other people’s feelings.” Cronbach’s alpha for the LSRP was 0.82 overall (Falkenbach et al., Reference Falkenbach, Poythress, Falki and Manchak2007).

Eriksen’s Flanker task

The Flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, Reference Eriksen and Eriksen1974) assesses response inhibition. The task consists of five practice and 20 test trials. In each trial, there was a pattern of five fish against a white background – which has been shown to result in higher retention rates in immediate recall tasks (McConnohie, Reference McConnohie1999). The patterns were manipulated in nine of the 20 trials to show incongruent patterns of fish; and the participant had to correctly identify the correct direction of the central fish in the pattern as quickly as they could by pressing either the F key which indicated left or J which indicated right. The trials had a mixture of exposure times which ranged from 400 to 800 ms. Gorilla experimental software was used to generate the task and record the reaction times and number of correct responses for each trial. Cronbach’s alpha for the task was reported as 0.53 for congruent−incongruent reaction times.

Procedure

Participants who agreed to take part were led to a web screen which presented the participant information sheet and consent form. Once consent was established, participants were led to a screen which contained the CTES for them to complete. Participants then completed the LSRP before they were directed to the Flanker task. All tasks included instructions for the participant before they were able to complete them. Upon completion of these tasks, participants were shown an on-screen debrief. Ethical approval was granted by the University’s Ethics Committee on 3rd December 2019. The BPS ethical code of conduct was adhered to for the duration of the study.

Results

Sixty-five of the eighty participants reported at least one incidence of childhood trauma.

Hypothesis 1: Those who report higher levels of childhood trauma will score higher on measures of psychopathic traits.

Childhood trauma experience was assessed by totaling the scores of “intensity of childhood trauma” and levels of “confiding in others.” Higher frequencies of trauma, and subjective levels of their intensity, and lower scores of confiding resulted in higher trauma scores overall. Participants’ self-reported psychopathy scores were calculated by the sum of 26 responses scored on a scale from one to five: with higher scores indicating a higher psychopathy level.

To explore whether childhood traumatic experience is a significant predictor of psychopathy score, a linear regression analysis was applied (Table 1). The results of the regression analysis revealed that the full model did not significantly predict psychopathic traits (F(1,78) = 1.67 p = .201). More specifically, childhood trauma (CTES score) did not significantly predict psychopathic trait score (B = .14, t = 1.29, p = .201). For those who experienced no trauma at all during childhood, the average psychopathy score on the LSRP was marginally smaller. This indicates a possible weak relationship between childhood trauma exposure and psychopathic traits. These findings indicate that experience of childhood trauma does not predict development of psychopathic traits.

Table 1. Linear regression analyses for childhood trauma (CTES score) as a predictor of psychopathy childhood trauma (CTES score) as a predictor of response inhibition (mean reaction time)

Hypothesis 2: Those who report higher levels of childhood trauma will score lower on measures of response inhibition.

To explore whether childhood traumatic experience could predict response inhibition score, a linear regression analysis was applied (Table 1). The results of the regression analysis revealed that the full model did not significantly predict response inhibition (F(1,78) = 1.80, p = .183). More specifically, childhood trauma (CTES score) did not significantly predict response inhibition (Flanker task mean reaction time) (B = .079, t = 1.342, p = .183). These findings indicate that experience of childhood trauma does not predict development of response inhibition.

Discussion

The present study aimed to determine the presence of a predictive relationship between childhood trauma and response inhibition as well as childhood trauma and psychopathic traits. It was predicted that those who had reported higher levels of trauma during childhood would display higher self-reported psychopathy scores using the LSRP (Levenson et al., Reference Levenson, Kiehl and Fitzpatrick1995), and also have a longer reaction time to correct answers on the Flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, Reference Eriksen and Eriksen1974). These predicted effects were not found. Our analyses indicated no significant predictor effect of childhood trauma on psychopathic traits or response inhibition. Therefore, contrary to suggestions from the previous literature, our findings show no evidence of a link between self-reported childhood trauma exposure and response inhibition or psychopathic traits.

Arguably, the most notable contribution that the present study offers is that the findings further support and strengthen the related literature of the prevalence of childhood trauma in the population. Sixty-five of the eighty participants reported at least one incidence of childhood trauma, which conforms to many large-scale studies that report prevalence rates of between 15% and 80% (Alisic et al., Reference Alisic, Van der Schoot, Van Ginkel and Kleber2008). As the results from the current study favor the higher estimate, it could therefore be argued that the prevalence of childhood trauma may in fact be higher than the current literature suggests. This therefore opens avenues for further study to validate the accepted knowledge.

There is a large body of literature that demonstrates a link between childhood trauma, neurobiological abnormalities, and development of executive functions (Courchesne et al., Reference Courchesne, Mouton, Calhoun, Semendeferi, Ahrens-Barbaeu, Hallet and Pierce2011; McCrory et al., Reference McCrory, De Brito and Viding2012; Pechtel & Pizzagalli, Reference Pechtel and Pizzagalli2011; Perlstein et al., Reference Perlstein, Carter, Noll and Cohen2001; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Hansen and Li2011; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Fan and Qiu2015). Evidence also suggests that children with historical experiences of abuse and PTSD perform poorly on measures of executive function compared with typical children and are more impulsive than their matched peers (De Bellis et al., Reference De Bellis, Hooper, Spratt and Woolley2009). The findings we have presented here offer a new insight into this relationship, suggesting that at least some aspects of executive function may be left unimpaired by traumatic experience, or at least an individual’s own perception of their traumatic experience. Further research should explore the differences and impact of retrospective self-perceived trauma versus trauma measured or observed by others.

Working memory has been found to be impaired in trauma-exposed children (Bucker et al., Reference Bucker, Kapczinski, Posy, Cereser, Szobot, Yatham, Kapczinski and Kauer-SantʼAnna2012). Maltreated children have also been found to have poorer emotional regulation than controls (Eturk & Gor, 2018; Pechtel & Pizzagalli, Reference Pechtel and Pizzagalli2011; Zou et al., Reference Zou, Meng, Ma, Deng, Du, Wang, Chen and Hu2013). Emotional regulation is also regarded as significant in psychopathy research (Casey et al., Reference Casey, Rogers, Burns and Yiend2013), suggesting a possible link between childhood trauma, psychopathy and executive functioning. Being that there is wide support for the effect of childhood trauma in a variety of executive functions, it has long been assumed that response inhibition, as a form of executive functioning, would be affected by trauma exposure. Childhood trauma exposure was previously found to account for over half of the variation in response inhibition (Ji & Wang, Reference Ji and Wang2018). This research was further strengthened by the corroboration of similar studies (Madole et al., Reference Madole, Johnson and Carver2019) and meta-analyses of 55 studies that supported a small to medium effect size of −0.46 (Op den Kelder et al., Reference Op den Kelder, Ensink, Overbeek, Maric and Lindauer2017). Therefore, it is plausible that trauma exposure in childhood can in fact impair an individuals’ response inhibition in later life. Based on the previous research evidence, the nonsignificant results that were found in the present study were not expected.

The CTES questionnaire used in the present study assessed the spectrum of childhood traumas in brief detail. The analyses examined the results of trauma exposure as a general phenomenon rather than separately analyzing each type of trauma assessed. As research demonstrates, childhood sexual abuse and neglect are commonly associated with long-term consequences and are frequently mentioned in the literature (Mandelli et al., Reference Mandelli, Petrelli and Serretti2015; Spinhoven et al., Reference Spinhoven, Elzinga, Van Hemert, de Rooij and Penninx2016). More in-depth analyses of each type of childhood trauma, and more in-depth accounts of individual perceptions of trauma intensity, would help clarify the true nature of this relationship.

As with the majority of studies, the design and results of the present study are subject to limitations. The cross-sectional nature of the study lacks predictive power in that it cannot account for and measure later behavior (Bland, Reference Bland2001). This fails to accurately measure whether participants’ psychopathic traits remain consistent over time; in addition to response inhibition levels that can degrade throughout the lifespan (Borella et al., Reference Borella, Carretti and De Beni2008). However, it is the case that the majority of studies in this area utilize cross-sectional designs and effects have still been found. Nevertheless, a temporal relationship between trauma exposure and any observed effects is central in this area of research and should be investigated.

It is evident that further studies are needed to explore this area thoroughly. Further research may utilize other methodologies, sample populations, and study designs but must ultimately aim to expand the findings of the present study to ascertain why the results found occurred and whether future research can support or refute the relationship between childhood trauma, response inhibition and psychopathy.

Furthermore, future research should strive to investigate each type of childhood trauma separately, for example the possible effects of childhood sexual abuse or childhood neglect on later functioning to determine whether effects may be observed. Current research has shown that complex trauma – that which is multiple and persistent − to have more of an adverse effect on later functioning, particularly in relation to depressive and posttraumatic stress diagnoses (Op den Kelder et al., Reference Op den Kelder, Ensink, Overbeek, Maric and Lindauer2017). In their study, youth exposed to single trauma reported less deficits in executive functioning compared to the complex trauma group and as a result executive functioning was only found to mediate PTSD symptoms for the complex trauma group. As the present study’s focus was that of trauma in general, regardless of type; the results found may not necessarily align with that of future studies if the scope was on the effects of complex traumas. It may have been the case that the sample in the present study contained a significantly larger proportion of single trauma participants that skewed the results in a way to result in nonsignificant findings. Consequently, future studies should aim to test the effects of both complex and single traumas in later functioning separately to ascertain whether effects can be found.

Methodological triangulation whereby qualitative methods are also employed to explore participants’ experiences of their childhood events may provide further insight into how individuals perceive their childhoods as traumatic or not and which events they regarded as more traumatic. Though events such as the death of a parent or personal illness are regarded by many measures of childhood adversity as traumatic (Pennebaker & Susman, Reference Pennebaker and Susman2013), whether this is categorized as a traumatic event by an individual is not certain. Thus, trauma experience is a complex and subjective phenomenon that may only have an effect on an individuals’ later behavior if the victim processes the event as traumatic.

Conclusion

The present study aimed to investigate the presence of a relationship between childhood trauma and psychopathy, as well as a relationship between childhood trauma and response inhibition. Data analysis revealed no significant role of self-reported childhood trauma in predicting psychopathic traits or response inhibition. These findings add further insight into our understanding of susceptibility to a range of psychological disorders and executive function impairments. Future directions for research should strive to establish the specific nature of the relationship between traumas, perceived traumas, and executive function development. Ultimately, the research provides an insight into previously undescribed consequences of trauma exposure that may assist in the awareness and education of adverse childhoods for later psychological wellbeing.

Funding statement

This work received no specific grant funding from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

Alisic, E., Jongmans, M. J., van Wesel, F., & Kleber, R. J. (2011). Building child trauma theory from longitudinal studies: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(5), 736747. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.03.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alisic, E., Van der Schoot, T. A. W., Van Ginkel, J. R., & Kleber, R. J. (2008). Trauma exposure in primary school children: Who is at risk? Journal of Child and Adolescent Trauma, 1, 263269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alisic, E., Zalta, A. K., Van Wesel, F., Larsen, S. E., Hafstad, G. S., Hassanpour, K.Smid, G. E. (2014). Rates of post-traumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed children and adolescents: Meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 204(5), 335340. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.113.131227 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, V., Godfrey, C., Rosenfeld, J., & Catroppa, C. (2012). Predictors of cognitive function and recovery 10 years after traumatic brain injury in young children. Pediatrics, 129(2), 254261. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-0311 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bland, M. (2001). An introduction to medical statistics (3rd edn. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Borella, E., Carretti, B., & De Beni, R. (2008). Working memory and inhibition across the adult life-span. Acta Psychologica, 128(1), 3344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.09.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucker, J., Kapczinski, F., Posy, R., Cereser, K. M., Szobot, C., Yatham, L. N., Kapczinski, N. S., & Kauer-SantʼAnna, M. (2012). Cognitive impairment in school-aged children with early trauma. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 53(6), 758764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camerer, C. F., Dreber, A., Holzmeister, F., Ho, T. H., Huber, J., Johannesson, M.Altmejd, A. (2018). Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in Nature and Science between 2010 and 2015. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(9), 637644. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0399-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, H., Rogers, R. D., Burns, T., & Yiend, J. (2013). Emotion regulation in psychopathy. Biological Psychology, 92(3), 541548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.06.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castaneda, A. E., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Marttunen, M., Suvisaari, J., & Lönnqvist, J. (2008). A review on cognitive impairments in depressive and anxiety disorders with a focus on young adults. Journal of Affective Disorders, 106(1-2), 127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2007.06.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coid, J., Yang, M., Ullrich, S., Roberts, A., & Hare, R. D. (2009). Prevalence and correlates of psychopathic traits in the household population of Great Britain. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 32(2), 6573. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2009.01.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courchesne, E., Mouton, P. R., Calhoun, M. E., Semendeferi, K., Ahrens-Barbaeu, C., Hallet, M. J.Pierce, K. (2011). Neuron number and size in prefrontal cortex of children with autism. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 306(18), 20012010. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2011.1638 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craparo, G., Schimmenti, A., & Caretti, V. (2013). Traumatic experiences in childhood and psychopathy: A study on a sample of violent offenders from Italy. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 4, 16. https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v4i0.21471 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Data Resource Center for Child & Adolescent Health. The National Survey of Children’s Health 2012, https://www.childhealthdata.org/learn-about-the-nsch/NSCH.Google Scholar
De Bellis, M. (2001). Developmental traumatology: The psychobiological development of maltreated children and its implications for research, treatment, and policy. Development and Psychopathology, 13(3), 539564. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579401003078 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Bellis, M. D., Hooper, S. R., Spratt, E. G., & Woolley, D. P. (2009). Neuropsychological findings in childhood neglect and their relationships to paediatric PTSD. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(6), 868878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dikmen, S. S., Corrigan, J. D., Levin, H. S., Machamer, J., Stiers, W., & Weisskopf, M. G. (2009). Cognitive outcome following traumatic brain injury. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 24(6), 430438. https://doi.org/10.1097/HTR.0b013e3181c133e9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, E., Nishimi, K., Gomez, S. H., Powers, A., & Bradley, B. (2018). Developmental timing of trauma exposure and emotion dysregulation in adulthood: Are there sensitive periods when trauma is most harmful? Journal of Affective Disorders, 227, 869877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143149. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fairchild, G., van Goozen, S. H., Stollery, S. J., Aitken, M. R., Savage, J., Moore, S. C.Goodyer, I. M. (2009). Decision making and executive function in male adolescents with early-onset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder and control subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 66(2), 162168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falkenbach, D., Poythress, N., Falki, M., & Manchak, S. (2007). Reliability and validity of two self-report measures of psychopathy. Assessment, 14(4), 341350. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191107305612 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, N., Kimonis, E. R., Wasserman, A. L., & Kline, S. M. (2012). Associations among childhood abuse and psychopathy facets in male sexual offenders. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 3(1), 6675. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025605 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ji, S., & Wang, H. (2018). A study of the relationship between adverse childhood experiences, life events, and executive function among college students in China. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 31(1), 28. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-018-0107-y Google ScholarPubMed
Kaczmarczyk, M., Wingenfeld, K., Kuehl, L. K., Otte, C., & Hinkelmann, K. (2018). Childhood trauma and diagnosis of major depression: Association with memory and executive function. Psychiatry Research, 270, 880886. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.10.071 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K. A., & Fitzpatrick, C. M. (1995). Assessing psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalized population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(1), 151. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.68.1.151 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, X. B., Bo, Q. J., Zhang, G. P., Zheng, W., Wang, Z. M., Li, A. N.Wang, C. Y. (2017). Effect of childhood trauma on cognitive functions in a sample of Chinese patients with schizophrenia. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 76, 147152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.04.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madole, J. W., Johnson, S. L., & Carver, C. S. (2019). A model of aggressive behavior: Early adversity, impulsivity, and response inhibition. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 1, 117. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926771.2019.1591561 Google Scholar
Malarbi, S., Abu-Rayya, H. M., Muscara, F., & Stargatt, R. (2017). Neuropsychological functioning of childhood trauma and post-traumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 72, 6886. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandelli, L., Petrelli, C., & Serretti, A. (2015). The role of specific early trauma in adult depression: A meta-analysis of published literature. Childhood trauma and adult depression. European Psychiatry, 30(6), 665680. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.04.007 Google ScholarPubMed
Marcusson-Clavertz, D., Gušić, S., Bengtsson, H., Jacobsen, H., & Cardeña, E. (2017). The relation of dissociation and mind wandering to unresolved/disorganized attachment: An experience sampling study. Attachment & Human Development, 19(2), 170190. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2016.1261914 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, L. A., & Cooke, D. J. (1999). The childhood experiences of psychopaths: A retrospective study of familial and societal factors. Journal of Personality Disorders, 13(3), 211225. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.1999.13.3.211 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McConnohie, B. V.. A Study of the Effect of Color in Memory Retention When Used in Presentation Software (Unpublished master’s thesis) 1999. Tennessee.: Johnson Bible College, https://eric.ed.gov/?.Google Scholar
McCrory, E., De Brito, S. A., & Viding, E. (2012). The link between child abuse and psychopathology: A review of neurobiological and genetic research. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 105(4), 151156. https://doi.org/10.1258/jrsm.2011.110222 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miu, A. C., Bîlc, M. I., Bunea, I., & Szentágotai-Tătar, A. (2017). Childhood trauma and sensitivity to reward and punishment: Implications for depressive and anxiety symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 119(1), 134140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.07.015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mostofsky, S. H., & Simmonds, D. J. (2008). Response inhibition and response selection: Two sides of the same coin. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(5), 751761. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20500 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Donnell, B. F., & Hetrick, W. P. (2016). Psychophysiology of mental health. In Encyclopedia of mental health (2nd ed. pp. 372376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Op den Kelder, R., Ensink, J. B., Overbeek, G., Maric, M., & Lindauer, R. J. (2017). Executive function as a mediator in the link between single or complex trauma and posttraumatic stress in children and adolescents. Quality of Life Research, 26(7), 16871696. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-017-1535-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pechtel, P., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2011). Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: An integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology, 214(1), 5570. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-2009-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennebaker, J. W., & Susman, J. R.. Childhood traumatic events scale. [Measurement instrument]. 2013. https://www.midss.org/sites/default/files/trauma.pdf.Google Scholar
Perlstein, W. M., Carter, C. S., Noll, D. C., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Relation of prefrontal cortex dysfunction to working memory and symptoms in schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(7), 11051113. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.158.7.1105 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piquero, A. R., Farrington, D. P., Fontaine, N. M., Vincent, G., Coid, J., & Ullrich, S. (2012). Childhood risk, offending trajectories, and psychopathy at age 48 years in the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 18(4), 577. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027061 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spinhoven, P., Elzinga, B. M., Van Hemert, A. M., de Rooij, M., & Penninx, B. W. (2016). Childhood maltreatment, maladaptive personality types and level and course of psychological distress: A six-year longitudinal study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 191, 100108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.036 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuvblad, C., Wang, P., Bezdjian, S., Raine, A., & Baker, L. A. (2016). Psychopathic personality development from ages 9 to 18: Genes and environment. Development and Psychopathology, 28(1), 2744. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000267 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twamley, E. W., Allard, C. B., Thorp, S. R., Norman, S. B., Cissell, S. H., Berardi, K. H.Stein, M. B. (2009). Cognitive impairment and functioning in PTSD related to intimate partner violence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(6), 879887. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2009.03.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wamser-Nanney, R., Howell, K. H., Schwartz, L. E., & Hasselle, A. J. (2018). The moderating role of trauma type on the relationship between event centrality of the traumatic experience and mental health outcomes. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 10(5), 499. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0000344 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, K. R., Hansen, D. J., & Li, M. (2011). The traumatic stress response in child maltreatment and resultant neuropsychological effects. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16(2), 8797. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2010.12.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhou, Y., Fan, L., & Qiu, C. (2015). Prefrontal cortex and the dysconnectivity hypothesis of schizophrenia. Neuroscience Bulletin, 31, 207219. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12264-014-1502-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zou, Z., Meng, H., Ma, Z, Deng, W, Du, L, Wang, H., Chen, P., & Hu, H. (2013). Executive functioning deficits and childhood trauma in juvenile violent offenders in China. Psychiatry Research, 207(3), 218224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Linear regression analyses for childhood trauma (CTES score) as a predictor of psychopathy childhood trauma (CTES score) as a predictor of response inhibition (mean reaction time)