Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T03:50:58.342Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

On the overlap of vital exhaustion and depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 March 2020

Abstract

Type
Viewpoint
Copyright
Copyright © European Psychiatric Association 2017

“A potential hazard is that using different terms for a similar phenomenon produces confusion within a research community, leading to miscommunication and misunderstandings that can impede theory development. This concern is compounded in that the advancement of existing theory relies on future researchers’ ability to build on previous work.”

Cole et al. (2012, p. 1573).

Vital exhaustion (VE) has been defined as a syndrome combining pervasive fatigue, irritability, and demoralization and resulting from a lack of control over important aspects of one's life [Reference Appels and Mulder1,Reference Appels, Höppener and Mulder2]. Whether VE is anything other than an unnecessary variation on the theme of depression remains an unresolved question [Reference Frestad and Prescott3]. As an illustration, in an often-cited study, van Diest and Appels [Reference van Diest and Appels4] found that men categorized as exhausted:

  • globally reported more depressive symptoms (e.g., “fatigability” and “sleep disturbance”) than men categorized as non-exhausted;

  • but seldom complained about depressed mood—a core manifestation of depression [5].

These findings led these authors to suggest that VE is distinct from depression. The study, however, had a number of limitations that prevented clear conclusions from being drawn:

  • VE was arbitrarily categorized following a tercile-based split;

  • the study samples were small (ns ≤ 12);

  • and only male participants were examined.

Moreover, van Diest and Appels [Reference van Diest and Appels4] did not consider that, especially in men, feelings such as sadness (a component of depressed mood) can be denied at first and only become apparent when in-depth clinical interviews are conducted [5]; such interviews were not carried out in the study. The problem of the distinctiveness of VE with respect to depression may not be resolvable without an integrative reflection on the symptom picture and measurement of VE.

While the debate remains open, the view that VE is distinct from depression has prevailed in the literature—VE continues to be studied as a separate entity. In this viewpoint article, we propose a critical analysis of the VE construct, pointing to overlooked similarities between VE and depression. We argue that:

On these grounds, we call for a reconsideration of the view that VE is distinct from depression.

1. Comparative symptomatology of VE and depression

The first component of VE, pervasive fatigue, is also one of the nine diagnostic criteria for major depressive disorder (MDD) [5]. Pervasive fatigue is additionally involved in the diagnosis of persistent depressive disorder (dysthymia), a less acute form of depression [5]. Importantly, the claim that the primacy of pervasive fatigue distinguishes VE from depression [Reference Appels and Mulder1] overlooks the fact that pervasive fatigue can be a central symptom in many forms of depression, for example, depression with atypical features.Footnote 1

Although not a diagnostic criterion for MDD in the latest edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5), irritability, the second component of VE, is highly prevalent among depressed individuals. As mentioned in the DSM-5, “many individuals report or exhibit increased irritability (e.g., persistent anger, a tendency to respond to events with angry outbursts or blaming others, an exaggerated sense of frustration over minor matters)” when experiencing MDD ([5], p. 163). Judd et al. [Reference Judd, Schettler, Coryell, Akiskal and Fiedorowicz6] even found that the presence of irritability/anger during major depressive episodes was a clinical marker of a more severe, chronic, and complex depressive illness.

Demoralization, the third and last component of VE, is characterized by feelings of impotence, isolation, and despair and is associated with damaged self-esteem [Reference Frank7]. As made explicit by Frank [Reference Frank7], depressive symptoms are “direct expressions of demoralization” (p. 271). Feelings of impotence, isolation, and despair are usual constituents of depressed mood. This observation further calls into question van Diest and Appels's [Reference van Diest and Appels4] conclusion that depressed mood plays a minor role in VE.

The three defining features of VE overlap with long-documented manifestations of depressive conditions. The symptomatology-based arguments that have been put forward to justify the claim that VE is a unique syndrome do not stand up to close scrutiny.

2. Assessing the assessor

The overlap of VE with depression is further detectable in the Maastricht Questionnaire [Reference Appels, Höppener and Mulder2]—the instrument of reference for the assessment of VE. As can be seen from Appendix A, the items of the Maastricht Questionnaire refer to pervasive fatigue and loss of energy (e.g., items 1 and 4), sleep disturbance (e.g., item 3), cognitive impairment (e.g., item 21), death wishes (item 16), feelings of helplessness, apathy, and hopelessness (e.g., items 6, 8, and 10), irritability (e.g., item 12), loss of libido (item 9), and lowered mood (e.g., items 14 and 18). Most of these symptoms correspond to established diagnostic criteria for depression; all of them pertain to depressive clinical pictures [5].

Consistent with these observations, Wojciechowski et al. [Reference Wojciechowski, Strik, Falger, Lousberg and Honig8] found strong and virtually identical correlations between two measures of depression (the Zung Self-Rating Depression Scale and the depression subscale of the SCL-90) and the Maastricht Questionnaire at four different measurement points. Based on principal component analyses, Vroege et al. [Reference Vroege, Zuidersma and de Jonge9] did not find evidence that the symptoms of VE, as assessed with the Maastricht Questionnaire, exceeded the scope of depressive manifestations, as assessed by the Beck Depression Inventory. VE specifically overlapped with the somatic-affective dimension of depression. Frestad and Prescott [Reference Frestad and Prescott3] conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of 16 studies dedicated to the impact of VE on coronary heart disease (CHD). Remarkably, in the only included study in which depression (assessed with the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression scale) was analyzed as a potential confounder, VE no longer predicted CHD once depressive symptoms were controlled.

All in all, there is converging evidence that VE is integral part of the depressive spectrum. Some investigators, however, may suppose that VE represents a specific subtype of depression. Whether a new, VE-inspired subtype of depression is needed requires further investigation. This being noted, depression with atypical features, an existing subtype of depression, may already overlap VE. Pervasive fatigue (e.g., loss of energy, hypersomnia, leaden paralysis) lies at the heart of depression with atypical features [5]. In addition, interpersonal rejection sensitivity, a diagnostic criterion for depression with atypical features, is an aspect of the demoralization component of VE [Reference Frank7].

3. Conclusion

Our reexamination of the symptoms and main measure of VE suggests that the overlap of VE with depression has been problematically underestimated due to inconsistent comparisons and blindness to definitional similarities. In our view, the case of VE is emblematic of a trend toward construct proliferation [Reference Cole, Walter, Bedeian and O’Boyle10]. Many “new” constructs are introduced in the literature precipitously, with insufficient examination of their added value, and based on “linguistic contortions” and anecdotal evidence rather than robust empirical findings and sound theorizing. Such constructs are often “distinguished” from existing ones on the basis of specious arguments and missing-the-point conclusions. This is for instance the case when Appels and Mulder [Reference Appels and Mulder1] overlook the fact that depressed mood and lowered self-esteem cannot be invoked to distinguish depression from VE because depressed mood and lowered self-esteem are constituents of demoralization, a component of VE. We plead for a greater respect of the principle of parsimony, in the interest of theoretical clarity and effective transdisciplinary communication.

To close this article, we note that VE has often been equated with burnout [Reference Frestad and Prescott3]. Interestingly, burnout has been found to overlap with depression. These findings provide indirect support for the view that VE is a depressive condition. Whether other exhaustion syndromes such as neurasthenia—originally considered to be part of “melancholia”—and compassion fatigue—a recently described condition with fuzzy definitional contours—would not be better conceived of as depressive syndromes should be clarified.

Disclosure of interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interest.

Appendix A The Maastricht Questionnaire (21-item version) [Reference Appels, Höppener and Mulder2]

Medical research is constantly trying to track down the causes of disease. You would help this research by answering the following questions about how you feel lately. Please mark the answers that are true for you. If you don’t know or cannot decide circle the ‘?’. There are no ‘right’ or ‘wrong’ answers.

Footnotes

1 The term atypical “does not connote an uncommon or unusual clinical presentation” ([5], p. 186).

References

Appels, AMulder, PFatigue and heart disease. The association between ‘vital exhaustion’ and past, present and future coronary heart disease. J Psychosom Res 1989; 33(6): 727738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Appels, AHöppener, PMulder, PA questionnaire to assess premonitory symptoms of myocardial infarction. Int J Cardiol 1987; 17(1): 1524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frestad, DPrescott, EVital exhaustion and coronary heart disease risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychosom Med 2017; 79(3): 260272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Diest, RAppels, AVital exhaustion and depression: a conceptual study. J Psychosom Res 1991; 35(4–5): 535544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th ed., Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.Google Scholar
Judd, LLSchettler, PJCoryell, WAkiskal, HSFiedorowicz, JGOvert irritability/anger in unipolar major depressive episodes: past and current characteristics and implications for long-term course. JAMA Psychiatry 2013; 70(11): 11711180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, JDPsychotherapy: the restoration of morale. Am J Psychiatry 1974; 131(3): 271274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wojciechowski, FLStrik, JJMHFalger, PLousberg, RHonig, AThe relationship between depressive and vital exhaustion symptomatology post-myocardial infarction. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2000; 102(5): 359365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vroege, EMZuidersma, Mde Jonge, PVital exhaustion and somatic depression: the same underlying construct in patients with myocardial infarction?. Psychosom Med 2012; 74(5): 446451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, MSWalter, FBedeian, AGO’Boyle, EHJob burnout and employee engagement: a meta-analytic examination of construct proliferation. J Manag 2012; 38(5): 15501581.Google Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.