Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-nwzlb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T10:19:36.753Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Brain games: Toward a neuroecology of social behavior

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 July 2013

Jean-François Gariépy
Affiliation:
Department of Neurobiology, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, NC 27701. jeanfrancois.gariepy@gmail.com Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708. steve.chang@duke.edu
Steve W. C. Chang
Affiliation:
Department of Neurobiology, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, NC 27701. jeanfrancois.gariepy@gmail.com Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708. steve.chang@duke.edu
Michael L. Platt
Affiliation:
Department of Neurobiology, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, NC 27701. jeanfrancois.gariepy@gmail.com Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708. steve.chang@duke.edu Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708 Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, and Levine Science Research Center, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708. platt@neuro.duke.eduhttp://www.neuro.duke.edu/faculty/platt/

Abstract

In the target article, Schilbach et al. defend a “second-person neuroscience” perspective that focuses on the neural basis of social cognition during live, ongoing interactions between individuals. We argue that a second-person neuroscience would benefit from formal approaches borrowed from economics and behavioral ecology and that it should be extended to social interactions in nonhuman animals.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Azzi, J. C., Sirigu, A. & Duhamel, J. R. (2012) Modulation of value representation by social context in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 109:2126–31.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (2005) Continuity of the conceptual system across species. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9:309–11.Google Scholar
Baumgartner, T., Fischbacher, U., Feierabend, A., Lutz, K. & Fehr, E. (2009) The neural circuitry of a broken promise. Neuron 64:756–70.Google Scholar
Baumgartner, T., Knoch, D., Hotz, P., Eisenegger, C. & Fehr, E. (2011) Dorsolateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex orchestrate normative choice. Nature Neuroscience 14:1468–74.Google Scholar
Becchio, C., Cavallo, A., Begliomini, C., Sartori, L., Feltrin, G. & Castiello, U. (2012) Social grasping: From mirroring to mentalizing. Neuroimage 61(1):240–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, C. D. & Emery, N. J. (2010) Rooks perceive support relations similar to six-month-old babies. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B: Biological Sciences 277:147–51.Google ScholarPubMed
Braun, D. A., Ortega, P. A. & Wolpert, D. M. (2009) Nash equilibria in multi-agent motor interactions. PLoS Computational Biology 5:e1000468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, C. J., Tobler, P. N., Schultz, W. & Baddeley, M. (2010) Striatal BOLD response reflects the impact of herd information on financial decisions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 4:48.Google ScholarPubMed
Chang, S. W., Barter, J. W., Ebitz, R. B., Watson, K. K. & Platt, M. L. (2012) Inhaled oxytocin amplifies both vicarious reinforcement and self reinforcement in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 109:959–64.Google Scholar
Chang, S. W., Gariépy, J. F. & Platt, M. L. (2013) Neuronal reference frames for social decisions in primate frontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience 16:243–50.Google Scholar
Chang, S. W., Winecoff, A. A. & Platt, M. L. (2011) Vicarious reinforcement in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Frontiers in Neuroscience 5:27.Google Scholar
Cooper, J. C., Kreps, T. A., Wiebe, T., Pirkl, T. & Knutson, B. (2010) When giving is good: Ventromedial prefrontal cortex activation for others' intentions. Neuron 67:511–21.Google Scholar
Debreu, G. (1952) A social equilibrium existence theorem. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 38:886–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J. (2010) The neurodevelopment of empathy in humans. Developmental Neuroscience 32:257–67.Google Scholar
Dorris, M. C. & Glimcher, P. W. (2004) Activity in posterior parietal cortex is correlated with the relative subjective desirability of action. Neuron 44:365–78.Google Scholar
Emery, N. J. & Clayton, N. S. (2001) Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature 414:443–46.Google Scholar
Fujii, N., Hihara, S. & Iriki, A. (2007) Dynamic social adaptation of motion-related neurons in primate parietal cortex. PLoS One 2:e397.Google Scholar
Gintis, H. (2009) The bounds of reason: Game theory and the unification of the behavioral sciences. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Gu, X., Liu, X., Guise, K. G., Naidich, T. P., Hof, P. R. & Fan, J. (2010) Functional dissociation of the frontoinsular and anterior cingulate cortices in empathy for pain. Journal of Neuroscience 30:3739–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayden, B. Y., Pearson, J. M. & Platt, M. L. (2011) Neuronal basis of sequential foraging decisions in a patchy environment. Nature Neuroscience 14:933–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heilbronner, S. R., Rosati, A. G., Stevens, J. R., Hare, B. & Hauser, M. D. (2008) A fruit in the hand or two in the bush? Divergent risk preferences in chimpanzees and bonobos. Biology Letters 4:246–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeon, D., Kim, S., Chetana, M., Jo, D., Ruley, H. E., Lin, S. Y., Rabah, D., Kinet, J. P. & Shin, H. S. (2010) Observational fear learning involves affective pain system and Cav1.2 Ca2+ channels in ACC. Nature Neuroscience 13:482–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kacelnik, A. & Bateson, M. (1996) Risky theories: The effects of variance on foraging decisions. American Zoology 36:402–34.Google Scholar
Knoch, D., Schneider, F., Schunk, D., Hohmann, M. & Fehr, E. (2009) Disrupting the prefrontal cortex diminishes the human ability to build a good reputation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106:20895–99.Google Scholar
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U. & Fehr, E. (2005) Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 435:673–76.Google Scholar
Lee, D. (2008) Game theory and neural basis of social decision making. Nature Neuroscience 11:404409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Platt, M. L. & Glimcher, P. W. (1999) Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature 400:233–38.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. & Sinigaglia, C. (2010) The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11(4):264–74. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2805.Google Scholar
Rudebeck, P. H., Buckley, M. J., Walton, M. E. & Rushworth, M. F. (2006) A role for the macaque anterior cingulate gyrus in social valuation. Science 313:1310–12.Google Scholar
Sanchez-Andrade, G. & Kendrick, K. M. (2009) The main olfactory system and social learning in mammals. Behavioural Brain Research 200:323–35.Google Scholar
Stephens, D. W., McLinn, C. M. & Stevens, J. R. (2002) Discounting and reciprocity in an Iterated Prisoner's Dilemma. Science 298:2216–18.Google Scholar
Sugrue, L. P., Corrado, G. S. & Newsome, W. T. (2004) Matching behavior and the representation of value in the parietal cortex. Science 304:1782–87.Google Scholar
Tomlin, D., Kayali, M. A., King-Casas, B., Anen, C., Camerer, C. F., Quartz, S. R. & Montague, P. R. (2006) Agent-specific responses in the cingulate cortex during economic exchanges. Science 312(5776):1047–50. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1125596.Google Scholar
Tsao, D. Y., Moeller, S. & Freiwald, W. A. (2008) Comparing face patch systems in macaques and humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 105:19514–19.Google Scholar
Washburn, D. A., Hopkins, W. D. & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1990) Effects of competition on video-task performance in monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Comparative Psychology 104:115–21.Google Scholar