Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T21:59:40.670Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Social Determinants of Loneliness During COVID-19: Personal, Community, and Societal Predictors and Implications for Treatment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 April 2023

Marlee Bower*
Affiliation:
The Matilda Centre for Research in Mental Health and Substance Use, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Eleisha Lauria
Affiliation:
The Matilda Centre for Research in Mental Health and Substance Use, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Olivia Green
Affiliation:
The Matilda Centre for Research in Mental Health and Substance Use, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Scarlett Smout
Affiliation:
The Matilda Centre for Research in Mental Health and Substance Use, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Julia Boyle
Affiliation:
The Matilda Centre for Research in Mental Health and Substance Use, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Amarina Donohoe-Bales
Affiliation:
The Matilda Centre for Research in Mental Health and Substance Use, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Genevieve Dingle
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Emma Barrett
Affiliation:
The Matilda Centre for Research in Mental Health and Substance Use, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Erin Fearn-Smith
Affiliation:
Discipline of Rehabilitation Counselling, Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Kevin Gournay
Affiliation:
The Matilda Centre for Research in Mental Health and Substance Use, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Maree Teesson
Affiliation:
The Matilda Centre for Research in Mental Health and Substance Use, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
*
*Corresponding author, Email: marlee.bower@sydney.edu.au

Abstract

Objective

The COVID-19 pandemic dramatically altered social determinants of health including work, education, social connections, movement, and perceived control; and loneliness was commonly experienced. This longitudinal study examined how social determinants at the personal (micro), community (meso), and societal (macro) levels predicted loneliness during the pandemic.

Methods

Participants were 2056 Australian adults surveyed up to three times over 18 months in 2020 and 2021. Multi-level mixed-effect regressions were conducted predicting loneliness from social determinants at baseline and two follow-ups.

Results

Loneliness was associated with numerous micro determinants: male gender, lifetime diagnosis of a mental health disorder, experience of recent stressful event(s), low income, living alone or couples with children, living in housing with low natural light, noise, and major building defects. Lower resilience and perceived control over health and life were also associated with greater loneliness. At the meso level, reduced engagement with social groups, living in inner regional areas, and living in neighbourhoods with low levels of belongingness and collective resilience was associated with increased loneliness. At the macro level, increased loneliness was associated with State/Territory of residence.

Conclusions

Therapeutic initiatives must go beyond psychological intervention, and must recognise the social determinants of loneliness at the meso and macro levels.

Type
Shorter Communication
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Australian Association for Cognitive and Behaviour Therapy

Introduction

During the COVID-19 pandemic, loneliness has been associated with worse mental health outcomes, including increased psychological distress and suicidal ideation (Allan et al., Reference Allan, Volarov, Koscinski, Pizzonia, Potter, Accorso and Allan2021). There is an urgent need to identify those at increased risk of loneliness to ensure that they are provided with appropriate therapeutic supports. Emerging research surrounding predictors of loneliness during COVID-19 has demonstrated that disadvantaged groups tend to be most at risk, including those with lower income and education, unemployment, living alone, and pre-existing physical or mental health conditions (Bu, Steptoe, & Fancourt, Reference Bu, Steptoe and Fancourt2020). The link between disadvantage and loneliness during COVID-19 can be understood by considering social determinants of mental health frameworks, which posit that one's social context and relative standing, including socio-economic status, employment, and living environments are strongly linked with mental health outcomes, including loneliness (McQuaid, Cox, Ogunlana, & Jaworska, Reference McQuaid, Cox, Ogunlana and Jaworska2021).

Social determinants may influence the extent to which an extreme event, like a pandemic, will influence a person's ongoing mental health, including experiences of loneliness (Mao & Agyapong, Reference Mao and Agyapong2021). Whitehead and colleagues (Reference Whitehead, Pennington, Orton, Nayak, Petticrew, Sowden and White2016) proposed a three-level framework that identifies pathways through which social determinants can influence mental health. Firstly, the micro/personal level, whereby a person's financial, informational and power-related resources influence their social standing, living conditions, personal resilience to external events, and control over decisions affecting their daily lives. Secondly, the meso/community level includes the collective capacity an individual has, as part of their membership in broader social groups/communities to create change, be resilient and control negative events, which may be more difficult in neighbourhoods with low resources. Finally, the macro/societal level includes influences on perceived control due to legislation, cultural norms and patterns of discrimination, and the broader context of socio-political transitions. Each of these levels are interrelated – for example, legislation made on a macro level will impact the control people have over their lives on a personal/micro level – and subsequently, the three levels should be explored in context. At each level, social determinants appeared to impact an individual's mental health by interacting with two psychological phenomena: their personal or collective resilience, or capacity to adapt well and continue functioning after such an event (Mao & Agyapong, Reference Mao and Agyapong2021) and perceived control over their daily lives and capacity to remain well (Lachman & Weaver, Reference Lachman and Weaver1998). While theory has linked these phenomena to loneliness (Perlman & Peplau, Reference Perlman, Peplau, Duck and Gilmour1981), limited research has explored this relationship empirically.

Figure 1 adapts this framework to illustrate the potential mechanisms through which the pandemic has resulted in increased loneliness, with impacts on social determinants, perceived control, and resilience. At the micro level, COVID-19 research has demonstrated that those confined to low-quality and unaffordable housing during lockdown experienced constrained control over their environment, particularly when lacking the financial resources to make home improvements, compared to those confined to better-quality housing (Bower et al., Reference Bower, Buckle, Rugel, Donohoe-Bales, McGrath, Gournay and Teesson2021). At the meso level, one example of collective resilience has been the advent of COVID-19 mutual aid groups, sharing resources to support community members experiencing isolation or financial stress (Mao, Drury, Fernandes-Jesus, & Ntontis, Reference Mao, Drury, Fernandes-Jesus and Ntontis2021). At the macro level, state-ordained social restrictions have impacted the perceived control people have over their daily lives throughout the pandemic (Misamer, Signerski-Krieger, Bartels, & Belz, Reference Misamer, Signerski-Krieger, Bartels and Belz2021).

Figure 1. Potential pathways of perceived control, resilience, and loneliness during the COVID-19 pandemic, based on theories of Whitehead et al. (Reference Whitehead, Pennington, Orton, Nayak, Petticrew, Sowden and White2016).

The current paper had several aims: (1) to explore which social determinants predicted loneliness during the first 2 years of the pandemic and (2) to explore the relationship between perceived control and personal and collective resilience with loneliness and social determinants.

Method

Participants

Analysis was conducted on longitudinal data collected over three time points from 7 July 2020 and 31 December 2021 as part of Alone Together, a 24-month study investigating the mental health impacts of COVID-19 among Australian adults. The baseline survey included 2056 individuals, with 65.66% retention (n = 1350) at 6 months and 62.79% (n = 1291) at 12 months, utilising participant data when they had completed all three waves of loneliness questions (n = 1512). This study was approved by The University of Sydney Human Research Ethics Committee (2020/460) and data were collected using Qualtrics software. Participants were recruited across all Australian states and territories through targeted advertisements on social media (i.e. Facebook, Instagram, Twitter), community websites, and physical flyers in public spaces. To reach a broad sample of Australians, including those unlikely to use social media, recruitment also occurred through charitable organisations providing support, social care, accommodation or health services for those experiencing, or at risk of, homelessness, isolation, and financial hardship. To be eligible to participate, participants were required 18+, residing in Australia and have sufficient English proficiency to understand the survey and consent procedures. Further description of recruitment and consent procedures are available in Bower et al. (Reference Bower, Buckle, Rugel, Donohoe-Bales, McGrath, Gournay and Teesson2021, Reference Bower, Donohoe-Bales, Smout, Ngyuen, Boyle, Barrett and Teesson2022).

Participants who had missing data on any of the predictors or outcome variables were excluded from analyses.

Measures

Loneliness (dependent variable)

Social and emotional loneliness was measured at three time points using the six-item De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale (DJGL-S; De Jong Gierveld & Van Tilburg, Reference De Jong Gierveld and Van Tilburg2010), resulting in a total loneliness score between 0 and 6, where higher scores indicated greater loneliness. Cronbach's alpha was satisfactory at each time point (α = 0.73, 0.78, and 0.73, respectively).

Micro/personal level independent variables

Demographics and life circumstances

Information was collected regarding participants’ gender identity, age, Aboriginal and/or Torres Strait Islander identity, current employment status, education, residency, language proficiency, mental health diagnosis, household income and expenditure, and LGBTQIA+ identity. See Supplementary Table 2 for coding information on demographic variables. Experiences of stressful life events within 12-months pre-COVID were measured using the List of Threatening Experiences Questionnaire (LTE-Q; Brugha & Cragg, Reference Brugha and Cragg1990), where a higher count indicated a higher number of experiences.

Living conditions

Living conditions, including number of major building problems, satisfaction with natural light in dwelling, frequency bothered by noise while indoors at home and family structure of household, were measured and adapted from The Australian Housing Conditions Dataset (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Daniel, Bentley, Pawson, Stone, Rajagopalan, Hulse, Beer, London and Randolph2018). See Supplementary Table 2 for coding information on these variables.

Resilience and control variables

The 10-item Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale-10 (Campbell-Sills & Stein, Reference Campbell-Sills and Stein2007), was used to measure personal resilience, on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (not true at all) to 4 (true nearly all the time), with higher scores indicating higher resilience. Perceived control both generally and over one's health was measured via the Perceived Control and Self-Reported Health Questionnaire (Bobak, Pikhart, Hertzman, Rose, & Marmot, Reference Bobak, Pikhart, Hertzman, Rose and Marmot1998) with responses coded between 0 (agree strongly) and 5 (disagree strongly), with higher scores signalling higher control. Baseline Cronbach's alpha was respectable for resilience (α = 0.92) and perceived control (α = 0.71).

Meso/community level independent variables

Neighbourhood/community characteristics

Based on postcode data provided in the baseline survey, two Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS) location index scores were generated, including Australia Remoteness Structure (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2018) and Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas Index of relative socio-economic disadvantage (IRSD) for residence area (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2018).

Personal perceptions of neighbourhood and community

Participants’ perceived neighbourhood belonging was measured using the nine-item Neighbourhood Belonging Scale (Young, Russell, & Powers, Reference Young, Russell and Powers2004), resulting in a total score between 9 and 45. Collective resilience was measured using the five-item Fletcher-Lyons Collective Resilience Scale, summing a total score between 5 and 35 (Lyons, Fletcher, & Bariola, Reference Lyons, Fletcher and Bariola2016). Baseline Cronbach's alpha was very good for collective resilience (α = 0.95). Higher scores on both scales indicated greater belongingness and resilience.

Social networks

The baseline survey measured participants’ social group membership using the four-item Exeter Identity Transition Scale (EXITS) (Haslam et al., Reference Haslam, Holme, Haslam, Iyer, Jetten and Williams2008), assessing both retrospective (pre-COVID) and current social group membership(s). Participants’ responses were coded on whether their engagement in multiple group membership(s), had changed since the start of the pandemic.

Macro/societal level independent variables

Participants were asked about their State/Territory of residence. This acted as a proxy variable to account for the variation in Australian COVID-19 government responses, particularly lockdowns and outbreaks.

Analysis

The STATA mixed command was used to fit two stepwise multi-level linear mixed-effect models predicting loneliness at baseline and two follow-up occasions. Random effects were clustered at a participant level and random slopes were fit, covarying for timing of each participant response. An unstructured covariance model was used. The first model accounted for social determinant variables, and the second model added resilience and control covariates. Given the large number of predictors, tests for multicollinearity were conducted using the STATA collin command, with no evidence for multicollinearity detected.

Due to the temporally staggered nature of participation at baseline, unequal spacing of subsequent follow-ups, and the specific timing of events that may have impacted loneliness (e.g. timing of COVID-19 outbreaks, state-based lockdowns/restrictions), the time variable used for analysis was days since the survey opened (e.g. Day 60, Day 242, Day 425). Timing of major outbreaks/lockdowns (see Supplementary Table 1) and preliminary analysis of graphs presenting loneliness over time suggested loneliness followed a non-linear trajectory; hence, a quadratic time variable was fitted into the model.

Results

Participant Demographic Characteristics

The sample ranged in age (18–89, median bracket: 40–49). Two-thirds (66.47%) identified as female, 8.12% spoke English as a second language, and 1.32% were Aboriginal and/or Torres Strait Islander. Median weekly income of the sample was between $1,075 and $1,700 per week (range: <$300 to >$2,400).

Loneliness

Loneliness mean score at baseline (July–December 2020) was 3.41 on the scale of 0–6 (SD = 1.87, n = 1825), 3.18 (SD = 2.04, n = 1185) at time two (January–June 2021), and 3.40 (SD = 1.90, n = 1269) at time three (July–December 2021).

Regression Analysis

After excluding missing data, 1523 participants were included in analyses, with 3442 observations across three time points.

The following results firstly report the results of model 1 (social determinants as predictors of loneliness) and then secondly reports any changes that occurred in model 2 (model 1 with addition of resilience and control covariates). All model findings reported account for all other variables.

Detailed results from the first (χ2(56, N = 1523) = 584.02, p ≤ .0001) and second models (χ2(59, N = 1512) = 1167.31, p ≤ .0001) are in Supplementary Table 3. Likelihood ratio testing showed that adding perceived control and resilience variables in model 2 made a significant contribution to loneliness, over and above the social determinants tested in model 1 (χ2 (3) = 450.10, p ≤ .0001).

In model 1, trajectories of loneliness showed significant linear (p = .006) and quadratic patterns (p = .003) over time, although the magnitude of change was small. The negative linear coefficient and very small positive quadratic coefficient suggest loneliness followed a slight convex trajectory over time. Examining fitted loneliness scores across time below (Figure 2), trajectories of loneliness appear to follow periods of COVID-19 outbreaks (often accompanied by lockdowns). These results were consistent in model 2.

Figure 2. Fitted loneliness trajectories with 95% confidence intervals over the 1.5 years since survey was opened (three time points). The end of the national lockdown associated with the Coronavirus-19 Alpha outbreak coincided with June 2020, and varying lockdowns associated with the Coronavirus-19 Delta outbreak started in late June 2021.

Micro/Personal Factors

Females had an average loneliness score .25 lower than males (p = .006). Those with a lifetime diagnosis for a mental health disorder had an average loneliness score .41 higher than those without (p ≤ .001). Those with higher income tended to be significantly less lonely (β = −.07, p = .041) compared to those with lower income. LGBTQIA+ or Aboriginal and/or Torres Strait Islander status, age, income loss, current employment and tertiary education were not significantly associated with loneliness (p ≥ .05).

Every additional stressful life event experienced in the year pre-COVID was associated with a .09 increase in loneliness score during the pandemic period (p = .003). However, once perceived control and resilience variables were added in model 2, mental health diagnoses, current income, and history of stressful life events were no longer associated with loneliness (p ≥ .05).

Living in a house with satisfactory natural light was associated with a .36-point reduction in loneliness score (p ≤ .001) and every additional major building problem (mould, etc.) was associated with a .11-point increase in loneliness score (p = .002). Issues with noise while at home was associated with a .22-point increase in loneliness, relative to those without noise issues (p = .03). Household family structure was associated with loneliness, whereby single-person households (β = .24, p = .012) and couples with children (β = .29, p = .02) were significantly lonelier than couples without children.

In model 2, living in a house with satisfactory natural light (β = −.31, p = .001) and cumulative major building problems both remained associated with loneliness (β = .07, p = .03), but issues with noise were no longer associated with loneliness (p ≥ .05). Lone person households (β = .31, p = .005) and parents living with adult children (β = .28, p = .025) were significantly lonelier than couples without children in both models. Participants with higher levels of personal resilience (β = −.03) and perceived control over their lives and health (β = −.07) at baseline were less likely to experience loneliness across time (p ≤ .001 for both).

Meso/Neighbourhood and Community Characteristics

In both models, living in inner regional areas, rather than major cities, was associated with an increase in loneliness score (model 1: β = .39, p ≤ .001; model 2: β = .30, p = .002). There were no significant differences across other remoteness indicators and socio-economic disadvantage of living area was not associated with loneliness (p > .05).

Feeling a greater sense of belonging to one's neighbourhood was associated with reduced loneliness (model 1: β = −.08, p ≤ .001; model 2: β = −.04, p ≤ .001). Added at model 2, participants with higher levels of collective resilience at baseline were less likely to experience loneliness across time (β = −.04, p ≤ .001).

Participants whose involvement in multiple social groups had reduced since COVID-19 were significantly lonelier during the pandemic than those whose social contact increased or stayed at pre-COVID levels (β = .37, p < .001). This association remained in model 2 (β = .28, p < .001).

Macro/Societal

There was a significant effect of State/Territory on loneliness, where participants from Victoria were significantly lonelier than participants in New South Wales (NSW) in model 1 (β = .27, p = .02). In model 2, participants from both Victoria (β = .23, p = .04) and Queensland (β = .38, p = .03) were significantly lonelier than those from NSW. Slopes were fitted, testing whether changes in loneliness differed by State/Territory over linear and quadratic time. There was one significant linear effect where participants from South Australia (SA) had a positive linear change, relative to those from NSW (β = 2.13, p = .03), but there was no significant quadratic effect. In model 2, significant linear (β = 2.14, p = .03) and negative quadratic effects (β = −1.44, p ≤ .05) were found for SA. Figure 3 shows the trajectories of States with significant effects, revealing a concave loneliness trajectory over the three time points for participants from SA, whereas participants from NSW showed a convex trajectory.

Figure 3. Fitted loneliness trajectories over the 1.5 years since survey was opened (three time points), categorised by participants’ State/Territory of residence.

Discussion

Social determinants at the micro, meso, and macro levels contributed towards the likelihood of experiencing loneliness during the COVID-19 pandemic. This paper has clear implications for treatment and interventions around loneliness. Results may assist clinicians to identify clients at risk of experiencing loneliness during the pandemic, including Australians who live alone, with children, or in regional areas; are male; have a mental disorder; low income; low sense of belonging to their neighbourhood; low collective resilience; reduced social group membership; or recent experiences of stressful life events prior to COVID-19. People living in poorer quality housing (with building issues, noise, or low light) were also more likely to be lonely, revealing the significant impact of living environments on mental health. Clinicians could also use brief measures of loneliness, like the DJGL to confirm client loneliness.

There was evidence that, as hypothesised, perceived control over daily life and health and resilience (both personal and collective) may play a role in the relationship between some social determinants and loneliness. This was particularly the case among personal/micro-level social determinants, some of which – lower income, lifetime mental disorder, and living in noisy housing – have already been explicitly linked with lower perceived control (Bower et al., Reference Bower, Buckle, Rugel, Donohoe-Bales, McGrath, Gournay and Teesson2021; Eklund & Bäckström, Reference Eklund and Bäckström2006; Lachman & Weaver, Reference Lachman and Weaver1998). Others included being male and having experienced stressful life events in the year prior to COVID-19, such as divorce or natural disaster or couples living with children, compared to those living without. Other research has reflected on the added pressures of parenting and home learning during COVID-19 and resulting social impairment and mental ill-health (Calear et al., Reference Calear, McCallum, Morse, Banfield, Gulliver, Cherbuin and Batterham2022) and of adult children when pandemic-related job loss meant moving back home with ageing parents (Patulny & Bower, Reference Patulny and Bower2022). Although not examined explicitly, one can imagine such added responsibilities in living environment may impact one's resilience and control.

These results show building a sense of control and resilience among people at risk of, or experiencing, loneliness may buffer some of the negative effects of social determinants on health. As these psychological factors are often more malleable than social determinants (e.g. housing), there is a great opportunity for clinicians to work with people at risk of loneliness to target perceived control and resilience and potentially disrupt the impact such social determinants have on loneliness. Past research has confirmed higher perceived control is associated with positive effects on mental health for those experiencing financial disadvantage, low income, and pre-existing mental illness (Eklund & Bäckström, Reference Eklund and Bäckström2006; Lachman & Weaver, Reference Lachman and Weaver1998).

Other social determinants, mostly community level and living conditions were not impacted by resilience and perceived control, suggesting that they may be associated with loneliness through other pathways. These included living in housing that was dark or in poor-repair. Clinicians working with clients living in poor quality housing, without the financial means to make improvements, may wish to advocate for minimum-habitability housing standards for rental and public housing properties, to ensure that all Australians can live in health-promoting housing conditions, as New Zealand's Healthy Homes Standard has done (Tenancy Services New Zealand Government, 2021).

Participants who felt a sense of belonging to their neighbourhood were less lonely, regardless of how well-off their local area was. Disasters like COVID-19 may organically produce emergent neighbourhood ‘groups’, based on a shared identity and desire to help their community survive difficult situations (Drury et al., Reference Drury, Carter, Cocking, Ntontis, Tekin Guven and Amlôt2019). In other instances, simple community-based interventions can improve neighbourhood identification and effectively reduce loneliness (Fong et al., Reference Fong, Cruwys, Robinson, Haslam, Haslam, Mance and Fisher2021).

Participants who perceived their main social groups as more resilient against potential threats and changes tended to be less lonely. As were those who stayed involved in multiple social groups during the pandemic, relative to those whose networks became smaller and less diverse. Unfortunately, maintaining group relationships was not easy for Australians: COVID-19 restrictions prompted temporary or permanent disruption to many in-person community and hobby groups, e.g. choirs (Draper & Dingle, Reference Draper and Dingle2021). Clinicians can leverage the benefits of group membership: connecting clients to meaningful social groups can successfully reduce loneliness (Cruwys, Haslam, Rathbone, Williams, & Haslam, Reference Cruwys, Haslam, Rathbone, Williams and Haslam2021).

Although not tested explicitly, changes in loneliness over time initially appeared to mirror major COVID-19 outbreaks and lockdown periods, suggesting that enforced lockdowns and outbreaks are related to participant loneliness. However, this did not appear the case once differences were tested between States/Territories. Relative to NSW, participants from Queensland and Victoria tended to be lonelier during the pandemic, despite Queensland having fewer lockdowns and COVID-19 cases than NSW (see Supplementary Material). Participants from SA had a different loneliness trajectory than those from NSW, with increases in periods while SA was not experiencing lockdowns. Together, these findings suggest the relationship between lockdowns and loneliness is complex, and state/territory may not be a useful proxy. Future research may benefit from using the Oxford Stringency Index as a proxy for COVID-related restrictions, however these are only available at a national level, potentially missing nuances in local variability in restrictions (Hale et al., Reference Hale, Angrist, Goldszmidt, Kira, Petherick, Phillips and Helen2021). However, other research has also failed to find significant associations between COVID restrictions (e.g. social distancing or quarantine measures) and loneliness (Groarke et al., Reference Groarke, Berry, Graham-Wisener, McKenna-Plumley, McGlinchey and Armour2020).

Strengths and Weaknesses

This is the first Australian study to explore the complex interplay of micro/meso/macro factors and their conjoint influence on loneliness. Key strengths include our longitudinal design and data collection at pivotal points during the pandemic, when most existing pandemic research was cross-sectional, with data collected at a specific point-in-time (e.g. the beginning of a lockdown). This study also offered a unique exploration of the Australian social determinants of loneliness and their relationship with resilience and perceived control.

Unfortunately, the ability to identify the role of these characteristics in the causal pathway between social determinants and loneliness was limited by the single measure of these characteristics at baseline rather than over time. Future research could use a methodology like Structural Equation Modelling with longitudinal data points to develop a more detailed understanding of this relationship. Another limitation was our non-representative sample (e.g. with significantly fewer male than female respondents; under-representation of Aboriginal peoples), limiting the generalisability of findings. Our non-significant finding around a known association – Aboriginality and mental health – could be because our small sample limited power to detect this effect. Another very unusual finding – that men tended to be lonelier than women – could be attributed to self-selection bias. As is common in voluntary online surveys, men were much less likely to participate than women, but it's feasible that men feeling lonely during the pandemic are more likely to participate in a survey about mental health and connection than those feeling more socially connected.

In conclusion, social determinants (including quality housing, neighbourhoods and social relationships, among others) increased loneliness during the COVID-19 pandemic among Australians. While effects were not completely negated by an individual's perceived control and resilience, results suggest that these characteristics may be protective. As these are more readily modifiable than many social determinants, these results provide health practitioners guidance around identifying and helping clients vulnerable to loneliness.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/bec.2023.3.

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge the participants of the Alone Together study for their generosity in sharing their experiences over the COVID-19 period.

Funding

This work was supported by the University of Sydney's LifeSpan Research Network (seed funding grant) and the Henry Halloran Trust (Festival of Urbanism research grant).

Declaration of Interest

None.

Footnotes

The online version of this article has been updated since original publication. A notice detailing the change has also been published

References

Allan, NP, Volarov, M, Koscinski, B, Pizzonia, KL, Potter, K, Accorso, C, … Allan, DM (2021). Lonely, anxious, and uncertain: Critical risk factors for suicidal desire during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatry Research, 304, 114144. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2021.114144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Australian Bureau of Statistics (2018). Socio-economic Indexes For Areas (SEIFA) 2016. (2033.0.55.001). Canberra. Retrieved from: https://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@.nsf/mf/2033.0.55.001Google Scholar
Baker, E, Daniel, L, Bentley, R, Pawson, H, Stone, W, Rajagopalan, P, Hulse, K, Beer, A, London, K ... Randolph, B (2018) The Australian Housing Conditions: Technical Report. Adelaide, South Australia: The University of Adelaide – Healthy Cities Research.Google Scholar
Bobak, M, Pikhart, H, Hertzman, C, Rose, R and Marmot, M (1998). Socioeconomic factors, perceived control and self-reported health in Russia. A cross-sectional survey. Social Science & Medicine, 47, 269279. doi:10.1016/s0277-9536(98)00095-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bower, M, Buckle, C, Rugel, E, Donohoe-Bales, A, McGrath, L, Gournay, K, … Teesson, M (2021). ‘Trapped’, ‘anxious’ and ‘traumatised’: COVID-19 intensified the impact of housing inequality on Australians’ mental health. International Journal of Housing Policy, 132. doi:10.1080/19491247.2021.1940686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, M, Donohoe-Bales, A, Smout, S, Ngyuen, AQH, Boyle, J, Barrett, E and Teesson, M (2022). In their own words: An Australian community sample's priority concerns regarding mental health in the context of COVID-19. PLoS ONE, 17, e0268824. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0268824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brugha, TS and Cragg, D (1990) The List of Threatening Experiences: the reliability and validity of a brief life events questionnaire. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 82(1), 7781. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1990.tb01360.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bu, F, Steptoe, A and Fancourt, D (2020). Who is lonely in lockdown? Cross-cohort analyses of predictors of loneliness before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. Public Health, 186, 3134. doi:10.1016/j.puhe.2020.06.036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calear, AL, McCallum, S, Morse, AR, Banfield, M, Gulliver, A, Cherbuin, N, … Batterham, PJ (2022). Psychosocial impacts of home-schooling on parents and caregivers during the COVID-19 pandemic. BMC Public Health, 22, 119. doi:10.1186/s12889-022-12532-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell-Sills, L and Stein, MB (2007). Psychometric analysis and refinement of the Connor-davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC): Validation of a 10-item measure of resilience. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 20, 10191028. doi:10.1002/jts.20271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cruwys, T, Haslam, C, Rathbone, JA, Williams, E and Haslam, SA (2021). Groups 4 Health protects against unanticipated threats to mental health: Evaluating two interventions during COVID-19 lockdown among young people with a history of depression and loneliness. Journal of Affective Disorders, 295, 316322. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2021.08.029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Jong Gierveld, J and Van Tilburg, T (2010) The De Jong Gierveld short scales for emotional and social loneliness: tested on data from 7 countries in the UN generations and gender surveys. European Journal of Ageing, 7(2), 121130. doi: 10.1007/s10433-010-0144-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Draper, G and Dingle, GA (2021). “It's not the same”: A comparison of the psychological needs satisfied by musical group activities in face to face and virtual modes. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.646292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drury, J, Carter, H, Cocking, C, Ntontis, E, Tekin Guven, S and Amlôt, R (2019). Facilitating collective psychosocial resilience in the public in emergencies: Twelve recommendations based on the social identity approach. Frontiers in Public Health, 7. doi:10.3389/fpubh.2019.00141.Google ScholarPubMed
Eklund, M and Bäckström, M (2006). The role of perceived control for the perception of health by patients with persistent mental illness. Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 13, 249256. doi:10.1080/11038120600928823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fong, P, Cruwys, T, Robinson, SL, Haslam, SA, Haslam, C, Mance, PL and Fisher, CL (2021). Evidence that loneliness can be reduced by a whole-of-community intervention to increase neighbourhood identification. Social Science & Medicine, 277, 113909. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.113909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groarke, JM, Berry, E, Graham-Wisener, L, McKenna-Plumley, PE, McGlinchey, E and Armour, C (2020). Loneliness in the UK during the COVID-19 pandemic: Cross-sectional results from the COVID-19 Psychological Wellbeing Study. PLoS ONE, 15, e0239698. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0239698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hale, T, Angrist, N, Goldszmidt, R, Kira, B, Petherick, A, Phillips, T, … Helen, T (2021). A global panel database of pandemic policies (Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker). Nature Human Behaviour. doi:10.1038/s41562-021-01079-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haslam, C, Holme, A, Haslam, SA, Iyer, A, Jetten, J and Williams, WH (2008). Maintaining group memberships: Social identity continuity predicts well-being after stroke. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 18, 671691. doi:10.1080/09602010701643449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lachman, ME and Weaver, SL (1998). The sense of control as a moderator of social class differences in health and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 763773. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.74.3.763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons, A, Fletcher, G and Bariola, E (2016). Assessing the well-being benefits of belonging to resilient groups and communities: Development and testing of the Fletcher-Lyons Collective Resilience Scale (FLCRS). Group Dynamics: Theory Research and Practice, 20, 6577. doi:10.1037/gdn0000041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mao, W and Agyapong, VIO (2021). The role of social determinants in mental health and resilience after disasters: Implications for public health policy and practice. Frontiers in Public Health, 9. doi:10.3389/fpubh.2021.658528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mao, G, Drury, J, Fernandes-Jesus, M and Ntontis, E (2021). How participation in COVID-19 mutual aid groups affects subjective well-being and how political identity moderates these effects. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 21, 10821112. doi:10.1111/asap.12275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McQuaid, RJ, Cox, SML, Ogunlana, A and Jaworska, N (2021). The burden of loneliness: Implications of the social determinants of health during COVID-19. Psychiatry Research, 296, 113648. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Misamer, M, Signerski-Krieger, J, Bartels, C and Belz, M (2021). Internal locus of control and sense of coherence decrease during the COVID-19 pandemic: A survey of students and professionals in social work. Frontiers in Sociology, 6. doi:10.3389/fsoc.2021.705809.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patulny, R and Bower, M (2022). Beware the “loneliness gap”? Examining emerging inequalities and long-term risks of loneliness and isolation emerging from COVID-19. Australian Journal of Social Issues, 57, 562583. doi:10.1002/ajs4.223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perlman, D and Peplau, LA (1981). Towards a social psychology of loneliness. In Duck, S and Gilmour, R (Eds.), Personal relationships in disorder (pp. 3156). Cambridge: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Whitehead, M, Pennington, A, Orton, L, Nayak, S, Petticrew, M, Sowden, A and White, M (2016). How could differences in ‘control over destiny’ lead to socio-economic inequalities in health? A synthesis of theories and pathways in the living environment. Health Place, 39, 5161. doi:10.1016/j.healthplace.2016.02.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, AF, Russell, A and Powers, JR (2004). The sense of belonging to a neighbourhood: Can it be measured and is it related to health and well being in older women? Social Science & Medicine, 59, 26272637. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.05.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Potential pathways of perceived control, resilience, and loneliness during the COVID-19 pandemic, based on theories of Whitehead et al. (2016).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Fitted loneliness trajectories with 95% confidence intervals over the 1.5 years since survey was opened (three time points). The end of the national lockdown associated with the Coronavirus-19 Alpha outbreak coincided with June 2020, and varying lockdowns associated with the Coronavirus-19 Delta outbreak started in late June 2021.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Fitted loneliness trajectories over the 1.5 years since survey was opened (three time points), categorised by participants’ State/Territory of residence.

Supplementary material: File

Bower et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S3

Download Bower et al. supplementary material(File)
File 43 KB