Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T14:53:20.469Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Gene–environment interaction using polygenic scores: Do polygenic scores for psychopathology moderate predictions from environmental risk to behavior problems?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 September 2022

Robert Plomin*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
Agnieszka Gidziela
Affiliation:
School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK
Margherita Malanchini
Affiliation:
School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK
Sophie von Stumm
Affiliation:
Department of Education, University of York, York, UK
*
Corresponding author. Robert Plomin, email: robert.plomin@kcl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The DNA revolution has energized research on interactions between genes and environments (GxE) by creating indices of G (polygenic scores) that are powerful predictors of behavioral traits. Here, we test the extent to which polygenic scores for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and neuroticism moderate associations between parent reports of their children’s environmental risk (E) at ages 3 and 4 and teacher ratings of behavior problems (hyperactivity/inattention, conduct problems, emotional symptoms, and peer relationship problems) at ages 7, 9 and 12. The sampling frame included up to 6687 twins from the Twins Early Development Study. Our analyses focused on relative effect sizes of G, E and GxE in predicting behavior problems. G, E and GxE predicted up to 2%, 2% and 0.4%, respectively, of the variance in externalizing behavior problems (hyperactivity/inattention and conduct problems) across ages 7, 9 and 12, with no clear developmental trends. G and E predictions of emotional symptoms and peer relationship problems were weaker. A quarter (12 of 48) of our tests of GxE were nominally significant (p = .05). Increasing the predictive power of G and E would enhance the search for GxE.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Gene–environment interaction (GxE) refers to environmental effects that depend on genetic effects, that is, genetic sensitivity to the environment (Kendler & Eaves, Reference Kendler and Eaves1986). In relation to developmental psychopathology, GxE can be thought of as genetic moderation of the association between environmental factors, such as parenting, and children’s behavior problems.

GxE is important because it recognizes that one size does not fit all and offers the possibility of personalized tailoring of children’s environments based on their genetic propensities. Moreover, weak environmental effects in the population could have strong effects on children with particular genetic proclivities. GxE is the genetic extension of phenotypic research on differential reactivity to the environment (Garmezy & Rutter, Reference Garmezy and Rutter1983; Slagt et al., Reference Slagt, Dubas, Dekovic and van Aken2016; Wachs & Gruen, Reference Wachs and Gruen1982), such as research on the goodness-of-fit model of temperament (Thomas & Chess, Reference Thomas and Chess1977). GxE also has the virtue of moving beyond nature versus nurture to consider their interplay.

GxE is distinct conceptually from gene–environment correlation, which denotes experiences that are correlated with genetic propensities, that is, genetic exposure to environmental effects and genetic mediation of associations between environmental factors and psychopathology (Plomin et al., Reference Plomin, DeFries and Loehlin1977). In this paper, we define GxE as an interaction in the statistical sense that the effects of G and E are conditional on one another, independent of their main effects. There are other ways to construe the interplay between G and E (Rutter et al., Reference Rutter, Moffitt and Caspi2006).

GxE is not interactionism, the view that environmental and genetic threads in the fabric of behavior are so tightly interwoven that they cannot be disentangled, as implied by the often-repeated phrases “the organism is a product of its genes and its past environment” (Anastasi, Reference Anastasi1958, p. 197). Interactionism is a truism at the level of the individual, but it is not true for individual differences in a population. As discussed later, environmental effects on differences between children can exist without genetic effects, genetic effects can exist without environmental effects, and environmental and genetic effects can interact, that is, the effects of environments can depend on genes.

Although in this article we occasionally use words like effect and explanation instead of correlation and prediction, these words are only used in their statistical sense, not to imply causation. Our aim is to predict behavior problems from genes, environmental measures, and their interaction, without regard to causality (Plomin & von Stumm, Reference Plomin and von Stumm2021). That is, we use measures deemed to be measures of the environment, even though these measures show significant genetic influence, the issue of the “nature of nurture” (Plomin, Reference Plomin1994; Plomin & Bergeman, Reference Plomin and Bergeman1991). Similarly, we do not know the causal biological and environmental pathways through which inherited differences in DNA affect development (Pingault et al., Reference Pingault, O’Reilly, Schoeler, Ploubidis, Rijsdijk and Dudbridge2018). As we shall see, our focus on the relatively simple issue of prediction raises complicated issues even though we do not attempt to address issues of causality.

Three stages of GxE research

For the first century of genetic research on human behavior, quantitative genetic designs such as identical and fraternal twins and adoptive and nonadoptive relatives were used to estimate genetic and environmental components of observed (phenotypic) variance. These are anonymous components of variance in the sense that specific genetic and environmental factors are not identified. Although quantitative genetic theory recognizes that GxE can contribute to phenotypic variance, quantitative genetic designs in themselves can only provide indirect glimpses of GxE (Jinks & Fulker, Reference Jinks and Fulker1970; Plomin et al., Reference Plomin, DeFries and Loehlin1977).

We differentiate three stages in GxE research.

Stage 1

The first stage of GxE research incorporated measures of the environment into twin and adoption designs. In the twin design, environmental measures made it possible to ask whether heritability differs as a function of environment, a limited type of GxE. For example, it has been reported that greater parental warmth and directiveness is associated with greater heritability of conduct problems (Burt et al., Reference Burt, Klahr, Neale and Klump2013). For this and similar findings, replication is needed because the power demands for detecting significant differences in heritability are daunting (Hanscombe et al., Reference Hanscombe, Trzaskowski, Haworth, Davis, Dale and Plomin2012).

Environmental measures can be integrated in more powerful ways in the adoption design (Plomin et al., Reference Plomin, DeFries and Loehlin1977). For example, psychopathology of birth (biological) parents provides an estimate of their adopted-away children’s genetic risk free of postnatal environmental influence of the parents. Parenting of the adoptive parents of these children can be used to estimate environmental risk free of parental genetic confounds. GxE can then be assessed as the statistical interaction between these G and E indices.

GxE is illustrated in Figure 1 in a 2x2 framework in which parenting (low vs. high risk) and genetic propensity (low vs. high risk) predict children’s behavior problems. Main effects of parenting and genetics can occur without interaction (panel A). GxE can occur without main effects (panel D), which is called a disordinal (cross-over) interaction in that the environment has opposite effects depending on children’s genetic propensities. For example, differential susceptibility theory predicts a type of disordinal interaction in which genetically sensitive children are more affected by their environments, both for better and for worse (Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009; Pluess & Belsky, Reference Pluess and Belsky2010). However, a recent systematic analysis of differential susceptibility theory found little support for the theory (Cree et al., Reference Cree, Liu, Gueorguieva, Neiderhiser, Leve, Connell, Shaw, Natsuaki, Ganiban, Beekman, Smith and Reiss2021).

Figure 1. A 2x2 illustration of the prediction of children’s behavior problems from environments (low vs. high risk), from a genetic estimate (low vs. high risk), and from their interaction (GxE). See text for explanation.

The more likely type of GxE is an ordinal interaction, in which genetic effects magnify or diminish environmental effects. Panel B in Figure 1 shows an example in which high environmental risk disproportionately affects children who are at high genetic risk. In psychiatric genetics, this type of GxE is known as diathesis-stress (Gottesman, Reference Gottesman1991; Monroe & Simons, Reference Monroe and Simons1991; Paris, Reference Paris1999). That is, children at genetic risk for psychopathology, the diathesis, are especially sensitive to the effects of stressful environments. For behavioral problems, GxE also occurs in the opposite direction (panel C) in which high-risk environments overwhelm genetic propensities and low-risk environments allow genetic differences to be expressed.

A better use of the data is to analyze the variables in a continuous rather than dichotomous manner with multiple regression models in which main effects of G and E are modeled before testing GxE. The GxE interaction term can be a “dummy” variable created by the product of the main variables of G and E. In the Colorado Adoption Project (CAP), some ordinal GxE effects were found in infancy (Plomin & DeFries, Reference Plomin and DeFries1985) and early childhood (Plomin et al., Reference Plomin, DeFries and Fulker1988). As an example of an ordinal interaction shown in panel C, high parental control damped down genetic differences relevant to difficult temperament in infancy, whereas low parental control facilitated expression of genetic differences. Overall, however, only a chance number of significant GxE effects emerged from dozens of analyses of this type.

In contrast, a more recent adoption study, the Early Growth and Development Study (EGDS) (Leve et al., Reference Leve, Neiderhiser, Ganiban, Natsuaki, Shaw and Reiss2019), reported several GxE effects for behavior problems. Similar to CAP, the association between parental control and children’s behavior problems was moderated by genetic risk (Leve et al., Reference Leve, Harold, Ge, Neiderhiser, Shaw, Scaramella and Reiss2009). Significant GxE was also reported in EGDS for internalizing problems (Brooker et al., Reference Brooker, Neiderhiser, Ganiban, Leve, Shaw and Reiss2014) and externalizing problems (Lipscomb et al., Reference Lipscomb, Laurent, Neiderhiser, Shaw, Natsuaki, Reiss and Leve2014). Most GxE in EGDS was of the ordinal type shown in panel C in Figure 1 (Cree et al., Reference Cree, Liu, Gueorguieva, Neiderhiser, Leve, Connell, Shaw, Natsuaki, Ganiban, Beekman, Smith and Reiss2021).

Stage 2

The DNA revolution fueled the second and third stages of GxE research by making it possible to incorporate direct DNA measures of genetic propensities of children (Plomin, Reference Plomin2018). The huge benefit of these direct DNA measures is that they can be used in research on any sample of unrelated or related children, circumventing the need for special samples of twins and adoptees.

Until a decade ago, it was necessary to genotype one DNA variant at a time for each individual. The time and expense of this process meant that researchers could only genotype a few DNA variants. This led to the second stage of GxE research, which genotyped a few “candidate” genes, usually genes coding for neurotransmitters presumed to be related to psychopathology. Two of the most cited papers in behavioral genetics reported GxE of the diathesis-stress type (Panel B in Figure 1). The first study showed that childhood maltreatment was only associated with adult antisocial behavior for individuals who carried a specific version (allele) of the monoamine oxidase A gene that causes lower levels of the monoamine oxidase A, which is involved in metabolizing a broad range of neurotransmitters (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin, Craig, Taylor and Poulton2002). Similarly in the second study, the effect of stressful life events on depression was strongest for individuals who carried an allele that increased serotonin transport (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Sugden, Moffitt, Taylor, Craig, Harrington, McClay, Mill, Martin, Braithwaite and Poulton2003).

These early candidate-gene GxE findings triggered an explosion of research on GxE (Keers & Pluess, Reference Keers and Pluess2017), but many studies were underpowered to detect reasonable effect sizes, and failures to replicate accumulated (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Agrawal, Keller, Adkins, Aliev, Monroe, Hewitt, Kendler and Sher2015; Duncan & Keller, Reference Duncan and Keller2011). Some journals imposed a ban on GxE reports unless they included a replication study (Hewitt, Reference Hewitt2012).

Stepping back from GxE, the fundamental problem with a candidate-gene strategy is that we now know that single DNA variants hardly ever account for as much as one percent of the variance in the population. Most reported single-variant associations with behavioral traits are false (Chabris et al., Reference Chabris, Hebert, Benjamin, Beauchamp, Cesarini, van der Loos, Johannesson, Magnusson, Lichtenstein, Atwood, Freese, Hauser, Hauser, Christakis and Laibson2012).

Stage 3

About 15 years ago, a technological advance sparked the DNA revolution by making it possible to genotype hundreds of thousands of DNA variants quickly and inexpensively. The technology was a small DNA array that genotyped the most common type of DNA variant, a single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP). This tool, called a SNP chip, facilitated a strategy that is the opposite of the candidate-gene approach. Genome-wide association (GWA) is a hypothesis-free method that looks for associations using millions of variants across the entire genome genotyped for each individual on a SNP chip. GWA research has shown that the heritability of complex traits and common disorders is caused by thousands of inherited DNA differences, each with miniscule effects. Success in GWA research came only after sample sizes in the tens and hundreds of thousands were achieved to reach the power needed to detect these tiny effects (Plomin, Reference Plomin2018).

Thousands of GWA with a particular trait can be aggregated in a genetic index of that trait for each individual, called a polygenic score. Polygenic scores have many labels, but we prefer genome-wide polygenic scores (GPS) to distinguish polygenic scores that are derived from all SNPs, usually tens of thousands, throughout the genome that are associated with the trait from polygenic scores that are based on only a few selected SNPs (Plomin & von Stumm, Reference Plomin and von Stumm2018). Although GWA studies require huge samples, associations from these GWA studies can be used to create polygenic scores for any sample (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Mak and O’Reilly2020). In contrast to candidate genes, polygenic scores can predict up to six percent of the variance of behavior problems in childhood (Gidziela et al., Reference Gidziela, Rimfeld, Malanchini, Allegrini, McMillan, Selzam, Ronald, Viding, Stumm, Eley and Plomin2021).

Polygenic scores mark the third stage of GxE research. Lessons can be learned from candidate-gene GxE research to avoid the pitfalls of underpowered studies and questionable research practices (Domingue et al., Reference Domingue, Trejo, Armstrong-Carter and Tucker-Drob2020). It has been estimated that large GxE effects, whether indexed by a single gene or a polygenic score, account for about 1% of the variance, which requires sample sizes of about 600 to reach 80 percent power to detect them as calculated by Duncan and Keller (Reference Duncan and Keller2011). For moderate GxE effects accounting for 0.1 percent of the variance, sample sizes in the tens of thousands are needed to reach 80 percent power.

Most behavioral GxE research using polygenic scores has involved adult psychiatric disorders, although no solidly replicated GxE findings have emerged as yet (e.g., Arnau-Soler et al., Reference Arnau-Soler, Macdonald-Dunlop, Adams, Clarke, MacIntyre, Milburn, Navrady, Hayward, McIntosh, Thomson, Wray, Ripke, Mattheisen, Trzaskowski, Byrne, Abdellaoui, Agerbo, Air, Andlauer and Sullivan2019; Bogdan et al., Reference Bogdan, Baranger, Agrawal, Widiger and Cannon2018; Colodro-Conde et al., Reference Colodro-Conde, Couvy-Duchesne, Zhu, Coventry, Byrne, Gordon, Wright, Montgomery, Madden, Ripke, Eaves, Heath, Wray, Medland and Martin2018; Kandaswamy et al., Reference Kandaswamy, Allegrini, Nancarrow, Cave, Plomin and von Stumm2021; Robinson & Bergen, Reference Robinson and Bergen2021). Reports of GxE in developmental psychopathology are beginning to emerge. Two early studies using a polygenic score for major depressive disorder reported GxE for childhood adversity predicting adult depression (Mullins et al., Reference Mullins, Power, Fisher, Hanscombe, Euesden, Iniesta, Levinson, Weissman, Potash, Shi, Uher, Cohen-Woods, Rivera, Jones, Jones, Craddock, Owen, Korszun, Craig and Lewis2016; Peyrot et al., Reference Peyrot, Milaneschi, Abdellaoui, Sullivan, Hottenga, Boomsma and Penninx2014), but a later study could not replicate the finding (Peyrot et al., Reference Peyrot, Van der Auwera, Milaneschi, Dolan, Madden, Sullivan, Strohmaier, Ripke, Rietschel, Nivard, Mullins, Montgomery, Henders, Heat, Fisher, Dunn, Byrne, Air and Penninx2018). In another study, no GxE was found for childhood adversity predicting adult psychotic disorders (Trotta et al., Reference Trotta, Iyegbe, Forti, Sham, Campbell, Cherny, Mondelli, Aitchison, Murray, Vassos and Fisher2016).

Recent reports of significant GxE in developmental psychopathology in which G was indexed by polygenic scores include critical parenting and depression (Nelemans et al., Reference Nelemans, Boks, Lin, Oldehinkel, van Lier, Branje and Meeus2021), family and neighborhood stress on conduct problems (Bares et al., Reference Bares, Chartier, Karriker-Jaffe, Aliev, Mustanski and Dick2020), and childhood adversity and emotion dysregulation and psychosis proneness (Pries et al., Reference Pries, Klingenberg, Menne-Lothmann, Decoster, van Winkel, Collip, Delespaul, De Hert, Derom, Thiery, Jacobs, Wichers, Cinar, Lin, Luykx, Rutten, van Os and Guloksuz2020). However, at least as many studies reported no significant GxE, including research on peer victimization and resilience (Armitage et al., Reference Armitage, Wang, Davis and Haworth2021), maltreatment and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms (He & Li, Reference He and Li2021), and parenting and adolescent externalizing problems (Ksinan et al., Reference Ksinan, Smith, Barr and Vazsonyi2021).

The present study

In this paper, we test GxE for behavior problems in childhood using data from up to 6687 twins from the Twins Early Development Study (Rimfeld et al., Reference Rimfeld, Malanchini, Spargo, Spickernell, Selzam, McMillan, Dale, Eley and Plomin2019). To avoid associations driven by rater bias or contemporaneous events, we focused on parent reports of their children’s environments at 3 and 4 years as they predict teacher ratings of behavior problems at 7, 9 and 12 years. Based on previous analyses (Gidziela et al., Reference Gidziela, Rimfeld, Malanchini, Allegrini, McMillan, Selzam, Ronald, Viding, Stumm, Eley and Plomin2021), we selected polygenic scores for ADHD and neuroticism that were found to be most predictive of teacher ratings of childhood behavior problems. We also selected environmental measures at ages 3 and 4 known to predict teacher ratings of behavior problems (Gidziela et al., Reference Gidziela, Malanchini, Rimfeld, McMillan, Ronald, Viding, Asbury, Eley, von Stumm and Plomin2022). Our primary aim was to compare effect sizes for G, E and GxE in predicting behavior problems as rated by teachers. A secondary aim was to explore G, E and GxE effects on behavior problems developmentally across childhood.

Method

Participants

Our sample consists of twins born in England and Wales between 1994 and 1996 who have participated in the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS; Rimfeld et al., Reference Rimfeld, Malanchini, Spargo, Spickernell, Selzam, McMillan, Dale, Eley and Plomin2019). TEDS is a longitudinal study designed to investigate the development of behavior, cognition and communication, as well as developmental problems. The original TEDS sample involved over 13,000 twin pairs, from whom data collection took place when the twins were aged 2, 3, 4, 7, 8, 9, 10, 12, 14, 16, 18 and 21. The sample of TEDS twins is representative of the UK population in terms of ethnicity and socio-economic status (SES) (Rimfeld et al., Reference Rimfeld, Malanchini, Spargo, Spickernell, Selzam, McMillan, Dale, Eley and Plomin2019).

In addition to phenotypic data, a subsample of 10,346 TEDS twins (i.e., one twin per pair for 3706 twin pairs and 3320 pairs of DZ twins) were genotyped using one of two genotyping platforms (AffymetrixGeneChip 6.0 and Illumina HumanOmniExpressExome-8v1.2) in two waves, 5 years apart. A detailed genotyping protocol is available (Selzam et al., Reference Selzam, McAdams, Coleman, Carnell, O’Reilly, Plomin and Llewellyn2018).

The present analyses included a subsample of TEDS twins with complete genotype data along with parent reports of environmental risk and discipline in preschool (ages 3 and 4) and teacher ratings of behavior problems in childhood and early adolescence (ages 7, 9 and 12), resulting in up to 6687 individuals included in our analyses. The sample included only one member of a twin pair for all monozygotic twins and for half of the dizygotic twin pairs (4561 unrelated twins). We also included 2126 dizygotic co-twins to increase the power of our analyses; the inclusion of co-twins slightly affects statistical significance (which were not adjusted for lack of independence) but does not affect estimates of effect size, which is the focus of our analyses.

Measures

In this section, we outline our measures of environmental risk, behavior problems, and polygenic scores. Details about the measures are available in the TEDS data dictionary (https://www.teds.ac.uk/datadictionary/home.htm). We selected environmental measures in early childhood and polygenic scores that we knew from previous analyses predicted behavior problems in the school years (Gidziela et al., Reference Gidziela, Rimfeld, Malanchini, Allegrini, McMillan, Selzam, Ronald, Viding, Stumm, Eley and Plomin2021; Gidziela et al., Reference Gidziela, Malanchini, Rimfeld, McMillan, Ronald, Viding, Asbury, Eley, von Stumm and Plomin2022) in order to increase the likelihood of finding ordinal GxE (Duncan & Keller, Reference Duncan and Keller2011).

Environmental measures

Parent-reported environmental measures in early childhood were selected based on their prediction of teacher-rated behavior problems in childhood, following a procedure described in detail elsewhere (Gidziela et al., Reference Gidziela, Malanchini, Rimfeld, McMillan, Ronald, Viding, Asbury, Eley, von Stumm and Plomin2022). Using two criteria, this procedure reduced the number of environmental items from several hundred to eight items using two criteria. The first criterion was a phenotypic correlation greater than 0.20 between the environmental measure and at least one of the behavior problem measures. The second criterion excluded highly correlated environmental measures using a penalized elastic net regularization with training and test iterations.

We began with two environmental risk measures at age 3 and at age 4: a family-general measure based on variables that are the same for co-twins and a twin-specific measure based on variables that differentiated between co-twins. The family-general environmental risk composite was computed as a standardized mean of five standardized scores: family SES based on both parents’ education and occupation and mothers’ age at first contact, prenatal and perinatal medical risk, household chaos (the Confusion, Hubbub and Order Scale; Matheny et al., Reference Matheny, Wachs, Ludwig and Phillips1995), maternal postnatal depression (the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (Cox et al., Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987), and life events such as changes to marital status, new siblings, mother’s pregnancy, job changes and serious illness/accident).

The twin-specific environmental risk measure was computed as a standardized mean of the same variables that were included in the family-general environmental risk measure, with the addition of standardized scores of twin medical risk factors (4 items), parental discipline scale (6 items) and a parental feelings scale (7 items) (Deater-Deckard et al., Reference Deater-Deckard, Dodge, Bates and Pettit1998). At each age we also included a separate item about smacking and shouting because of its predictiveness (Gidziela et al., Reference Gidziela, Malanchini, Rimfeld, McMillan, Ronald, Viding, Asbury, Eley, von Stumm and Plomin2022).

Because the environmental measures were correlated with one another across age 3 and 4 (see Supplementary Figure 1), we used exploratory factor analysis (EFA) as a data reduction technique (see Supplementary Note 1). EFA analysis yielded two clear factors (see Supplementary Figure 2). The first factor was a general environmental risk factor that included both twin-specific and family-general environmental risk composites at ages 3 and 4, as well as SES (see Supplementary Figure 3). The second factor was a discipline factor including parental discipline composites and smacking/shouting items at ages 3 and 4.

The environmental risk and discipline factors correlated moderately (0.40). Using the two-factor structure suggested by the EFA, we created factor scores derived from a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) because CFA can account for data missingness using Full Information Maximum Likelihood. Although we were simply using CFA as a data reduction technique to handle data missingness, we achieved semi-independence of the EFA and CFA by conducting EFA on one randomly selected member of each twin pair and conducting CFA on the other twin (see Supplementary Note 2). Results of the CFA are illustrated in Supplementary Figure 4 and model fit indices presented in Supplementary Table 1. The two factors, which we refer to as environmental risk and discipline, were used in our subsequent analyses.

Behavior problems

We assessed teacher ratings of hyperactivity/inattention, conduct problems, emotional symptoms, and peer relationship problems at ages 7, 9 and 12 using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, Reference Goodman1997). Teacher ratings of behavior problems were obtained via mail and included a total of 20 items at each age, that is, five items for each of the four scales. The items were rated on a three-point Likert scale (certainly true; sometimes true; not true), with items scored in the direction of greater problems or reversed where necessary so that higher scale scores indicated more behavior problems.

Analysis

Environmental and behavior problems variables were residualized for the effects of age and sex. Both polygenic scores were corrected for the effects of genotyping chip and 10 principal components of ancestry prior to downstream analyses. All regression analyses were conducted using stats for R (R Core Team, 2021).

Main effects of polygenic scores and environment on behavior problems

The effects of genotype and environment on teacher-rated behavior problems at ages 7, 9 and 12 were examined using regression analysis, with the GPS (ADHD or neuroticism) and environment (environmental risk or discipline) as predictors. Effects of each of the four predictors were examined separately to estimate the variance they predicted in teacher-rated hyperactivity/inattention, conduct problems, emotional symptoms, and peer relationship problems at ages 7, 9 and 12. Although these single-variable regressions are the same as simple correlations, we present the results in the regression model to facilitate comparisons with the multiple regressions we used to test for GxE.

Interaction between polygenic scores and environment

To estimate the proportion of variance explained by the interaction between genotype and environment (GxE), we used multiple regression analysis, testing for GxE after controlling for the joint effects of GPS and environmental factors (i.e., G+E). We first estimated G+E prediction of each of the four teacher-rated behavior problems at ages 7, 9 and 12 from the GPS (ADHD or neuroticism) and environmental factors (environmental risk or discipline). Subsequently we ran another regression model with the GPS, environmental factor, and the interaction between them as predictors. To estimate the proportion of variance in behavior problems predicted by GxE independent of G+E, we subtracted the variance explained (R 2) by the G+E model from the R 2 of the GxE model, which is a hierarchical multiple regression analysis.

Results

Descriptive statistics, including means, standard deviations and sample sizes for the environmental measures and behavior problems are presented in Supplementary Table 2. The full correlation matrix between the environmental measures, GPS, and behavior problems across the three ages is shown in Supplementary Figure 1.

Main effects of polygenic scores and environment on behavior problems

Figure 2 summarizes results for the prediction of behavior problems from the environmental factors and GPS, with details of the regression analyses in Supplementary Table 3. The strongest GPS prediction was for the ADHD GPS predicting hyperactivity/inattention at age 7, accounting for 2.0% of the variance. Results were similar for teacher ratings of behavior problems at ages 7, 9 and 12. The strongest E prediction was for the environmental risk factor predicting hyperactivity/inattention at age 9, which accounted for 2.7% of the variance. The environmental risk factor accounted on average for 2.3% of the variance in hyperactivity/inattention across ages and 2.1% of the variance in conduct problems, but less than 1% for emotional symptoms and peer relationship problems. The discipline factor yielded a similar degree of prediction, accounting for 2.1% variance in hyperactivity/inattention across ages, 1.5% in conduct problems, 0.2% for emotional symptoms and 0.4% for peer relationship problems.

Figure 2. Variance explained by G (ADHD and neuroticism GPS) and E (environmental risk and discipline factors) predicting teacher-rated hyperactivity/inattention (a), conduct problems (b), emotional symptoms (c) and peer relationship problems (d) at ages 7, 9 and 12.

The environmental and GPS predictions were higher for externalizing problems (hyperactivity/inattention and conduct problems) than for internalizing problems (emotional symptoms and peer relationship problems). For GPS, prediction from the ADHD GPS was greater than from the neuroticism GPS: 1.8% versus 0.1% for hyperactivity/inattention, 1.2% versus 0.1% for conduct problems, and 0.2% versus 0.1% for peer relationship problems, averaged across the three ages. The one exception was that the neuroticism GPS explained slightly more variance in emotional symptoms than the ADHD GPS (0.4% vs. 0.2% across ages). No clear developmental trends emerged across childhood.

Interactions between polygenic scores and environment

Figure 3 presents the proportion of variance in behavior problems explained jointly by the GPS and environmental factors (G+E) and the variance explained by the interaction between them (GxE) after controlling for G+E. Details of the multiple regression results are presented in Supplementary Table 5.

Figure 3. G+E and GxE prediction of teacher-rated hyperactivity/inattention (a), conduct problems (b), emotional symptoms (c) and peer relationship problems (d) at ages 7, 9 and 12.

The joint G+E prediction in Figure 3 reflects the individual G and E predictions shown in Figure 2. G and E together predict less variance than the sum of variance explained by G and E separately (Figure 2) because G and E are correlated (see Supplementary Figure 1). The R 2 estimate of the joint effect of G and E discounts the covariance between G and E.

The strongest G+E predictions are for hyperactivity/inattention. For the ADHD GPS and environmental risk factor, G and E together predict 3.7% of the variance in hyperactivity/inattention on average across the three ages. Similar results were obtained for discipline (3.6%). For the other three Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire scales, the ADHD GPS and the environmental risk factor jointly predicted 3% of the variance in conduct problems and 0.8% for both emotional symptoms and peer relationship problems. Again, the neuroticism GPS was less predictive. Neuroticism GPS and environmental risk jointly predicted 2.3% of the variance for hyperactivity/inattention, 2.1% for conduct problems, 0.9% for emotional symptoms, and 0.7% for peer relationship problems. Results for the discipline factor were similar: 2.1% for hyperactivity/inattention, 1.6% for conduct problems, 0.5% for emotional symptoms and 0.4% for peer relationship problems. No developmental trends were apparent.

GxE results are also shown in Figure 3. The most striking aspect of Figure 3 is how little variance is predicted by GxE beyond the joint effects of G and E. The proportions of variance explained by G+E as well as GxE are listed in Supplementary Table 4. Tests of significance from analysis of variance are presented in Supplementary Table 5. Overall, 12 of the 48 tests of GxE were nominally significant (p < .05). Eight of these significant GxE tests involved the ADHD GPS. However, the significant GxE that predicted the most variance, the ADHD GPS and the discipline factor predicting conduct problems at age 12, only accounted for 0.4% of the variance. Because the variance explained by GxE is less than our power to detect GxE (0.5%), caution is warranted until these interactions are replicated. No developmental trends emerged.

Figure 4 plots these 12 significant GxE interactions in 2x2 analyses in which children were selected in the +/- 1 SD quadrants of G and E. Analysis of variance results for these 2x2 analyses are included in Supplementary Table 6.

Figure 4. Significant GxE interactions in 2x2 analyses of extreme (+/- 1 SD) ADHD GPS, neuroticism GPS and environmental factor scores. Solid line = +1 SD for GPS; dashed line = -1 SD for GPS.

The 2x2 plots generally show main effects of G and E but only hint at GxE. Eight of the 12 main effects for G were significant and, in each case, the high G group had more behavioral problems than the low G group. Ten of the 12 main effects for E were significant and there were more behavioral problems in the high E group than the low E group. However, for GxE, only three of the 12 analyses yielded significant GxE, even though these 12 comparisons were selected because they yielded significant GxE in the continuous analyses (Supplementary Table 5). In these three cases, as well as most of the other GxE, the GxE were ordinal and of the diathesis-stress type. That is, stressful environments exacerbated genetic risk for behavior problems. For example, in the significant GxE in the upper left-hand corner of Figure 4, high parental discipline at ages 3 and 4 predicted more hyperactivity/inattention problems at age 9 primarily for children with high GPS scores for hyperactivity.

Discussion

To illustrate issues involved in GxE analysis, we explored GxE in the prediction of teacher ratings of behavior problems at ages 7, 9 and 12 from parent reports of children’s environments at ages 3 and 4. We found significant but modest GxE with no developmental trends across childhood. Although one quarter (12 of 48) of our GxE tests were significant, the largest GxE effect accounted for only 0.4% of the variance. This GxE was an example of ordinal diathesis-stress interaction in that the difference in conduct problems between children with high and low ADHD GPS scores at age 12 was greater for children whose parents were high in discipline, as shown in Figure 4. That is, children with high ADHD GPS scores showed disproportionately more conduct problems when their parents were disciplinarians.

The average effect size of all 12 significant GxE is 0.2%, which represents 50% power for a nominal p value of 0.05. Because our sample size provides substantial power to detect “large” GxE effects that account for 1% of the variance (Duncan & Keller, Reference Duncan and Keller2011), we can safely exclude the possibility of GxE accounting for more than 1% of the variance for these measures of E, G and behavior problems at these ages in our sample. Smaller GxE effects could be useful in understanding causal pathways between G and E (Götz et al., Reference Götz, Gosling and Rentfrow2021), but they are of limited utility from the perspective of prediction. The goal of prediction is to account for as much variance as possible without regard for explanation; the goal of explanation is to deduce causality, usually without regard for prediction (Shmueli, Reference Shmueli2010; Yarkoni & Westfall, Reference Yarkoni and Westfall2017). Although causal explanation will remain the long-term goal for psychology, prediction has immediate practical utility for identifying individuals at risk and is a necessary first step towards explanation (Plomin & von Stumm, Reference Plomin and von Stumm2021). One example is the success of genomic research after candidate-gene analyses were supplanted by hypothesis-free GWA analyses. Polygenic scores flourished because they are constructed to maximize the prediction of a target trait. Another example for the value of prediction without explanation is artificial intelligence in which machine learning explicitly eschews explanation.

When G and E are combined to predict behavior problems (Allegrini et al., Reference Allegrini, Karhunen, Coleman, Selzam, Rimfeld, von Stumm, Pingault and Plomin2020), they predict less variance than the sum of their individual predictions because G and E indices correlate. For example, the ADHD GPS and discipline factor correlate 0.14 and 0.16, respectively, with teacher ratings of hyperactivity/inattention at age 7 (see Supplementary Figure 1). The sum of the square of these correlations (0.05) exceeds the R 2 from the multiple regression (0.04) because R 2 adjusts for the correlated effect of G and E on the trait. GE correlation is an important topic beyond the scope of this article (Krapohl et al., Reference Krapohl, Hannigan, Pingault, Patel, Kadeva, Curtis, Breen, Newhouse, Eley, O’Reilly and Plomin2017); its relevance to GxE is that it is necessary to control for the correlation between G and E in GxE analyses (Keller, Reference Keller2014), which is what the multiple regression does.

The potential for identifying ordinal GxE depends on the effect sizes of G and E (Duncan & Keller, Reference Duncan and Keller2011). For this reason, a larger issue that emerges from our results is that the effect sizes of our G and E predictors of behavior problems are modest. The strongest E predictor was the environmental risk factor predicting hyperactivity/inattention at age 9, although this prediction accounted for only 2.7% of the variance. The strongest G predictor, the ADHD polygenic score predicting hyperactivity/inattention at age 7, accounted for 2.0% of the variance.

Much recent research has been directed towards increasing the effect size of polygenic score predictions. Different methods for constructing polygenic scores have not had much effect (Allegrini et al., Reference Allegrini, Selzam, Rimfeld, von Stumm, Pingault and Plomin2019; Pain et al., Reference Pain, Glanville, Hagenaars, Selzam, Fürtjes, Gaspar, Coleman, Rimfeld, Breen, Plomin, Folkersen and Lewis2020), but using multiple polygenic scores improves prediction of cognitive traits (Allegrini et al., Reference Allegrini, Karhunen, Coleman, Selzam, Rimfeld, von Stumm, Pingault and Plomin2020; Grotzinger et al., Reference Grotzinger, Rhemtulla, de Vlaming, Ritchie, Mallard, Hill, Ip, Marioni, McIntosh, Deary, Koellinger, Harden, Nivard and Tucker-Drob2019; Krapohl et al., Reference Krapohl, Patel, Newhouse, Curtis, von Stumm, Dale, Zabaneh, Breen, O’Reilly and Plomin2018) and behavior problems (Gidziela et al., Reference Gidziela, Rimfeld, Malanchini, Allegrini, McMillan, Selzam, Ronald, Viding, Stumm, Eley and Plomin2021). By far the most effective boost to predictive power has come from increasing the sample sizes of GWA studies so that they are powered to detect small effect sizes of individual DNA variants. For example, the most predictive polygenic score in the behavioral sciences is based on GWA analyses of educational attainment (years of education). The predictive power of this polygenic score in independent samples not included in the GWA study increased from 2% with a GWA sample of 125,00 (Rietveld et al., Reference Rietveld, Medland, Derringer, Yang, Esko, Martin, Westra, Shakhbazov, Abdellaoui, Agrawal, Albrecht, Alizadeh, Amin, Barnard, Baumeister, Benke, Bielak, Boatman, Boyle and Koellinger2013) to 3% with a sample size of 294,000 (Okbay et al., Reference Okbay, Beauchamp, Fontana, Lee, Pers, Rietveld, Turley, Chen, Emilsson, Meddens, Oskarsson, Pickrell, Thom, Timshel, de Vlaming, Abdellaoui, Ahluwalia, Bacelis, Baumbach and Benjamin2016) to more than 10% with a sample size of 1.1 million (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Wedow, Okbay, Kong, Maghzian, Zacher, Nguyen-Viet, Bowers, Sidorenko, Karlsson Linnér, Fontana, Kundu, Lee, Li, Li, Royer, Timshel, Walters, Willoughby and Cesarini2018) to about 14% with a sample size of 3 million (Okbay et al., Reference Okbay, Wu, Wang, Jayashankar, Bennett, Nehzati, Sidorenko, Kweon, Goldman, Gjorgjieva, Jiang, Hicks, Tian, Hinds, Ahlskog, Magnusson, Oskarsson, Hayward, Campbell and Young2022).

GWA samples for behavior problems are a long way from these sizes. At present, the most powerful polygenic score predictor of behavior problems is derived from a GWA analysis of 20,000 diagnosed ADHD cases and 35,000 controls (Demontis et al., Reference Demontis, Walters, Martin, Mattheisen, Als, Agerbo, Baldursson, Belliveau, Bybjerg-Grauholm, Bækvad-Hansen, Cerrato, Chambert, Churchhouse, Dumont, Eriksson, Gandal, Goldstein, Grasby, Grove and Neale2019). This ADHD polygenic score, used in the current study, predicted 5.5% of the liability variance (Nagelkerke’s R 2 for analysis of cases versus controls) in an independent sample (Demontis et al., Reference Demontis, Walters, Martin, Mattheisen, Als, Agerbo, Baldursson, Belliveau, Bybjerg-Grauholm, Bækvad-Hansen, Cerrato, Chambert, Churchhouse, Dumont, Eriksson, Gandal, Goldstein, Grasby, Grove and Neale2019). On the other hand, the polygenic score for neuroticism that was also used in our study was derived from a GWA analysis with a sample size of nearly 330,000 adults but predicted only 2.8% of the variance of self-reported neuroticism in an independent sample (Luciano et al., Reference Luciano, Hagenaars, Davies, Hill, Clarke, Shirali, Harris, Marioni, Liewald, Fawns-Ritchie, Adams, Howard, Lewis, Gale, McIntosh and Deary2018). These earlier findings on out-of-sample prediction of polygenic scores align with our results that the ADHD polygenic score predicted hyperactivity/inattention (1.8%) better than the neuroticism polygenic score predicted emotional problems (0.4%).

For environmental measures, less effort has been directed towards increasing – or even noticing – predictive power. Much environmental research, and psychological research in general, still focuses on statistical significance rather than effect size (Funder & Ozer, Reference Funder and Ozer2019). Despite a century of research on environmental predictors of psychopathology, surprisingly little can be concluded about prediction effect sizes. For example, even for the most well studied disorders of schizophrenia and especially bipolar disorder, it is not possible to say how much variance in liability can be predicted by environmental factors (Robinson & Bergen, Reference Robinson and Bergen2021). Studies that have examined the combined prediction of multiple environmental risk factors on the development of behavioral problems point to their weak effect size. For example, a study across four large European cohorts showed that perinatal factors (family SES, maternal drinking and smoking during pregnancy, maternal stress, breast-feeding, and gestational age) accounted for less than 1.5% of the variance in aggressive behavior in childhood and adolescence (Malanchini et al., Reference Malanchini, Rimfeld, Allegrini, Ritchie and Plomin2020).

The fundamental problem for prediction is that environmental research has nothing remotely comparable to the genetic code in DNA sequence or the technological advance of SNP chips that assess millions of DNA variants quickly, accurately, and inexpensively. A comparison between the genome and what could be called the environome is instructive (von Stumm & d’Apice, Reference von Stumm and d’Apice2022). In the genome, millions of inherited differences in DNA sequence have been identified and their tiny individual associations with a trait can be summed to create polygenic scores. Another huge advantage of polygenic scores as predictors is that they do not change after conception because SNPs are inherited DNA variants. This is why a polygenic score derived from a GWA of adult psychopathology can be usefully applied to investigate psychopathology in childhood, as in our study. In contrast, for the environome, there is no fundamental unit of transmission and the environome changes throughout development.

Pushing the analogy with DNA further, much environmental research is still anchored in the “candidate environment” stage, with researchers looking for a few reasonable factors, like parenting, that are assumed to have major effects on children’s outcomes. But what if, like genetics, environmental influences involve thousands of miniscule effects? It has been argued that it is unrealistic to expect large effects for psychological phenomena (Götz et al., Reference Götz, Gosling and Rentfrow2021). One way to increase effect size is to aggregate small effects (Funder & Ozer, Reference Funder and Ozer2019), which was key to the success of polygenic scores. For example, environmental risks across events and across time have long been used as cumulative risk indices in developmental research (Rutter, Reference Rutter1981; Widaman, Reference Widaman2021). But what environmental variables should be included such composites? As a parallel to the SNP chip, it has been suggested that digital technologies could be used to collect naturalistic observations in real time, “capturing the environome across levels, dimensions, and time in unprecedented depth and detail” (von Stumm & d’Apice, Reference von Stumm and d’Apice2022, p. 6).

More fundamental issues about assessing the environment go beyond the scope of this paper. For instance, what is the environment and what is its relationship to experience? Much discussion about environmental assessment assumes that the environment is “out there,” passively imposed on children. This view suggests that what might be most important is children’s active construction of their experience (Plomin, Reference Plomin1994), but this is not usually assessed by current environmental measures. Supporting this perspective is a recent study showing that subjective rather than objective maltreatment in childhood predicted psychopathology later in life (Danese & Widom, Reference Danese and Widom2020).

Another issue is that genetic research has shown that environmental influences on behavior problems are not shared by children growing up in the same family, called nonshared environment (Plomin, Reference Plomin2011). This important clue about the salient environmental influences has not yet been used in the construction of current measures of the environment. In the present study, we took a step in this direction by considering environmental measures that were to some extent specific to each twin separately from measures that are the same for all children in the family, such as SES. However, using available environmental measures that can differ between children in a family is a long way from environmental measures designed to maximize experiential differences between siblings (Daniels & Plomin, Reference Daniels and Plomin1985).

Children’s active construction of their experience might also explain why environmental influences on behavior problems are experienced differently by children growing up in the same family as events are filtered through their unique constellation of perceptions, cognitions, and emotions. Active construction of experience could result in nonshared environments that are largely idiosyncratic and stochastic (Plomin, Reference Plomin2018).

Ultimately what will be needed are large-scale collaborations, along the lines of GWA consortia, that can reach the statistical power needed to detect small effects of G, E and their interplay. GWA consortia could be a practical way to begin by incorporating environmental measures in large biobanks that already have GWA genotype data, especially new longitudinal cohorts that could track developmental changes in the environome (von Stumm & d’Apice, Reference von Stumm and d’Apice2022). The difficult trade-off will be between quality and depth of environmental assessments versus the need for very large samples.

Increasing the power of G and E to predict behavior problems is valuable in itself for the field of developmental psychopathology. It might also eventually lead to the identification of robust GxE.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579422000931

Funding statement

We gratefully acknowledge the ongoing contribution of the participants in the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) and their families. TEDS has been supported by a program grant to R.P. from the UK Medical Research Council (MR/M021475/1 and previously G0901245), with additional support from the US National Institutes of Health (AG046938) and the European Commission (602768; 295366). This work was supported by a Nuffield Foundation award (EDO/44110) to S.v.S, who is a recipient of a British Academy Mid-Career Fellowship (MF21\210017).

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

Allegrini, A. G., Karhunen, V., Coleman, J. R. I., Selzam, S., Rimfeld, K., von Stumm, S., Pingault, J.-B., & Plomin, R. (2020). Multivariable G-E interplay in the prediction of educational achievement. PLOS Genetics, 16, e1009153. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1009153 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allegrini, A. G., Selzam, S., Rimfeld, K., von Stumm, S., Pingault, J. B., & Plomin, R. (2019). Genomic prediction of cognitive traits in childhood and adolescence. Molecular Psychiatry, 24, 819827. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0394-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anastasi, A. (1958). Heredity, environment, and the question ‘how?’ Psychological Review, 65, 197208. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0044895 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Armitage, J. M., Wang, R. A. H., Davis, O. S. P., & Haworth, C. M. A. (2021). A polygenic approach to understanding resilience to peer victimisation. Behavior Genetics, 52, 112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-021-10085-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnau-Soler, A., Macdonald-Dunlop, E., Adams, M. J., Clarke, T.-K., MacIntyre, D. J., Milburn, K., Navrady, L., Hayward, C., McIntosh, A. M., Thomson, P. A., Wray, N. R., Ripke, S., Mattheisen, M., Trzaskowski, M., Byrne, E. M., Abdellaoui, A., Agerbo, E., Air, T. M., Andlauer, T. F. M., & Sullivan, P. F. (2019). Genome-wide by environment interaction studies of depressive symptoms and psychosocial stress in UK Biobank and generation Scotland. Translational Psychiatry, 9, 14. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-018-0360-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bares, C. B., Chartier, K. G., Karriker-Jaffe, K. J., Aliev, F., Mustanski, B., & Dick, D. (2020). Exploring how family and neighborhood stressors influence genetic risk for adolescent conduct problems and alcohol use. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 49, 13651378. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-019-01098-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 885908. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017376 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogdan, R., Baranger, D. A. A., & Agrawal, A. (2018). Polygenic risk scores in clinical psychology: bridging genomic risk to individual differences. In Widiger, T. & Cannon, T. D. (Eds.), Annual review of clinical psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 119157). Annual Reviews. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050817084847 Google Scholar
Brooker, R. J., Neiderhiser, J. M., Ganiban, J. M., Leve, L. D., Shaw, D. S., & Reiss, D. (2014). Birth and adoptive parent anxiety symptoms moderate the link between infant attention control and internalizing problems in toddlerhood. Development and Psychopathology, 26, 347359. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941300103X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, S. A., Klahr, A. M., Neale, M. C., & Klump, K. L. (2013). Maternal warmth and directiveness jointly moderate the etiology of childhood conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 54, 10301037. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12095 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., Taylor, A., & Poulton, R. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science (New York, N.Y.), 297, 851854. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1072290 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington, H., McClay, J., Mill, J., Martin, J., Braithwaite, A., & Poulton, R. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science (New York, N.Y.), 301, 386389. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1083968 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chabris, C. F., Hebert, B. M., Benjamin, D. J., Beauchamp, J., Cesarini, D., van der Loos, M., Johannesson, M., Magnusson, P. K. E., Lichtenstein, P., Atwood, C. S., Freese, J., Hauser, T. S., Hauser, R. M., Christakis, N., & Laibson, D. (2012). Most reported genetic associations with general intelligence are probably false positives. Psychological Science, 23(11), 13141323. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611435528 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, S. W., Mak, T. S.-H., & O’Reilly, P. F. (2020). Tutorial: A guide to performing polygenic risk score analyses. Nature Protocols, 15, 27592772. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41596-020-0353-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colodro-Conde, L., Couvy-Duchesne, B., Zhu, G., Coventry, W. L., Byrne, E. M., Gordon, S., Wright, M. J., Montgomery, G. W., Madden, P. A. F., Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, Ripke, S., Eaves, L. J., Heath, A. C., Wray, N. R., Medland, S. E., & Martin, N. G. (2018). A direct test of the diathesis-stress model for depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 23, 15901596. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.130 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M., & Sagovsky, R. (1987). Detection of postnatal depression: Development of the 10-item Edinburgh postnatal depression scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 782786. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.150.6.782 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cree, R. A., Liu, C., Gueorguieva, R., Neiderhiser, J. M., Leve, L. D., Connell, C. M., Shaw, D. S., Natsuaki, M. N., Ganiban, J. M., Beekman, C., Smith, M. V., & Reiss, D. (2021). Using an adoption design to test genetically based differences in risk for child behavior problems in response to home environmental influences. Development and Psychopathology, 33, 12291247. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420000450 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danese, A., & Widom, C. S. (2020). Objective and subjective experiences of child maltreatment and their relationships with psychopathology. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 811818. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0880-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniels, D., & Plomin, R. (1985). Differential experience of siblings in the same family. Developmental Psychology, 21, 747760. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.21.5.747 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1998). Multiple risk factors in the development of externalizing behavior problems: Group and individual differences. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 469493. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579498001709 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demontis, D., Walters, R. K., Martin, J., Mattheisen, M., Als, T. D., Agerbo, E., Baldursson, G., Belliveau, R., Bybjerg-Grauholm, J., Bækvad-Hansen, M., Cerrato, F., Chambert, K., Churchhouse, C., Dumont, A., Eriksson, N., Gandal, M., Goldstein, J. I., Grasby, K. L., Grove, J., … Neale, B. M. (2019). Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Genetics, 51, 6375. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dick, D. M., Agrawal, A., Keller, M. C., Adkins, A., Aliev, F., Monroe, S., Hewitt, J. K., Kendler, K. S., & Sher, K. J. (2015). Candidate gene–environment interaction research: Reflections and recommendations. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 10, 3759. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614556682 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domingue, B., Trejo, S., Armstrong-Carter, E., & Tucker-Drob, E. (2020). Interactions between polygenic scores and environments: Methodological and conceptual challenges. Sociological Science, 7, 465486. https://doi.org/10.15195/v7.a19 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, L. E., & Keller, M. C. (2011). A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 10411049. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020191 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Funder, D. C., & Ozer, D. J. (2019). Evaluating effect size in psychological research: Sense and nonsense. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2, 156168. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245919847202 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garmezy, N., & Rutter, M. (1983). Stress, coping, and development in children. McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Gidziela, A., Malanchini, M., Rimfeld, K., McMillan, A., Ronald, A., Viding, E., Asbury, K., Eley, T. C., von Stumm, S., & Plomin, R. (2022). How much variance in behaviour problems can be predicted by measured nonshared environment from preschool to adulthood? Submitted for Publication.Google Scholar
Gidziela, A., Rimfeld, K., Malanchini, M., Allegrini, A. G., McMillan, A., Selzam, S., Ronald, A., Viding, E., Stumm, S., Eley, T. C., & Plomin, R. (2021). Using DNA to predict behaviour problems from preschool to adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 781792 . https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13519 Google Scholar
Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottesman, I. I. (1991). Schizophrenia genesis: The origins of madness. W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Götz, F. M., Gosling, S. D., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2021). Small effects: The indispensable foundation for a cumulative psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17, 205215. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620984483 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grotzinger, A. D., Rhemtulla, M., de Vlaming, R., Ritchie, S. J., Mallard, T. T., Hill, W. D., Ip, H. F., Marioni, R. E., McIntosh, A. M., Deary, I. J., Koellinger, P. D., Harden, K. P., Nivard, M. G., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2019). Genomic structural equation modelling provides insights into the multivariate genetic architecture of complex traits. Nature Human Behaviour, 3, 513525. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0566-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanscombe, K. B., Trzaskowski, M., Haworth, C. M. A., Davis, O. S. P., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2012). Socioeconomic status (SES) and children’s intelligence (iq): In a UK-representative sample SES moderates the environmental, not genetic, effect on IQ. PLoS ONE, 7, e30320. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030320 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, Q., & Li, J. J. (2021). A gene–environment interaction study of polygenic scores and maltreatment on childhood ADHD. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 50, 309319. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-021-00873-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewitt, J. K. (2012). Editorial policy on candidate gene association and candidate gene-by-environment interaction studies of complex traits. Behavior Genetics, 42, 12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-011-9504-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jinks, J. L., & Fulker, D. W. (1970). Comparison of the biometrical genetical, MAVA, and classical approaches to the analysis of the human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 73, 311349. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0029135 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kandaswamy, R., Allegrini, A., Nancarrow, A. F., Cave, S. N., Plomin, R., & von Stumm, S. (2021). Predicting alcohol use from genome-wide polygenic scores, environmental factors, and their interactions in young adulthood. Psychosomatic Medicine, 84, 244250. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000001005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keers, R., & Pluess, M. (2017). Childhood quality influences genetic sensitivity to environmental influences across adulthood: A life-course Gene x Environment interaction study. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 19211933. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417001493 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, M. C. (2014). Gene × Environment interaction studies have not properly controlled for potential confounders: The problem and the (simple) solution. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 1824. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., & Eaves, L. J. (1986). Models for the joint effect of genotype and environment on liabililty to psychiatric illness. American Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 279289.Google ScholarPubMed
Krapohl, E., Hannigan, L. J., Pingault, J.-B., Patel, H., Kadeva, N., Curtis, C., Breen, G., Newhouse, S. J., Eley, T. C., O’Reilly, P. F., & Plomin, R. (2017). Widespread covariation of early environmental exposures and trait-associated polygenic variation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 1172711732. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1707178114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krapohl, E., Patel, H., Newhouse, S., Curtis, C. J., von Stumm, S., Dale, P. S., Zabaneh, D., Breen, G., O’Reilly, P. F., & Plomin, R. (2018). Multi-polygenic score approach to trait prediction. Molecular Psychiatry, 23, 13681374. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.163 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ksinan, A. J., Smith, R. L., Barr, P. B., & Vazsonyi, A. T. (2021). The associations of polygenic scores for risky behaviors and parenting behaviors with adolescent externalizing problems. Behavior Genetics, 52, 2637. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-021-10079-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J. J., Wedow, R., Okbay, A., Kong, E., Maghzian, O., Zacher, M., Nguyen-Viet, T. A., Bowers, P., Sidorenko, J., Karlsson Linnér, R., Fontana, M. A., Kundu, T., Lee, C., Li, H., Li, R., Royer, R., Timshel, P. N., Walters, R. K., Willoughby, E. A., … Cesarini, D. (2018). Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nature Genetics, 50, 11121121. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0147-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leve, L. D., Harold, G. T., Ge, X., Neiderhiser, J. M., Shaw, D., Scaramella, L. V., & Reiss, D. (2009). Structured parenting of toddlers at high versus low genetic risk: Two pathways to child problems. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 11021109. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b8bfc0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leve, L. D., Neiderhiser, J. M., Ganiban, J. M., Natsuaki, M. N., Shaw, D. S., & Reiss, D. (2019). The early growth and development study: A dual-family adoption study from birth through adolescence. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 22, 716727. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2019.66 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipscomb, S. T., Laurent, H., Neiderhiser, J. M., Shaw, D. S., Natsuaki, M. N., Reiss, D., & Leve, L. D. (2014). Genetic vulnerability interacts with parenting and early care education to predict increasing externalizing behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 7080. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025413508708 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luciano, M., Hagenaars, S. P., Davies, G., Hill, W. D., Clarke, T.-K., Shirali, M., Harris, S. E., Marioni, R. E., Liewald, D. C., Fawns-Ritchie, C., Adams, M. J., Howard, D. M., Lewis, C. M., Gale, C. R., McIntosh, A. M., & Deary, I. J. (2018). Association analysis in over 329,000 individuals identifies 116 independent variants influencing neuroticism. Nature Genetics, 50, 611. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-017-0013-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malanchini, M., Rimfeld, K., Allegrini, A. G., Ritchie, S. J., & Plomin, R. (2020). Cognitive ability and education: How behavioural genetic research has advanced our knowledge and understanding of their association. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 111, 229245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matheny, A. P., Wachs, T. D., Ludwig, J. L., & Phillips, K. (1995). Bringing order out of chaos: Psychometric characteristics of the confusion, hubbub, and order scale. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 16, 429444. https://doi.org/10.1016/0193-3973(95)90028-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monroe, S. M., & Simons, A. D. (1991). Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: Implications for the depressive disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 406425. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.406 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mullins, N., Power, R. A., Fisher, H. L., Hanscombe, K. B., Euesden, J., Iniesta, R., Levinson, D. F., Weissman, M. M., Potash, J. B., Shi, J., Uher, R., Cohen-Woods, S., Rivera, M., Jones, L., Jones, I., Craddock, N., Owen, M. J., Korszun, A., Craig, I. W., … Lewis, C. M. (2016). Polygenic interactions with environmental adversity in the aetiology of major depressive disorder. Psychological Medicine, 46, 759770. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715002172 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelemans, S. A., Boks, M., Lin, B., Oldehinkel, T., van Lier, P., Branje, S., & Meeus, W. (2021). Polygenic risk for major depression interacts with parental criticism in predicting adolescent depressive symptom development. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 50, 159176. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-020-01353-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okbay, A., Beauchamp, J. P., Fontana, M. A., Lee, J. J., Pers, T. H., Rietveld, C. A., Turley, P., Chen, G.-B., Emilsson, V., Meddens, S. F. W., Oskarsson, S., Pickrell, J. K., Thom, K., Timshel, P., de Vlaming, R., Abdellaoui, A., Ahluwalia, T. S., Bacelis, J., Baumbach, C., … Benjamin, D. J. (2016). Genome-wide association study identifies 74 loci associated with educational attainment. Nature, 533, 539542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okbay, A., Wu, Y., Wang, N., Jayashankar, H., Bennett, M., Nehzati, S. M., Sidorenko, J., Kweon, H., Goldman, G., Gjorgjieva, T., Jiang, Y., Hicks, B., Tian, C., Hinds, D. A., Ahlskog, R., Magnusson, P. K. E., Oskarsson, S., Hayward, C., Campbell, A., … Young, A. I. (2022). Polygenic prediction of educational attainment within and between families from genome-wide association analyses in 3 million individuals. Nature Genetics, 54, 437449. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-022-01016-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pain, O., Glanville, K. P., Hagenaars, S. P., Selzam, S., Fürtjes, A. E., Gaspar, H. A., Coleman, J. R. I., Rimfeld, K., Breen, G., Plomin, R., Folkersen, L., & Lewis, C. M. (2020). Evaluation of polygenic prediction methodology within a reference-standardized framework. PLOS Genomics, 17, e100921. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.28.224782 Google Scholar
Paris, J. (1999). Genetics and psychopathology: Predisposition-stress interactions. American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Peyrot, W. J., Milaneschi, Y., Abdellaoui, A., Sullivan, P. F., Hottenga, J. J., Boomsma, D. I., & Penninx, B. W. J. H. (2014). Effect of polygenic risk scores on depression in childhood trauma. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 205, 113119. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.113.143081 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peyrot, W. J., Van der Auwera, S., Milaneschi, Y., Dolan, C. V., Madden, P. A. F., Sullivan, P. F., Strohmaier, J., Ripke, S., Rietschel, M., Nivard, M. G., Mullins, N., Montgomery, G. W., Henders, A. K., Heat, A. C., Fisher, H. L., Dunn, E. C., Byrne, E. M., Air, T. A., Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, … Penninx, B. W. J. H. (2018). Does childhood trauma moderate polygenic risk for depression? A meta-analysis of 5765 subjects from the psychiatric genomics consortium. Biological Psychiatry, 84, 138147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.09.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pingault, J.-B., O’Reilly, P. F., Schoeler, T., Ploubidis, G. B., Rijsdijk, F., & Dudbridge, F. (2018). Using genetic data to strengthen causal inference in observational research. Nature Reviews Genetics, 19, 566580. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41576-018-0020-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R. (1994). Genetics and experience: The interplay between nature and nurture. Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R. (2011). Commentary: Why are children in the same family so different? Non-shared environment three decades later. International Journal of Epidemiology, 40, 582592. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyq144 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R. (2018). Blueprint: How DNA makes us who we are. Allen Lane/Penguin Books. https://www.penguin.co.uk/books/298/298391/blueprint/9780141984261.html Google Scholar
Plomin, R., & Bergeman, C. S. (1991). The nature of nurture: Genetic influence on “environmental” measures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 373386. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00070278 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1985). Origins of individual differences in infancy: The Colorado adoption project. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1988). Nature and nurture during infancy and early childhood. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511527654 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Loehlin, J. C. (1977). Genotype–environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 309322. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.84.2.309 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., & von Stumm, S. (2018). The new genetics of intelligence. Nature Reviews Genetics, 19, 148159. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg.2017.104 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., & von Stumm, S. (2021). Polygenic scores: Prediction versus explanation. Molecular Psychiatry, 27, 4952. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-021-01348-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2010). Differential susceptibility to parenting and quality child care. Developmental Psychology, 46, 379390. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015203 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pries, L.-K., Klingenberg, B., Menne-Lothmann, C., Decoster, J., van Winkel, R., Collip, D., Delespaul, P., De Hert, M., Derom, C., Thiery, E., Jacobs, N., Wichers, M., Cinar, O., Lin, B. D., Luykx, J. J., Rutten, B. P. F., van Os, J., & Guloksuz, S. (2020). Polygenic liability for schizophrenia and childhood adversity influences daily-life emotion dysregulation and psychosis proneness. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 141, 465475. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.13158 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Rietveld, C. A., Medland, S. E., Derringer, J., Yang, J., Esko, T., Martin, N. W., Westra, H.-J., Shakhbazov, K., Abdellaoui, A., Agrawal, A., Albrecht, E., Alizadeh, B. Z., Amin, N., Barnard, J., Baumeister, S. E., Benke, K. S., Bielak, L. F., Boatman, J. A., Boyle, P. A., … Koellinger, P. D. (2013). GWAS of 126,559 individuals identifies genetic variants associated with educational attainment. Science (New York, N.Y.), 340, 14671471. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1235488 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rimfeld, K., Malanchini, M., Spargo, T., Spickernell, G., Selzam, S., McMillan, A., Dale, P. S., Eley, T. C., & Plomin, R. (2019). Twins early development study (TEDS): A genetically sensitive investigation into behavioral and cognitive development from infancy to emerging adulthood. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 22, 508513. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2019.56 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, N., & Bergen, S. E. (2021). Environmental risk factors for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder and their relationship to genetic risk: Current knowledge and future directions. Frontiers in Genetics, 12, 999. https://doi.org/10.3389/fgene.2021.686666 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (1981). Stress, coping and development: Some issues and some questions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 22, 323356. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1981.tb00560.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2006). Gene–environment interplay and psychopathology: Multiple varieties but real effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 226261. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01557.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selzam, S., McAdams, T. A., Coleman, J. R. I., Carnell, S., O’Reilly, P. F., Plomin, R., & Llewellyn, C. H. (2018). Evidence for gene–environment correlation in child feeding: Links between common genetic variation for BMI in children and parental feeding practices. PLOS Genetics, 14, e1007757. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007757 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shmueli, G. (2010). To explain or to predict? Statistical Science, 25, 289310. https://doi.org/10.1214/10-STS330 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slagt, M., Dubas, J. S., Dekovic, M., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2016). Differences in sensitivity to parenting depending on child temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 10681110. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000061 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. Brunner-Routledge.Google Scholar
Trotta, A., Iyegbe, C., Forti, M. D., Sham, P. C., Campbell, D. D., Cherny, S. S., Mondelli, V., Aitchison, K. J., Murray, R. M., Vassos, E., & Fisher, H. L. (2016). Interplay between schizophrenia polygenic risk score and childhood adversity in first-presentation psychotic disorder: A pilot study. PLOS ONE, 11, e0163319. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0163319 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Stumm, S., & d’Apice, K. (2022). From genome-wide to environment-wide: Capturing the environome. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17, 3040. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620979803 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wachs, T. D., & Gruen, G. (1982). Early experience and human development. Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widaman, K. F. (2021). Interrogating the validity of cumulative indices of environmental and genetic risk for negative developmental outcomes. Development and Psychopathology. Online ahead of print. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001097 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yarkoni, T., & Westfall, J. (2017). Choosing prediction over explanation in psychology: Lessons from machine learning. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 11001122. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617693393 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. A 2x2 illustration of the prediction of children’s behavior problems from environments (low vs. high risk), from a genetic estimate (low vs. high risk), and from their interaction (GxE). See text for explanation.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Variance explained by G (ADHD and neuroticism GPS) and E (environmental risk and discipline factors) predicting teacher-rated hyperactivity/inattention (a), conduct problems (b), emotional symptoms (c) and peer relationship problems (d) at ages 7, 9 and 12.

Figure 2

Figure 3. G+E and GxE prediction of teacher-rated hyperactivity/inattention (a), conduct problems (b), emotional symptoms (c) and peer relationship problems (d) at ages 7, 9 and 12.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Significant GxE interactions in 2x2 analyses of extreme (+/- 1 SD) ADHD GPS, neuroticism GPS and environmental factor scores. Solid line = +1 SD for GPS; dashed line = -1 SD for GPS.

Supplementary material: File

Plomin et al. supplementary material

Plomin et al. supplementary material

Download Plomin et al. supplementary material(File)
File 8.9 MB