Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T15:09:46.617Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stephen T. Casper and Delia Gavrus (eds), The History of the Brain and Mind Sciences: Technique, Technology, Therapy (New York: University of Rochester Press, 2017), pp. i + 310, £95.00, hardback, ISBN: 9781580465953.

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 June 2018

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Book Review
Copyright
© The Author 2018. Published by Cambridge University Press. 

Stephen T. Casper and Delia Gavrus have two ambitions for their volume, The History of the Brain and Mind Sciences: Technique, Technology, Therapy. The first is to offer a critical reflection on how the mind and brain sciences have been shaped over the past two centuries by various medical concepts, practices and objects. What is most curious about the volume, however, is its second aim, for Casper and Gavrus hope that The History of the Brain and Mind Sciences becomes a ‘sophisticated and versatile teaching tool for graduate and senior undergraduate seminars’ (p. 1). Yet such an ambition feels too modest – unusual, even – for a collection that contains such high-quality, original contributions.

The first three chapters of The History of the Brain and Mind Sciences offer rich, generous commentaries on the place of technique, technology or therapy in the neurosciences. The first chapter, by L. Stephen Jacyna, focuses on the Menagerie in the Jardin des Plantes in post-revolutionary France, which was used as a site for the study of animals. Jacyna makes recourse to Foucault and Latour to outline the workings of the Menagerie’s ‘truth machine’ – the actors and observational techniques that constituted animal behaviour as a focus of study (and which then worked to generate knowledge of animal intelligence and, later, human behaviour). The chapter following, by Thomas Schlich, is as thorough as Jacyna’s. It documents the movement of therapeutic practices from experimental physiology to neurosurgery, where Schlich – in a wide survey of North America and Europe – illuminates how physiological technique, with its associations with scientific ‘objectivity’, was utilised by neurosurgeons in the 1930s to establish medical authority. The third chapter, by Kenton Kroker, focuses on the epidemic of encephalitis lethargica at the turn of the twentieth century, locating it not only within neurology, but also within the paper-technologies used by the World Health Organization to quantify and track the growth of the epidemic. Kroker’s is a fascinating and detailed account, and one of the real stand-out chapters in an already impressive volume.

It is to Casper and Gavrus’ credit that such lengthy, lavishly detailed contributions feature in The History of the Brain and Mind Sciences. Yet the most novel aspect of the volume is the focus on the ‘marginal’ – that is, on episodes that feel trivial, irrelevant or divergent when viewed from the perspective of contemporary neuroscience, but through which, Casper and Gavrus suggest, historians can disrupt the teleologies underpinning established histories of neuroscience. Max Stadler’s contribution is a case in point. Taking as his focus the neurophysiological study of nerve transmission in the interwar period, he enumerates the myriad sites through which knowledge was generated (implicating such non-neuroscientific objects as algae, whipped cream and radio transmitters). A history written from the perspective of present-day neuroscience would, Stadler argues, overlook this heterogeneity. A similar point is made by Delia Gavrus in her chapter on Edward Dockrill, a technician who worked under neurosurgeon Wilder Penfield. With close attention to Dockrill’s unpublished novel – rejected by publishers for its thinly-veiled criticisms of Penfield – Gavrus seeks to give a voice to an individual deemed marginal, both because of the job Dockrill did (as a mere technician) but also because the histological research that he was engaged on yielded no therapeutic breakthrough.

Two further chapters continue this focus on the marginal in the history of the brain and mind sciences, albeit rather briefly. Frank W. Stahnisch’s contribution explores the effects of technologies and techniques as they were transferred by emigrants from Europe to North America in the 1930s. Stahnisch indicates the ways in which photographs, data sets or instruments could be utilised by emigrants to claim new expertise. The chapter is at its strongest, though, and its most poignant, when documenting the difficulties neuroscientists faced when emigrating – ranging from the loss of a few prized research-notes or books (with a knock-on effect on research), to a loss of all scientific and worldly possessions. A greater focus on contingency, and on people less celebrated by modern-day neuroscience, teases out these stories. Elsewhere in The History of the Brain and Mind Sciences, Stephen T. Casper focuses on the 1951 Festival of Britain, a morale-boosting initiative sponsored by the government in the aftermath of the Second World War, in which science and medicine were given pride of place. Yet as Casper shows, despite scientists’ intentions to produce a holistic view of nature, the structure and architecture of their exhibits unwittingly introduced a dualism between illusion and reality. Casper uses the episode to underline the ease with which such dualisms can enter scientific representations, firing a warning shot to contemporary neuroscientists who believe that such out-dated dichotomies could never enter scientific thinking again.

It is at this point that the focus on marginality appears to diminish, with the final three chapters of The History of the Brain and Mind Sciences returning to the study of technique, technology and therapy. Justin Garson examines how shifting research techniques meant that amphetamine psychosis, as opposed to LSD intoxication, became the model for schizophrenia by the 1970s. Garson accounts for this through a combination of medical and cultural shifts – the former a reframing of medical thinking to make psychosis and schizophrenia comparable; the latter, a perception that amphetamine use was narcissistic and more associated with madness. Garson’s chapter is meticulously researched, as with earlier contributions, and ends with a hypothesis on how the history of schizophrenia could be approached in future research. The chapter that follows Garson’s continues the focus on schizophrenia, with a contribution by Brian P. Casey on the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) and schizophrenia in the decades after the Second World War. Casey successfully complicates the idea that schizophrenia became understood in biological terms through mere scientific enlightenment. Rather, he identifies the NIMH’s rhetoric as important in reframing the disorder, arguing that a focus on it could prove illuminating for future studies.

The History of the Brain and Mind Sciences concludes with a brief coda by Katja Guenther which summarises the preceding chapters, offers some gnomic statements about the value of studying marginality (to avoid teleology, for example) and ends with an important albeit terse point about how history is a technique and one imbricated in the time-period in which it is written. Although beyond the scope of The History of the Brain and Mind Sciences, one suspects that historians of the neurosciences will return to Guenther’s point in future publications – agitating, perhaps, for histories that not merely undermine teleological histories of neuroscience, but which also strive for a deeper, more creative engagement with our contemporary obsession with the neuro.