Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-x4r87 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T03:23:28.088Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Biology and stigma

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

T. B. Benning
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Doncaster Royal Infirmary, Armthorpe Road, Doncaster DN2 5LT, UK. E-mail: tonybbenning@hotmail.com
M. O'Leary
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Doncaster Royal Infirmary, Armthorpe Road, Doncaster DN2 5LT, UK. E-mail: tonybbenning@hotmail.com
E. A. Avevor
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Doncaster Royal Infirmary, Armthorpe Road, Doncaster DN2 5LT, UK. E-mail: tonybbenning@hotmail.com
E. D. Avevor
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Doncaster Royal Infirmary, Armthorpe Road, Doncaster DN2 5LT, UK. E-mail: tonybbenning@hotmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © 2006 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

We wish to offer a brief response to the article by Drs Angermeyer and Matschinger (Reference Angermeyer and Matschinger2005) entitled ‘Casual beliefs and attitudes to people with schizophrenia’.

Underlying all forms of discrimination, including psychiatric stigmatisation, is an exaggerated attribution of ‘other-ness’ to certain individuals or groups, so that there is an assumption (made by the discriminator) of the existence of fundamental differences between himself and ‘the other’. Be it in the area of criminology, race, morality or mental health, this myth is further reinforced by the use of historically and culturally determined dichotomous constructs such as good/evil, citizen/alien, sane/insane and normal/pathological. The association of a biological marker with any stigmatised group acts as a signifier, further emphasising that group's distinctiveness. Previous attempts to elucidate biological markers in criminals and in certain ethnic groups have served only to etch this mistaken notion of fundamental difference a little deeper in the mind of the discriminator and, in doing so, to reinforce prejudice. We believe that the findings of Angermeyer & Matschinger may be partly explained through a similar effect following the promulgation of a biological theory of schizophrenia.

Cognitive-behavioural therapists address this myth of difference as a therapeutic strategy when working with people with psychosis by emphasising the continuity of symptoms across the range from those designated as ‘sick’ to ‘normal’ (Reference Kingdon and TurkingtonKingdon & Turkington, 1994). We believe that the extension of such an approach in the wider treatment of society could have a powerful role to play in the ongoing campaign against psychiatric stigmatisation. The origins of our current unsatisfactory conceptualisation of mentally ill people as being separated from ‘normal people’ by an absolute and fundamental boundary are often attributed to the Kraepelinian model. We welcome then the predictions made by Craddock & Owen (Reference Craddock and Owen2005) of the imminent demise of this in favour of a newer, hopefully less iatrogenic paradigm.

References

Angermeyer, M. C. & Matschinger, H. (2005) Causal beliefs and attitudes to people with schizophrenia. Trend analysis based on data from two population surveys in Germany. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 331334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craddock, N. & Owen, M. J. (2005) The beginning of the end for the Kraepelinian dichotomy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 364366.Google Scholar
Kingdon, D. G. & Turkington, D. (1994) Cognitive-Behavioural Therapy of Schizophrenia. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.