Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T04:52:47.357Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stereotype Development in Andalusian Children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

Almudena Giménez de la Peña*
Affiliation:
University of Malaga
Jesús M. Canto Ortiz
Affiliation:
University of Malaga
Pablo Fernández Berrocal
Affiliation:
University of Malaga
Martyn Barrett
Affiliation:
University of Surrey
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Almudena Giménez de la Peña, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Málaga, 29071 Málaga, Spain. E-mail: almudena@uma.es

Abstract

Social psychologists have shown a profound interest in intergroup relationships, but there are very few papers focusing on the developmental aspects that explain the psychological mechanisms involved in the construction of group and cultural identity. Our research aims to explore how the self-categorization of Andalusian children evolves. We tried to assess the degree to which they self-identify as Andalusian, Spanish, and European, and how this identification changes with age. We were also interested in the affective evaluation of different groups (French, Italian, English, German, Spanish, Catalonian, and Andalusian) made by Andalusian children. Furthermore, we analyzed the relationship between self-categorization and the evaluation of these groups. Results show that the development of national (autonomous community) identity in these children is influenced by their cognitive development, as well as by the relationships among the regional communities of Spain and the relationships between Spain and other countries. The peculiarity of Andalusians as a group is that they assume both identities: Spanish and Andalusian, from a very early age. In-group favoritism is an extended phenomenon at all ages, and Andalusian children have a negative stereotype of the other Spanish groups and other European communities.

Los psicólogos sociales manifiestan un profundo interés por las relaciones intergrupales. Sin embargo, hay pocas publicaciones relacionadas con los aspectos evolutivos que explican los mecanismos psicológicos implicados en la construcción de la identidad grupal y cultural. Nuestra investigación ha pretendido estudiar cómo evoluciona la autocategorización de los niños andaluces. También hemos evaluado el grado en que la identificación de los niños como andaluces, españoles y europeos cambia con la edad. Al mismo tiempo, nos interesaba conocer cómo los niños andaluces evalúan a diferentes grupos (franceses, italianos, ingleses, alemanes, españoles, catalanes y andaluces). Igualmente, hemos analizado la relación entre la autocategorización y la evaluación de esos grupos. Los resultados muestran que el desarrollo y adquisición de la identidad nacional está influida tanto por los aspectos cognoscitivos (indicados por la edad) como por las relaciones entre las comunidades de España y las relaciones entre España y otros países. La peculiaridad de los andaluces como grupo es que asumen la identidad como español y como andaluz desde una edad muy temprana. El favoritismo endogrupal es un fenómeno extendido a todas las edades, de ahí que los niños andaluces tengan un estereotipo más negativo de los exogrupos.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aboud, F.E. (1980). A test of ethnocentrism with young children. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 12, 195209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aboud, F.E. (1988). Children and prejudice. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Asher, S.R., & Allen, V.L. (1969). Racial preference and social comparison processes. Journal of Social Issues, 25, 157166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M. (2000, March). The development of nation identity in childhood and adolescence. Inaugural lecture presented at the University of Surrey, UK.Google Scholar
Barrett, M., & Short, J. (1992). Images of European people in a group of 5-10 year old schoolchildren. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10, 339369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M., Wilson, H., & Lyons, E. (1999, April). Self-categorization theory and the development of national identity in English children. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Albuquerque, NM.Google Scholar
Brand, E.S., Ruiz, R.A., & Padilla, A.M. (1974). Ethnic identification and preference: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 860890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breakwell, G.M., & Lyons, E. (1996). Changing European identities. Social psychological analyses of social change. London: Surrey University Press.Google Scholar
Brewer, M.M., & Miller, N. (1996). Intergroup relations. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks-Cole.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1995). Prejudice. Its social psychology. London: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Giménez, A., Canto, J.M., Fernández, P., & Barrett, M. (1999). La identificación social como regulador del estereotipo: lo que piensan los niños andaluces. Boletín de Psicología, 64, 8199.Google Scholar
Hogg, M.A., & Abrams, D. (1988). Social identifications: A social psychology of intergroup relations and group processes. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Huici, C., Ros, M., Cano, I., Hopkins, N., Emler, N., & Carmona, M. (1997). Comparative identity and evaluation of sociopolitical change: Perceptions of the European Community as a function of the salience of regional identities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 97113.3.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jahoda, G. (1962). Development of Scottish children's ideas and attitudes about other countries. Journal of Social Psychology, 58, 91108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Javaloy, F., Cornejo, J.M., & Bechini, A. (1990). España vista desde Cataluña: estereotipos étnicos en una comunidad plural. Barcelona, Spain: PPU.Google Scholar
Johnson, N. (1966). What do children learn from war comics? New Society, 8, 712.Google Scholar
Lambert, W.E., & Klineberg, O. (1967). Children's view of foreign: A cross-national study. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.Google Scholar
Maccoby, E.E. (1988). Gender as social category. Developmental Psychology, 24, 755765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. (1983). Representation. In Mussen, P. H. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 123155). New York: Wiley,Google Scholar
Middleton, M., Tajfel, H., & Johnson, N. (1970). Cognitive and affective aspects of children's national attitudes. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 122134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morland, J.K. (1969). Race awareness among American and Hong Kong Chinese children. American Journal of Sociology, 75, 360374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J., & Weil, A. (1951). The development in children of the idea of the homeland and of relations with other countries. International Social Science Bulletin, 3, 561578.Google Scholar
Rodríguez, R., & Moya, M.C. (1998). España vista desde Andalucía. Estereotipos e identidad. Psicología Política, 16, 2748.Google Scholar
Sangrador, J.L. (1996). Identidades, actitudes y estereotipos en la España de las autonomías. Madrid: CIS.Google Scholar
Sedikides, C., Shopler, J., & Insko, C. (1998). Intergroup cognition and intergroup behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Shantz, C.U. (1983). Social cognition. In Mussen, P. H. (Ed.). Handbook of child psychology. Vol. III: Cognitive Development. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Slaby, R.G., & Frey, K.S. (1975). Development of gender constancy and selective attention to same-sex models. Child Development, 46, 849856.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tajfel, H., & Jahoda, G. (1966). Development in children of concepts and attitudes about their own and other nations: A cross-national study. Proceedings of the 18th International Congress in Psychology, Moscow, Symposium, 36, 1733.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H., Jahoda, G., Nemeth, C., Rim, C.Y., &. Johnson, N. (1972). The devaluation by children of their own national and ethnic group: Two case studies. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 11, 235243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J.C. (1979). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In Worchel, S. & Austin, W. G. (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 232261). Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall.Google Scholar
Thompson, S.K. (1975). Gender labels and early sex-role development. Child Development, 46, 339347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, J.C. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization behavior. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Van Avermaet, E., & McClintock, L.G. (1988). Intergroup fairness and bias in children. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 407427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J.E., & Morland, J.K. (1976). Race, color, and the young child. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina Press.Google Scholar
Yee, M.D., & Brown, R.J. (1994). The development of gender differentiation in young children. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 133196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zalk, S.R., & Katz, P.A. (1978). Gender attitudes in children. Sex Roles, 4, 349357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar