Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Theory of Mind in Children with Traumatic Brain Injury

  • Maureen Dennis (a1) (a2), Nevena Simic (a1) (a3), H. Gerry Taylor (a4), Erin D. Bigler (a5) (a6), Kenneth Rubin (a7), Kathryn Vannatta (a8) (a9), Cynthia A. Gerhardt (a8) (a9), Terry Stancin (a4) (a10), Caroline Roncadin (a11) and Keith Owen Yeates (a8) (a9)...

Abstract

Theory of mind (ToM) involves thinking about mental states and intentions to understand what other people know and to predict how they will act. We studied ToM in children with traumatic brain injury (TBI) and age- and gender-matched children with orthopedic injuries (OI), using a new three-frame Jack and Jill cartoon task that measures intentional thinking separate from contingent task demands. In the key ToM trials, which required intentional thinking, Jack switched a black ball from one hat to another of a different color, but Jill did not witness the switch; in the otherwise identical non-ToM trials, the switch was witnessed. Overall accuracy was higher in children with OI than in those with TBI. Children with severe TBI showed a larger decline in accuracy on ToM trials, suggesting a specific deficit in ToM among children with severe TBI. Accuracy was significantly higher on trials following errors than on trials following correct responses, suggesting that all groups monitored performance and responded to errors with increased vigilance. TBI is associated with poorer intentional processing in school-age children and adolescents relative to peers with OI; furthermore, children with TBI are challenged specifically by intentional demands, especially when their injury is severe. (JINS, 2012, 19, 1–9)

Copyright

Corresponding author

Correspondence and reprint requests to: Maureen Dennis, Department of Psychology, The Hospital for Sick Children, 555 University Avenue, Toronto, ON. Canada, M5G 1X8. E-mail: maureen.dennis@sickkids.ca

References

Hide All
Astington, J.W. (2003). Sometimes necessary, never sufficient: False-belief understanding and social competence. In B. Repacholi & V. Slaughter (Eds.), Individual differences in theory of mind: Implications for typical and atypical development. New York: Psychology Press.
Astington, J.W., Jenkins, J.M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 13111320.
Bartsch, K., Wellman, H. (1989). Young children's attribution of action to beliefs and desires. Child Development, 60(4), 946964.
Bibby, H., McDonald, S. (2005). Theory of mind after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 43(1), 99114.
Brown, R.L. (2010). Epidemiology of injury and the impact of health disparities. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 22, 321325.
Carlson, S.M., Moses, L.J., Claxton, L.J. (2004). Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: An investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(4), 299319.
Chapman, S.B., Sparks, G., Levin, H.S., Dennis, M., Roncadin, C., Zhang, L., Song, J. (2004). Discourse macrolevel processing after severe pediatric traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25, 3760.
Chevrier, A.D., Noseworthy, M.D., Schachar, R. (2007). Dissociation of response inhibition and performance monitoring in the stop signal task using event-related fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 13471358.
Dennis, M., Agostino, A., Roncadin, C., Levin, H. (2009). Theory of mind depends on domain-general executive functions of working memory and cognitive inhibition in children with traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 835847.
Dennis, M., Barnes, M. (1993). Oral discourse skills in children and adolescents after early-onset hydrocephalus: Linguistic ambiguity, figurative language, speech acts, and script-based inferences. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 18, 639652.
Dennis, M., Barnes, M.A. (2000). Speech acts after mild or severe childhood head injury. Aphasiology, 14(4), 391405.
Dennis, M., Barnes, M.A. (2001). Comparison of literal, inferential, and intentional text comprehension in children with mild or severe closed head injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 16, 114.
Dennis, M., Barnes, M.A., Donnelly, R.E., Wilkinson, M., Humphreys, R.P. (1996). Appraising and managing knowledge: Metacognitive skills after childhood head injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12, 77103.
Dennis, M., Francis, D.J., Cirino, P.T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M.A., Fletcher, J.M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 331343.
Dennis, M., Guger, S., Roncadin, C., Barnes, M., Schachar, R. (2001a). Attentional-inhibitory control and social-behavioral regulation after childhood closed head injury. Do biological developmental and recovery variables predict outcome? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 683692.
Dennis, M., Guger, S., Roncadin, C., Barnes, M., Schachar, R. (2001b). Attentional control and social discourse after childhood closed head injury: Are frontal contusions, age at injury and time since injury predictive? Brain and Cognition, 48, 197200.
Dennis, M., Purvis, K., Barnes, M.A., Wilkinson, M., Winner, E. (2001). Understanding of literal truth, ironic criticism, and deceptive praise after childhood head injury. Brain and Language, 78, 116.
Dennis, M., Wilkinson, M., Humphreys, R.P. (1998). How children with head injury represent real and deceptive emotion in short narratives. Brain and Language, 61, 450483.
Filippova, E., Astington, J.W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. Child Development, 79(1), 126138.
Frith, U., Frith, C. (2003). The biological basis of social interaction. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 151155.
Gordon, A.C.L., Olson, D.R. (1998). The relation between acquisition of a theory of mind and the capacity to hold in mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68(1), 7083.
Greenbaum, R.L., Stevens, S.A., Nash, K., Koren, G., Rovet, J. (2009). Social cognitive and emotion processing abilities of children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: A comparison with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 33(10), 16561670.
Hanten, G., Dennis, M., Zhang, L., Barnes, M., Roberson, G., Archibald, J., Levin, H.S. (2004). Childhood head injury and metacognitive processes in language and memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25(1-2), 85106.
Hein, G., Singer, T. (2008). I feel how you feel but not always: the empathic brain and its modulation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18, 153158.
Hein, G., Silani, G., Preuschoff, K., Batson, C.D., Singer, T. (2010). Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members' suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron, 68, 149160.
Hill, E.L., Frith, U. (2003). Understanding autism: Insights from mind and brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358, 281289.
Howard, I., Joseph, J.G., Natale, J.E. (2005). Pediatric traumatic brain injury: Do racial/ethnic disparities exist in brain injury severity, mortality, or medical disposition? Ethnicity & Disease, 15(Suppl. 5), 5156.
Langlois, J.A., Rutland-Brown, W., Thomas, K.E. (2005). The incidence of traumatic brain injury among children in the United States: Differences by race. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 20, 229238.
Levin, H., Hanten, G., Max, J., Li, X., Swank, P., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Schachar, R. (2007). Symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder following traumatic brain injury in children. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 28(2), 108118.
Levin, H.S., Hanten, G., Roberson, G., Li, X., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Dennis, M., Swank, P. (2008). Prediction of cognitive sequelae based on abnormal computed tomography findings in children following mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurosurgery. Pediatrics, 1(6), 461470.
Levin, H.S., Zhang, L., Dennis, M., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Schachar, R., Max, J., Hunter, J.V. (2004). Psychosocial outcome of TBI in children with unilateral frontal lesions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 305316.
Martín-Rodríguez, J.F., León-Carrión, J. (2010). Theory of mind deficits in patients with acquired brain injury: A quantitative review. Neuropsychologia, 48(5), 11811191.
Max, J.E., Keatley, E., Wilde, E.A., Bigler, E.D., Levin, H.S., Schachar, R.J., Yang, T.T. (2011). Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents in the first six months after traumatic brain injury. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 23(1), 2939.
Max, J.E., Levin, H.S., Landis, J., Schachar, R., Saunders, A.E., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Dennis, M. (2005). Predictors of personality change due to traumatic brain injury in children and adolescents in the first six months after injury. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 435442.
Max, J.E., Levin, H.S., Schachar, R., Landis, J., Saunders, A.E., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Dennis, M. (2006). Predictors of personality change due to traumatic brain injury in children and adolescents 6 to 24 months after injury. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 18, 2132.
McKinlay, A., Kyonka, E.G.E., Grace, R.C., Horwood, L.J., Fergusson, D.M., MacFarlane, M.R. (2010). An investigation of the pre-injury risk factors associated with children who experience traumatic brain injury. Injury Prevention, 16, 3135.
Meltzoff, A.N. (1995). What infant memory tells us about infantile amnesia: Long-term recall and deferred imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59(3), 497515.
Meltzoff, A.N., Gopnik, A., Repacholi, B. (1999). Toddlers understanding of intentions, desires and emotions: Explorations of the dark ages. In P. Zelazo (Ed.), Developing theories of intention. New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Moses, L.J. (2001). Executive accounts of theory of mind development. Child Development, 72, 688690.
Muscara, F., Catroppa, C., Eren, S., Anderson, V. (2009). The impact of injury severity on long-term social outcome following paediatric traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 19(4), 541546.
Olsson, A., Nearing, K.L., Phelps, E.A. (2007). Learning fears by observing others: The neural systems of social fear transmission. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 311.
Ornstein, T.J., Levin, H.S., Chen, S., Hanten, G., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Dennis, M., Schachar, R. (2009). Performance monitoring in children following traumatic brain injury. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(4), 506513.
Parslow, R.C., Morris, K.P., Tasker, R.C., Forsyth, R.J., Hawley, C.A. (2005). Epidemiology of traumatic brain injury in children receiving intensive care in the UK. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 90, 11821187.
Premack, D., Woodruff, G. (1978). Chimpanzee problem-solving: A test for comprehension. Science, 202, 532535.
Robinson, E.J. (2003). Six-year-olds’ contradictory judgments about knowledge and beliefs. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 235237.
Roncadin, C., Guger, S., Archibald, J., Barnes, M., Dennis, M. (2004). Working memory after mild, moderate, or severe childhood head injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25, 2136.
Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: A lesion study. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 30543067.
Shammi, P., Stuss, D.T. (1999). Humor appreciation. A role for the right frontal lobe. Brain, 122, 657666.
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R.J., Firth, C.D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303, 11571162.
Sodian, B., Taylor, C., Harris, P.L., Perner, J. (1991). Early deception and the child's theory of mind: False trails and genuine markers. Child Development, 62, 468483.
Stuss, D.T., Gallup, G.G. Jr, Alexander, M.P. (2001). The frontal lobes are necessary for ‘theory of mind’. Brain, 124(Pt 2), 279286.
Teasdale, G., Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness: A practical scale. Lancet, 2(7872), 8184.
Uekermann, J., Kraemer, M., Abdel-Hamid, M., Schimmelmann, B.G., Hebebrand, J., Daum, I., Kis, B. (2010). Social cognition in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(5), 734743.
Wellman, H.M., Cross, D., Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655684.
Wimmer, H., Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103128.
Wobbrock, J.O., Findlater, L., Gergle, D., Higgins, J.J. (2011). User-defined gestures for surface computing. Proceedings of the ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems ’11: The Aligned Rank Transform for nonparametric factorial analyses using only ANOVA procedures. Vancouver, British Columbia: New York: ACM Press, pp. 143–146.
Wu, T.C., Wilde, E.A., Bigler, E.D., Li, X., Merkley, T.L., Yallampalli, R., Levin, H.S. (2010). Longitudinal changes in the corpus callosum following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuroscience, 32(5–6), 361373.
Yeates, K., Bigler, E., Dennis, M., Gerhardt, C., Rubin, K., Stancin, T., Vannatta, K. (2007). Social outcomes in childhood brain disorder: A heuristic integration of social neuroscience and developmental psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 133(3), 535556.
Yeates, K.O., Taylor, H.G. (1997). Predicting premorbid neuropsychological functioning following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 825837.
Yates, P.J., Williams, W.H., Harris, A., Round, A., Jenkins, R. (2006). An epidemiological study of head injuries in a UK population attending an emergency department. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 77, 699701.
Yuh, E.L., Cooper, S.R., Ferguson, A.R., Manley, G.T. (2012). Quantitative CT improves outcome prediction in acute traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 29, 735746.

Keywords

Theory of Mind in Children with Traumatic Brain Injury

  • Maureen Dennis (a1) (a2), Nevena Simic (a1) (a3), H. Gerry Taylor (a4), Erin D. Bigler (a5) (a6), Kenneth Rubin (a7), Kathryn Vannatta (a8) (a9), Cynthia A. Gerhardt (a8) (a9), Terry Stancin (a4) (a10), Caroline Roncadin (a11) and Keith Owen Yeates (a8) (a9)...

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed