Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T17:06:40.487Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stroop Effects in Persons with Traumatic Brain Injury: Selective Attention, Speed of Processing, or Color-Naming? A Meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 February 2011

Boaz M. Ben-David*
Affiliation:
Oral Dynamics Laboratory, Department of Speech-Language Pathology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, Toronto, Canada Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Toronto, Canada
Linh L.T. Nguyen
Affiliation:
Oral Dynamics Laboratory, Department of Speech-Language Pathology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Toronto, Canada
Pascal H.H.M. van Lieshout
Affiliation:
Oral Dynamics Laboratory, Department of Speech-Language Pathology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, Toronto, Canada Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Toronto, Canada Institute of Biomaterials and Biomedical Engineering, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Boaz M. Ben-David, Oral Dynamics Laboratory, Department of Speech-Language Pathology, 160-500 University Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, M5G 1V7, Canada. E-mail: boaz.ben.david@utoronto.ca

Abstract

The color word Stroop test is the most common tool used to assess selective attention in persons with traumatic brain injury (TBI). A larger Stroop effect for TBI patients, as compared to controls, is generally interpreted as reflecting a decrease in selective attention. Alternatively, it has been suggested that this increase in Stroop effects is influenced by group differences in generalized speed of processing (SOP). The current study describes an overview and meta-analysis of 10 studies, where persons with TBI (N = 324) were compared to matched controls (N = 501) on the Stroop task. The findings confirmed that Stroop interference was significantly larger for TBI groups (p = .008). However, these differences may be strongly biased by TBI-related slowdown in generalized SOP (r2 = .81 in a Brinley analysis). We also found that TBI-related changes in sensory processing may affect group differences. Mainly, a TBI-related increase in the latency difference between reading and naming the font color of a color-neutral word (r2 = .96) was linked to Stroop effects. Our results suggest that, in using Stroop, it seems prudent to control for both sensory factors and SOP to differentiate potential changes in selective attention from other changes following TBI. (JINS, 2011, 17, 354–363)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alferes, V. (2003). Meta-analysis: Fixed and random effects models. Retrieved from http://www.spsstools.netGoogle Scholar
Bashore, T.R., Ridderinkhof, K.R. (2002). Older age, traumatic brain injury, and cognitive slowing: Some convergent and divergent findings. Psychological Bulletin, 128(1), 151198.Google Scholar
Bagiella, E., Novack, T., Ansel, B., Diaz-Arrastia, R., Dikmen, S., Hart, T., Temkin, N. (2010). Measuring outcome in traumatic brain injury treatment trials: Recommendations from the traumatic brain injury clinical trials network. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 25(5), 375382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balota, D., Tse, C.S., Hutchison, K., Spieler, D., Duchek, J., Morris, J. (2010). Predicting conversion to dementia of the Alzheimer's type in a healthy control sample: The power of errors in Stroop color naming. Psychology and Aging, 25(1), 208218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batchelor, J., Harvery, A., Bryant, R.A. (1995). Stroop colour word test as a measure of attentional deficit following mild head injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9(2), 180186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bate, A.J., Mathias, J.L., Crawford, J.R. (2001). Performance on the Test of Everyday Attention and standard tests of attention following severe traumatic brain injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15(3), 405422.Google Scholar
Ben-David, B.M., Schneider, B.A. (2009). A sensory origin for color-word Stroop effects in aging: A meta-analysis. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 16(5), 505534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-David, B.M., Schneider, B.A. (2010). A sensory origin for color-word Stroop effects in aging: Simulating age-related changes in color-vision mimics age-related changes in Stroop. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 17(6), 730746.Google Scholar
Ben-David, B.M., Van Lieshout, P.H.H.M., Leszcz, T. (2011). A resource of validated affective and neutral sentences to assess identification of emotion in spoken language after a brain injury. Brain Injury, 25(2), 206220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohnen, N., Jolles, J., Twijnstra, A. (1992). Modification of the Stroop color word test improves differentiation between patients with mild head injury and matched controls. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6(2), 178184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brinley, J.F. (1965). Cognitive sets, speed and accuracy of performance in the elderly. In Welford, A.T., Birren, J.E. (Eds.), Behavior, aging and the nervous system (pp. 114149). Springfield IL: C. C. Thomas.Google Scholar
Bush, G., Luu, P., Posner, M.I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, K.B., de Lugt, D.R. (1995). Event-related potential measures of cognitive deficits following closed head injury. In Johnson, R., Baron, J.C. (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 269298). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Cantin, J.F., McFadyen, B.J., Doyon, J., Swaine, B., Dumas, D., Vallée, M. (2007). Can measures of cognitive function predict locomotor behaviour in complex environments following a traumatic brain injury? Brain Injury, 21(3), 327334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cerella, J. (1990). Aging and information processing rate. In Birren, J.E., Schaie, K.W. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (3rd ed., pp. 201221). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cerella, J., Hale, S. (1994). The rise and fall in information-processing rates over the life span. Acta Psychologica, 86(2–3), 109197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, B., Colella, B., Inness, E., Hebert, D., Monette, G., Bayley, M., Green, R.E. (2008). Recovery of cognitive function after traumatic brain injury: A multilevel modeling analysis of Canadian outcomes. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 89(12), S3S15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, C.R., O'Hanlon, A.P., Wright, M.J., Geffen, G.M. (1992). Event-related potential measurement of deficits in information processing following moderate to severe closed head injury. Brain Injury, 6(6), 509520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dikmen, S., Machamer, J., Fann, J.R., Temkin, N.R. (2010). Rates of symptom reporting following traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(3), 401411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eidels, A., Townsend, J.T., Algom, D. (2010). Comparing perception of Stroop stimuli in focused versus divided attention paradigms: Evidence for dramatic processing differences. Cognition, 114(2), 129150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ettenhofer, M.L., Abeles, N. (2009). The significance of mild traumatic brain injury to cognition and self-reported symptoms in long-term recovery from injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31(3), 363372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., McCandliss, B.D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., Posner, M.I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferraro, R.F. (1996). Cognitive slowing in closed-head injury. Brain and Cognition, 32(3), 429440.Google Scholar
Forrester, G., Encel, J., Geffen, G. (1994). Measuring post-traumatic amnesia PTA: An historical review. Brain Injury, 8(2), 175184.Google Scholar
Gennarelli, T.A., Thibault, L.E., Graham, D.I. (1998). Diffuse axonal injury: An important form of traumatic brain damage. Neuroscientist, 4(3), 202215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goethals, I., Audenaert, K., Jacobs, F., Lannoo, E., Van de Wiele, C., & Ham, H., Dierckx, R. (2004). Cognitive neuroactivation using SPECT and the Stroop Colored Word Test in patients with diffuse brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 21(8), 10591069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golden, C.J. (1978). The Stroop Color and Word Test. Chicago, IL: Stoelting Company.Google Scholar
Halterman, C.I., Langan, J., Drew, A., Rodriguez, E., Osternig, L.R., Chou, L.S., van Donkelaar, P. (2006). Tracking the recovery of visuospatial attention deficits in mild traumatic brain injury. Brain, 129(3), 747753.Google Scholar
Hedges, L.V., Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kapoor, N., Ciuffreda, K.J. (2002). Vision disturbances following traumatic brain injury. Current Treatment Options in Neurology, 4, 271280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinsella, G.J. (1998). Assessment of attention following traumatic brain injury: A review. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 8(8), 351375.Google Scholar
Kinsella, G.J., Prior, M., Sawyer, M., Ong, B., Murtagh, D., & Eisenmajer, R., Klug, G. (1997). Predictors and indicators of academic outcome in children 2 years following traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3(6), 608616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koh, S.B., Kim, B.J., Lee, J., Suh, S.I., Kim, T.K., Kim, S.H. (2008). Stereopsis and color vision impairment in patients with right extrastriate cerebral lesions. European Neurology, 60(4), 174178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lannoo, E., Vingerhoets, G. (1997). Flemish normative data on common neuropsychological tests: Influence of age, education, and gender. Psychologica Belgica, 37(3), 141155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lansbergen, M.M., Kenemans, J.L., Van Engeland, H. (2007). Stroop interference and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 21(2), 251262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larson, M.J., Kaufman, D.A.S., Kellison, I.L., Perlstein, W.M., Schmalfuss, I.M. (2009). Double jeopardy! The additive consequences of negative affect on performance- monitoring decrements following traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 23(4), 433444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, A.W., Cohen, J.D., Stenger, V.A., Carter, C.S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288(5472), 18351838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLeod, C.M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLeod, C.M. (1992). The Stroop task: The “gold standard” of attentional measures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 1214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLeod, C.M., MacDonald, P.A. (2000). Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: Uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4(10), 383391.Google Scholar
Madigan, N.K., DeLuca, J., Diamond, B.J., Tramontano, G., Averill, A. (2000). Speed of information processing in traumatic brain injury: Modality-specific factors. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 15(3), 943956.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mani, T.M., Miller, L.S., Yanasak, N., Macciocchi, S. (2007). Variability in Stroop task performance and functional activation among a small brain-injured group. Neurocase: Case Studies in Neuropsychology, Neuropsychiatry, and Behavioural Neurology, 13(4), 229236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDowd, J.M., Shaw, R.J. (2000). Attention and aging: A functional perspective. In Craik, F.I.M., Salthouse, T.A. (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 221292). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Mcdowell, S., Whyte, J., D'Esposito, M. (1997). Working memory impairments in traumatic brain injury: Evidence from a dual-task paradigm. Neuropsychologia, 35(10), 13411353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melara, R.D., Algom, D. (2003). Driven by information: A tectonic theory of Stroop effects. Psychological Review, 110(3), 422471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, E.M. (1994). Intelligence and brain myelination: A hypothesis. Personality and Individual Differences, 17(6), 803832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, L., Yoash-Gantz, R., Pickett, T., Campbell, T. (2009). Relationship between processing speed and executive functioning performance among OEFOIF veterans: Implications for postdeployment rehabilitation. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 24(1), 3240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oddy, M., Coughlan, T., Tyerman, A., Jenkins, D. (1985). Social adjustment after closed head injury: A further follow-up seven years after injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 48(6), 564568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, N.W., Moscovitch, M., Robertson, I.H. (1999). Divided attention impairments after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 37(10), 11191133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perlstein, W., Carter, C., Barch, D., Baird, J. (1998). The Stroop task and attention deficits in schizophrenia: A critical evaluation of card and single-trial Stroop methodologies. Neuropsychology, 12(3), 414425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perlstein, W.M., Larson, M.J., Dotson, V.M., Kelly, K.G. (2006). Temporal dissociation of components of cognitive control dysfunction in severe TBI: ERPs and the cued-Stroop task. Neuropsychologia, 44(2), 260274.Google Scholar
Petrides, M. (2005). Lateral prefrontal cortex: Architectonic and functional organization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 360(1456), 781795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ponsford, J., Kinsella, G. (1992). Attentional deficits following closed-head injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14(5), 822838.Google Scholar
Posner, M.I., Snyder, C.R.R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In Solso, R.L. (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 5585). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Randolph, A., Strothkamp, K.G. (1988). Instability of purified 9-kDa porcine intestinal calcium-binding protein in the absence of calcium. Archives of Biochemistry and Biophysics, 267(1), 4653.Google Scholar
Rapoport, M., McCullagh, S., Shammi, P., Feinstein, A. (2005). Cognitive impairment associated with major depression following mild and moderate traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 17(1), 6165.Google Scholar
Ríos, M., Periáñez, J.A., Muñoz-Céspedes, J.M. (2004). Attentional control and slowness of information processing after severe traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 18(3), 257272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabri, M., Melara, R.D., Algom, D. (2001). A confluence of contexts: Asymmetric versus global failures of selective attention to Stroop dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 27(3), 515537.Google ScholarPubMed
Sbordone, R., Liter, J., Pettler-Jennings, P. (1995). Recovery of function following severe traumatic brain injury: A retrospective 10-year follow-up. Brain Injury, 9(3), 285299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (1996). The effects of divided attention on implicit and explicit memory performance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2(2), 111125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seignourel, P.J., Robins, D.L., Larson, M.J., Demery, J.A., Cole, M., Perlstein, W.M. (2005). Cognitive control in closed head injury: Context maintenance dysfunction or prepotent response inhibition deficit? Neuropsychology, 19(5), 578590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soeda, A., Nakashima, T., Okumura, A., Kuwata, K., Shinoda, J., Iwama, T. (2005). Cognitive impairment after traumatic brain injury: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study using the Stroop task. Neuroradiology, 47(7), 501506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spikman, J.M., Van der Naalt, J., Van Weerden, T.W., Van Zomeren, A.H. (2004). Indices of slowness of information processing in head injury patients: Tests for selective attention related to ERP latencies. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(6), 851861.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spikman, J.M., Van Zomeren, A.H., Deelman, B.G. (1996). Deficits of attention after closed-head injury: Slowness only? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18(5), 755767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643662.Google Scholar
Stuss, D.T., Gow, C.A. (1992). ‘Frontal dysfunction’ after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurology, 5(4), 272282.Google Scholar
Timmerman, M.E., Brouwer, W.H. (1999). Slow information processing after very severe closed head injury: Impaired access to declarative knowledge and intact application and acquisition of procedural knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 37(4), 467478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vakil, E., Weisz, H., Jedwab, L., Groswasser, Z., Aberbuch, S. (1995). Stroop color-word task as a measure of selective attention: Efficiency in closed-head-injured patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17(3), 335342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veltman, J.C., Brouwer, W.H., Van Zomeren, A.H., Van Wolffelaar, P.C. (1996). Central executive aspects of attention in subacute severe and very severe closed head injury patients: Planning inhibition, flexibility, and divided attention. Neuropsychology, 10(3), 357367.Google Scholar
Verhaeghen, P., Cerella, J. (2002). Aging, executive control, and attention: A review of meta-analyses. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 849857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, R. (2003). Neural correlates of cognitive control and conflict detection in the Stroop and digit-location tasks. Neuropsychologia, 41(8), 11221135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahn, T.P., Mirsky, A.F. (1999). Reaction time indicators of attention deficits in closed head injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21(3), 352367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed