Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T21:45:10.855Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Prefrontal lesions and attentional skills in childhood

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 December 2005

V. ANDERSON
Affiliation:
Australian Centre for Child Neuropsychology Studies, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia Royal Children's Hospital, Melbourne, Australia University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia
R. JACOBS
Affiliation:
Australian Centre for Child Neuropsychology Studies, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia
A.S. HARVEY
Affiliation:
Royal Children's Hospital, Melbourne, Australia University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia

Abstract

Despite the potential impact on development, few studies have examined the influence of prefrontal lesions occurring prior to maturation of the central nervous system. This study investigates the effect of prefrontal lesions in general, as well as the impact of lesion laterality, with respect to attentional abilities. The sample comprised 36 children with prefrontal lesions and 40 healthy controls. Attentional function was assessed across four domains: selective, shifting and divided attention, and processing speed. Group mean performances for children with prefrontal lesions indicated global attentional deficits, with greatest difficulties for “higher-order” skills including shifting and divided attention. Children with left prefrontal lesions performed similarly to controls, with a specific deficit characterized by difficulties with on-line processing of auditory-verbal information. Right prefrontal lesions were primarily associated with impairments in day-to-day executive functions, including reduced monitoring, poor shifting attention and disinhibition. Children with bilateral prefrontal lesions performed worse than controls on tasks requiring greater cognitive resources. These results provide evidence of the important role played by prefrontal cortex in the development of attentional skills, and the particular role of the right prefrontal cortex. The pattern of attention deficits observed following early prefrontal lesions suggests some lateralization of function within the frontal lobes, even during childhood. (JINS, 2005, 11, 817–831.)Source of funding: Australian National Health and Medical Research Council, Australian Research Council, Murdoch Children's Research Institute.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2005 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Ackerly, S. & Benton, A. (1948). Report of a case of bilateral frontal lobe defect. Association for Research on Nervous and Mental Disorders, 27, 479504.Google Scholar
Anderson, P., Anderson, V., Northam, E., & Taylor, H.G. (2000a). Standardization of the contingency naming test (CNT) for school-aged children: A measure of reactive flexibility. Clinical Neuropsychological Assessment, 1, 247273.Google Scholar
Anderson, S., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. (2000b). Long-term sequelae of pre-frontal cortex damage acquired in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18, 281296.Google Scholar
Anderson, V. (1988). Recovery of function in children: The myth of cerebral plasticity. In M. Matheson & H. Newman (Eds.), Brain Impairment, Proceedings from the Thirteenth Annual Brain Impairment Conference (pp. 223247). Sydney: ASSBI.
Anderson, V. (1998). Assessing executive functions in children: Biological, psychological, and developmental considerations. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 8, 319349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, V., Anderson, P., Northam, E., Jacobs, R., & Catroppa, C. (2001a). Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 385406.Google Scholar
Anderson, V., Levin, H., & Jacobs, R. (2002). Developmental and acquired lesions of the frontal lobes in children: Neuropsychological implications. In D. Stuss & R. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 504527). New York: Oxford University Press.
Anderson, V. & Moore, C. (1995). Age at injury as a predictor of outcome following pediatric head injury. Child Neuropsychology, 1, 187202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, V., Morse, S., Klug, G., Catroppa, C., Haritou, F., Rosenfeld, J., & Pentland, L. (1997). Predicting recovery from head injury in school-aged children: A prospective analysis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 568580.Google Scholar
Anderson, V., Northam, E., Hendy, J., & Wrennall, J. (2001b). Developmental Neuropsychology: A Clinical approach. London: Psychology Press.
Anderson, V. & Pentland, L. (1998). Attention deficit following paediatric head injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 8, 283300.Google Scholar
Aram, D. & Enkelman, B. (1986). Cognitive profiles of children with early onset unilateral lesions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2, 155172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Ballantyne, A., Scarvie, K., & Trauner, D. (1992). IQ patterns in children with early-onset focal brain lesions. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 115.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Vicari, S., & Trauner, D. (1999). Neural mediation of language development: Perspectives from lesions studies of infants and children. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 533581). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Casey, B., Trainor, R., Orendi, J., Schubert, A., Nystrom, L., Giedd, J., Castellanos, X., Haxby, J., Noll, D., Cohen, J., Forman, S., Dahl, R., & Rapoport, J. (1997). A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation during performance of a Go-No-Go task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 835847.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chelune, G. & Baer, R. (1986). Developmental norms for the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8, 219228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooley, E. & Morris, R. (1990). Attention in children: A neuropsychologically-based model for assessment. Developmental Neuropsychology, 6, 239274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dall'Oglio, A., Bates, E., Volterra, V., Di Capua, M., & Pezzini, G. (1994). Early cognition, communication and language in children with focal brain injury. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 36, 10761098.Google Scholar
Daniel, A. (1983). Power, privilege and prestige: Occupations in Australia. Melbourne: Longman-Cheshire.
Dennis, M. (1989). Language and the young damaged brain. In T. Boll & B. Bryant (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology and brain function: Research, measurement and practice (pp. 89123). Washington: American Psychological Association.
Dennis, M., Wilkinson, M., Koski, L., & Humphreys, R. (1995). Attention deficits in the long term after childhood head injury. In S. Broman & M. Michel (Eds.), Traumatic head injury in children (pp. 165187). New York: Oxford University Press.
Dobler, V., Manly, T., Verity, C., Woolrych, J., & Roberson, R. (2003). Modulation of spatial attention in a child with developmental spatial neglect. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45, 282288.Google Scholar
Douglas, V. (1983). Attentional and cognitive problems. In M. Rutter (Ed.). Developmental neuropsychiatry (pp. 280329). New York: Guilford Press.
Dove, A., Pollman, S., Schubert, T., Wiggins, C., & van Cramon, D. (2000). Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: An event-related fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 9, 103109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eslinger, P. & Biddle, K. (2000). Adolescent neuropsychological development after early right prefrontal cortex damage. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18, 297330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eslinger, P., Biddle, K., & Grattan, L. (1997). Cognitive and social development in children with prefrontal cortex lesions. In N. Krasnegor, G. Lyon, & P. Goldman-Rakic (Eds.), Development of the prefrontal cortex: Evolution, neurology, and behavior (pp. 295336). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Eslinger, P., Biddle, K., Pennington, B., & Page, R. (1999). Cognitive and behavioral development up to 4 years after early right frontal lobe lesion. Developmental Neuropsychology, 15, 157191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eslinger, P., Grattan, L., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (1992). Developmental consequences of childhood frontal lobe damage. Archives of Neurology, 49, 764769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ewing-Cobbs, L., Brookshire, B., Scott, M., & Fletcher, J. (1998). Children's narratives following traumatic brain injury: Linguistic structure, cohesion, and thematic recall. Brain and Language, 61, 395419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garth, J., Anderson, V., & Wrennall, J. (1997). Executive functions following moderate-to-severe frontal lobe injuries: Impact of injury and age at injury. Pediatric Rehabilitation, 1, 99108.Google Scholar
Gioia, G., Isquith, P., Guy, S., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Odessa, FL: PAR Inc.
Godefroy, O., Lhullier, C., & Rousseaux, M. (1996). Non-spatial disorders in patients with frontal or posterior brain damage. Brain, 119, 191202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godefroy, O. & Rousseaux, M. (1996). Divided and focussed attention in patients with lesion of the prefrontal cortex. Brain and Cognition, 30, 155174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, E. & Costa, L. (1981). Hemispheric differences in the acquisition and use of descriptive systems. Brain and Language, 14, 144173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, C., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood and early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 101, 81748179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grattan, L., Bloomer, R., Archambault, F., & Eslinger, P. (1994). Cognitive flexibility and empathy after frontal lobe lesion. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurology, 7, 251259.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. (1947). The effects of early experience on problem solving at maturity. American Psychologist, 2, 737745.Google Scholar
Heilman, K., Voeller, K., & Nadeau, S. (1991). A possible pathophysiologic substrate of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Neurology, 6 (Suppl.), 7681.Google Scholar
Kail, R. (1986). Sources of age differences in speed of processing. Child Development, 57, 969987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R. (1988). Developmental processes for speeds of cognitive processes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 45, 339364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennard, M.A. (1936). Age and other factors in motor recovery from precentral lesions in monkeys. American Journal of Physiology, 115, 138146.Google Scholar
Kennard, M.A. (1940). Reorganization of motor function in cerebral cortex of monkeys deprived of motor and pre-motor areas in infancy. Journal of Neurophysiology, 1, 477496.Google Scholar
Kennard, M.A. (1942). Cortical reorganization of motor function. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 48, 227240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, T. (2000). The development of executive function in school-aged children. Clinical Neuropsychological Assessment, 1, 3855.Google Scholar
Kolb, B., Cioe, J., & Muirhead, D. (1998). Cerebral morphology and functional sparing after prenatal frontal cortex lesions in rats. Behavioral Brain Research, 91, 143155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolb, B. & Gibb, R. (1993). Possible anatomical basis of recovery of function after neonatal frontal lesions in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 799811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolb, B. & Gibb, R. (2002). Frontal plasticity and behavior. In D. Stuss & R. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 541556). New York: Oxford University Press.
Kolb, B., Gibb, R., & Gorny, G. (2000). Cortical plasticity and the development of behavior after early frontal cortical injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18, 423444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolb, B., Gibb, R., & van der Kooy, D. (1994). Neonatal frontal cortical lesions in rats alter cortical structure and connectivity. Behavioral Brain Research, 645, 8597.Google Scholar
Kolb, B., Petrie, B., & Cioe, J. (1996). Recovery from early cortical damage in rats, VII. Comparison of the behavioural and anatomical effects of medial prefrontal lesions at different ages of neural maturation. Behavioral Brain Research, 79, 113.Google Scholar
Kolb, B., Stewart, J., & Sutherland, R.J. (1997). Recovery of function is associated with increased spine density in cortical pyramidal cells after frontal lesions and/or noradrenaline depletion in neonatal rats. Behavioral Brain Research, 89, 6170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, H., Song, J., Scheibel, R., Fletcher, J., Harward, H., Lily, M., & Goldstein, F. (1997). Concept formation and problem solving following closed head injury in children. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 598607.Google Scholar
Lezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.
Luria, A.R. (1973). The working brain. New York: Basic Books.
Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., & Robertson, I. (2001). The differential assessment of children's attention: The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 10651081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manly, T., Robertson, I., Anderson, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1999). The test of everyday attention for children. Cambridge, UK: Thames Valley Test Company.
Marlowe, W. (1992). The impact of a right pre-frontal lesion on the developing brain. Brain and Cognition, 20, 205213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mateer, C.A. (1990). Cognitive and behavioral sequalae of face and forehead injury in childhood. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 95.Google Scholar
McKay, K., Halperin, J., Schwartz, S., & Sharma, V. (1994). Developmental analysis of three aspects of information processing: Sustained attention, selective attention, and response organization. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10, 121132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mecklinger, A., von Cramon, D., Springer, A., & Matthes-von Cramon, G. (1999). Executive control functions in task switching: Evidence from brain injured patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 606619.Google Scholar
Mesulam, M. (1981). A cortical network for directed attention on unilateral neglect. Annals of Neurology, 10, 309325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirsky, A. (1996). Disorders of attention: A neuropsychological perspective. In G. Lyon & N. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory and executive function (pp. 7196). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Pub.
Mirsky, A., Anthony, B., Duncan, C., Ahearn, M., & Kellam, S. (1991). Analysis of the elements of attention: A neuropsychological approach. Neuropsychological Review, 2, 109145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, A.M., Roberts, A.C., Polkey, C.E., Sahakian, B.J., & Robbins, T.W. (1991). Extra-dimensional versus intra-dimensional set shifting performance following frontal lobe excisions, temporal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man. Neuropsychologia, 29, 9931006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pentland, L., Todd, J., & Anderson, V. (1998). The impact of head injury on planning ability in adolescence: A functional analysis. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 8, 301317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. (1988). Structures and functions of selective attention. In T. Boll & B.K. Bryant (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology and brain function: Research, measurement and practice (pp. 173202). Washington: American Psychological Association.
Posner, M. (1995). Attention in cognitive neuroscience: An overview. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 531556). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Posner, M. & Petersen, S.E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neurosciences, 15, 2542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rebok, G., Smith, C., Pascualvaca, D., Mirsky, A., Anthony, B., & Kellam, S. (1997). Developmental changes in attentional performance in urban children from eight to thirteen years. Child Neuropsychology, 3, 4760.Google Scholar
Reitan, R.M. & Davison, L.A. (Eds.) (1974). Clinical neuropsychology: Current status and applications. Washington: Winston.
Riva, D. & Cassaniga, L. (1986). Late effect of unilateral brain lesions before and after the first year of life. Neuropsychologia, 24, 423428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robertson, I. (1999). The rehabilitation of attention. In D.T. Stuss, G. Winocur, & I. Robertson (Eds.), Cognitive Neuropsychological Rehabilitation (pp. 302313). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Rourke, B.P. (1989). Nonverbal learning disabilities. New York: Guilford Press.
Rubia, K., Overmeyer, S., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Williams, S., Simmons, A., & Bullmore, E. (1999). Hypofrontality in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: A study with functional MRI. American Journal of Pediatrics, 156, 891896.Google Scholar
Ruff, H. & Rothbart, M. (1996). Attention in early development: Themes and variations. New York: Oxford University Press.
Semrud-Clikeman, M., Steingard, R., Filipek, P., Biederman, J., Bekken, K., & Renshaw, P. (2000). Using MRI to examine brain-behavior relationships in males with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 477484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimamura, A. (1995). Memory and frontal lobe function. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 803813). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Smith, E. & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 282, 16571661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sowell, E., Thompson, P., Welcome, S., Henkenius, A., Toga, A., & Petersen, B. (2003). Cortical abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Lancet, 362, 16991707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stuss, D., Shallice, T., Alexander, M., & Picton, T. (1995). A multi-disciplinary approach to anterior attentional functions. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 769, 191211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stuss, D.T., Bisschop, S., Alexander, M., Levine, B., Katz, D., & Izukawa, D. (2001a). The Trail Making Test: A study of focal lesion patients. Psychological Assessment, 13, 230239.Google Scholar
Stuss, D.T., Floden, D., Alexander, M., Levine, B., & Katz, D. (2001b). Stroop performance in focal lesion patients: Dissociation of processes and frontal lobe lesion location. Neuropsychologia, 39, 774786.Google Scholar
Stuss, D.T., Levine, B., Alexander, M., Hong, J., Palumbo, C., Hamer, L., Murphy, K., & Izukawa, D. (2000). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with focal frontal and posterior brain damage: Effects of lesion location and test structure on separable cognitive processes. Neuropsychologia, 38, 388402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stuss, D.T., Toth, J., Franchi, D., Alexander, M., Tipper, S., & Craik, F. (1999). Dissociation of attentional processes in patients with focal frontal and posterior lesions. Neuropsychologia, 37, 10051027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamm, L., Menon, V., & Reiss, A.L. (2002). Maturation of brain function associated with response inhibition. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 12311238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, H.G. & Alden, J. (1997). Age-related differences in outcomes following childhood brain insults: An introduction and overview. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 555567.Google Scholar
Todd, J., Anderson, V., & Lawrence, J. (1996). Planning skills in head injured adolescents and their peers. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 6, 8199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vargha-Khadem, F., Isaacs, E., & Muter, V. (1994). A review of cognitive outcome after unilateral lesions sustained during childhood. Journal of Child Neurology, 9(Suppl. 2), 2S672S73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vargha-Khadem, F., O'Gorman, A., & Watters, G. (1985). Aphasia and handedness in relation to hemispheric side, age at injury and severity of cerebral lesion during childhood. Brain, 108, 677696.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vargha-Khadem, F. & Polkey, C. (1992). A review of cognitive outcome after hemidecortication in humans. In F. Rose & D. Johnson (Eds.), Recovery from brain damage: Advances in experimental medicine and biology; Vol. 325. Reflections and directions (pp. 137151). New York: Plenum Press.
Walsh, K.W. (1978). Neuropsychology: A clinical approach. New York: Churchill Livingston.
Walsh, K.W. (1985). Understanding brain damage: A primer of neuropsychological evaluation. New York: Churchill Livingston.
Wechsler, D. (1991). Manual for the Wechsler Scale of Children's Intelligence-III. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Williams, D. & Mateer, C. (1992). Developmental impact of frontal lobe injury in middle childhood. Brain and Cognition, 20, 96204.Google Scholar
Woods, D.L. & Knight, R.T. (1986). Electrophysiological evidence of increased distractibility after dorsolateral prefrontal lesions. Neurology, 36, 212216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeates, K.O., Taylor, H.G., Drotar, D., Wade, S., Stancin, T., & Klein, S. (1997). Pre-injury environment as a determinant of recovery from traumatic brain injuries in school-aged children. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 617630.Google Scholar